This systematic review and meta-analysis examined the neurofeedback training (NFT) effects on attentional performance in healthy adults. Six databases were searched until June 2022 to identify parallel randomized controlled trials (RCTs) evaluating attentional improvements after NFT. Risk of bias was assessed using the Cochrane Collaboration tool. We identified 41 RCTs for qualitative synthesis and 15 RCTs (569 participants) for meta-analysis. The overall NFT effect on attentional performance was significant (standardized mean difference = 0.27, 95% confidence interval = 0.10–0.44). However, no significant pooled effect was found within the trials comparing its effect with sham-NFT (eight RCTs). Additionally, variable effects were observed on individual subsets of attentional performance. Further sham-controlled RCTs are required to validate the improvement of attentional performance with NFT.

Attention is essential for cognitive functions and complex behaviors. It helps to select contextually critical information, ignore irrelevant information, and maintain or control task-relevant goals, thoughts, and emotions. Given its importance in cognitive processes, improving attentional performance would extensively benefit in daily life processes, including enhancing work performance and safety.

Several researchers have attempted to clearly define the term “attention” (Egeth & Kahneman, 1975; Pashler, 1998; Wickens, 2002). In fact, there is no unequivocal definition of attention as it is a multifaceted term that represents several behavioral benefits, including improvement in object detection or identification, response facilitation, and successful inhibition. Nevertheless, previous studies have identified multiple subtypes of attention. For instance, Posner and colleagues defined three distinct subtypes of attention: executive function, spatial orientation, and arousal (Posner & Petersen, 1990). Executive function establishes and maintains task-relevant goals while preventing interferences and conflicts. Spatial orientation localizes a target and prioritizes processing toward that target, irrespective of foveal fixation if the target is within the visual field. Arousal sustains the optimal level of alertness to effectively perceive high-priority signals and prepare an appropriate response. Posner and colleagues also argued that these three subtypes are mediated by largely independent brain networks (Fan et al., 2002; Posner & Petersen, 1990).

It is noteworthy that multiple studies have uncovered new aspects of attention since Posner and colleagues’ proposal. Several researchers argue that spatial orientation consists of two or three components (Corbetta et al., 2008; Luck et al., 2021; Summerfield & Egner, 2009). Others maintain that there is a substantial overlap between attention and executive functions (Diamond, 2013; Miyake et al., 2000; Oberauer, 2019) or that attention interacts with consciousness and subjective experience (Carrasco, 2018; Chun et al., 2011). Nonetheless, the three major subsets identified by Posner and colleagues (Posner & Petersen, 1990) remain unaffected by these studies and serve as a general guide to interpret the effect of attention on human performance (Petersen & Posner, 2012).

Neurofeedback training (NFT) is used to intervene in the activity of a specific brain region or network and hence is a viable way to improve attentional performance. NFT trains the participants or patients to self-regulate a specific brain activity. This can be achieved by monitoring each individual’s brain activity in real time with a device (i.e., electroencephalography [EEG], functional magnetic resonance imaging [fMRI], or near-infrared spectroscopy [NIRS]), and then by providing feedback with visual or (and) auditory stimuli. Since the first report showing the potential of EEG-based NFT to modulate the brain activity in humans (Kamiya, 1968), multiple NFT protocols have been developed to enhance attentional performance in healthy adults (See Gruzelier (2014) for a review).

NFT targeting to increase sensorimotor rhythm (SMR) power is a major protocol used to increase attentional performance. SMR is a low-end beta band (typically 12–15 Hz) derived from the sensorimotor cortex. This rhythm was shown to occur immediately prior to the performance of certain goal-directed behaviors (Wyrwicka & Sterman, 1968). After the SMR protocol of NFT was shown to reduce inattention and hyperactivity in patients with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (Lubar & Shouse, 1976), this has been used to increase arousal (Egner & Gruzelier, 2001, 2004a). Moreover, this method was shown to increase the performance of goal-directed complex behaviors (Cheng et al., 2015; Egner & Gruzelier, 2003). NFT to increase low beta (beta1; 15–18 Hz) was also reported to increase attentional performance but different attentional metrics than NFT to increase SMR power (Egner & Gruzelier, 2004a). This suggests that different NFT protocols might enhance different components of attentional performance.

NFTs intended to increase alpha power can also improve attentional performance. Although alpha band (typically 8–12 Hz) was originally reported to be enhanced in the relaxed and eyes-closed state rather than in the alert state (Berger, 1929), this band has been also shown to be important for maintaining task performance (Hord et al., 1976; Nowlis & Kamiya, 1970). Since then, this band has been thought to be crucial for inhibiting irrelevant stimuli (top-down attention), thereby enhancing executive function or spatial orientation (Hanslmayr et al., 2005). In addition to the bands described above, several reports have also elucidated the relationship between attentional performance and frontal theta power (Cavanagh et al., 2012; Wang & Hsieh, 2013). All these protocols can be potentially used to alter attentional performance.

Recently, two meta-analyses have shown that NFT, including protocols described above, can mitigate inattention and hyperactivity in patients with ADHD (Lambez et al., 2020; Van Doren et al., 2019). However, it remains unclear whether NFT can further enhance attentional performance in healthy adults rather than alleviate the attentional deficits in clinical populations. The results of studies in patients with ADHD cannot be directly applicable to the healthy population given that psychostimulants used to treat ADHD, such as methylphenidate, may affect neural plasticity (Korchounov & Ziemann, 2011; Ridding & Ziemann, 2010). Therefore, it is essential to investigate whether NFT is an effective approach for improving attentional performance among healthy adults. This knowledge is crucial for determining the applicability of NFT in improving their performances at work (Mazur et al., 2017; Ros et al., 2009) and in sports (de Brito et al., 2022).

Here, we conducted a systematic review of the effects of NFT on attentional performance among healthy adults. A meta-analysis was also performed to quantitatively evaluate the NFT effect in enhancing attentional performance and to clarify the NFT effect on each subtype of attentional performance based on the definition proposed by Posner and colleagues, the effect with each type of NFT protocol (e.g., enhancing SMR or alpha power), and the effect over each type of control condition. The present study differs from two previous systematic reviews on the effects of NFT on attentional performance (Da Silva & De Souza, 2021) and executive function (Da Silva & De Souza, 2021; Viviani & Vallesi, 2021) in healthy adults for the following reasons. First, neither conducted a meta-analysis. Second, the effects of NFT on the individual subsets of attentional performance (executive function, spatial orientation, and arousal) and which protocol of NFT has been used to intervene on each subset were not examined. Third, the superiority of NFT over each type of control condition (e.g., sham-NFT, general training methods such as video game play (Nouchi et al., 2021) or meditation (Egner & Gruzelier, 2004b; Nouchi et al., 2021), and no intervention) has not been assessed. This is worthwhile because comparing the efficacy of NFT with sham-NFT is crucial for clarifying the effect specific to modulating the targeted brain activity (Schönenberg et al., 2017), while comparisons with general training methods are essential for demonstrating superiority over other alternatives.

This systematic review and meta-analysis were conducted in accordance with Chapter 4 of the Minds manual for clinical practice guideline development (Minds Manual Developing Committee, 2021). The present study was not pre-registered and, because it is a review study, does not require approval from the ethics committee or informed consent from participants.

2.1 Database search

Two of four authors (I.K., H.N., Ri.O., and J.K.) independently searched for English and Japanese articles investigating NFT effects on attentional performance using the English databases (PubMed [I.K. and H.N.], Scopus [I.K. and H.N.], Web of Science [I.K. and H.N.], and APA PsycInfo [Ri.O. and J.K.]), the Japanese database for scientific studies (JDream Ⅲ [Ri.O. and J.K.]), and the Japanese database for medical trials (Ichu-shi [Ri.O. and J.K.]). Searches of PubMed and Web of Science were conducted in May 2022, while searches of all other databases were conducted in June 2022. Search strings included both attention- AND NFT-related terms. Attention-related terms were “attention*” OR “wakefulness” OR “executive function,” while NFT-related terms were “neurofeedback” OR “neuro feedback” OR “brain machine interface” OR “brain–computer interface” OR (“feedback” AND EEG OR electroencephalogra*). To further eliminate clinical and animal studies, we excluded if the following keywords were present in the title: (ADHD OR disorder* OR deficit* OR patient* OR rehabilitation OR epilep* OR disease* OR depress* OR injur* OR damage*) OR (animal* OR rodent* OR monkey* OR rat OR rats OR macaque*). These searches were conducted in English using PubMed, Scopus, Web of Sciences, and APA PsycInfo, whereas JDream Ⅲ and Ichu-shi were searched using equivalent Japanese terms (See Supplementary Table 1 for the exact search queries used for each database).

2.2 Screening procedures

We then screened the articles retrieved from all databases in two phases. First, we removed articles that did not meet eligibility criteria based on title and abstract (first phase), and then inspected the remaining full articles for eligibility (second phase). During the second phase of screening, the following data were extracted and considered for eligibility: study design, participant demographics, intervention type, control condition, and outcome measures related to attentional performance. The eligibility criteria used in both phases of screenings were as follows: (1) Populations: healthy adults aged 18–64 years; (2) Intervention: NFT using a non-invasive measure of brain activity to improve attentional performance; (3) Comparison: any type of control condition; (4) Outcome: measurements reflecting attentional performance to assess the effect of NFT; (5) Study design: parallel randomized control trial (parallel RCT); and (6) Source: Published in a scholarly peer-reviewed journal and written in English or Japanese. Three authors (I.K., H.N., and Ry.O.) were involved in the screening procedure. We divided the articles into three groups for each phase, and each group was screened independently by two of three authors (I.K. and H.N., H.N. and Ry.O., or Ry.O. and I.K.). In case of disagreement between these two authors, another author reviewed the article (Ry.O. for the groups reviewed by I.K. and H.N., I.K. for those by H.N. and Ry.O., H.N. for those by Ry.O. and I.K.) to reach a consensus. If the three authors still disagreed on eligibility, the other authors inspected the articles, and all six authors reached agreement through further discussion. Of the eligible studies, we excluded from the subsequent meta-analysis if (1) data were not available to calculate the effect size between experimental and control conditions or (2) the study was judged to have a high risk of bias (See Section 2.4. for detailed definitions of risk of bias).

2.3 Data extraction

Two authors (I.K. and H.N.) extracted the data from each eligible article for qualitative synthesis and meta-analysis. For qualitative synthesis, the following details were extracted: participant demographics, intervention, control condition, outcome measures of attentional performance, and adverse events if any. Adverse events are any unfavorable medical events that occurred during a study period, such as headache, nausea, or dizziness (Cumpston et al., 2019). The attentional outcome measures in each study were further categorized according to the targeted attentional performance subsets as follows: executive function (e.g., Flanker effect: measuring interference and response competitions from spatially flanking irrelevant distractors toward targets (Eriksen & Eriksen, 1974; Eriksen, 1995); Simon effect: measuring interference and response competitions from spatially congruent but irrelevant distractors (Simon & Rudell, 1967); Navon task: measuring interference and response competitions from global (or local) distractors (Navon, 1977, 2003); Psychological refractory period effect: measuring temporal separations between two processes that are assumed to share common attentional resources (Pashler, 1994; Welford, 1952); Dual-task performance: measuring interference from one task to another that is assumed to share common attentional resources (Allport et al., 1972; Logan & Gordon, 2001); Task switching: measuring a temporal delay reflecting time to reconfigure one set of attention to another (Monsell, 2003; Vandierendonck et al., 2010); Working memory span task: measuring attentional capacity of working memory during reading sentences (Daneman & Carpenter, 1980); Operation span task: measuring attentional capacity of working memory during using executive function while concurrently maintaining information temporarily (Draheim et al., 2022; Turner & Engle, 1989); Paced auditory serial attention test: measuring attentional capacity during calculating auditorily presented numbers while listening to another number (Gronwall & Sampson, 1974; Tombaugh, 2006); Trail making test: measuring executive function by assessing performances in tracking visually scattered alphabets and numbers sequentially but interchangeably (e.g., 1-A-2-B-3-C …) (Tombaugh, 2004); N-back task: measuring working memory capacity by assessing performance in searching for a target letter or number repeated in a sequence (Jaeggi et al., 2008; Kirchner, 1958); Attentional blink: measuring temporal limitation of selective encoding on a second target presented in a rapid sequence of stimuli (Dux & Marois, 2009; Raymond et al., 1992); Change detection: measuring maintenance of visuospatial array over time (Pashler, 1988; Rensink, 2003); and Priming: measuring the effect on behavior or perception after exposure to certain stimuli (Neely, 1977)), spatial orientation (e.g., Spatial cueing: measuring spatial allocation of attentional resources (Corbetta et al., 2002; Posner, 1978); Visual search: measuring distribution and focus of attentional resources over spatial array (Neisser, 1964; Wolfe, 2020); Multiple object tracking: measuring distribution and focus and maintenance of attentional resources for moving objects over time (Pylyshyn & Storm, 1988; Scimeca & Franconeri, 2015); Subitizing: measuring parallel access to multiple spatially distributed items (Balakrishnan & Ashby, 1992; Pylyshyn, 1989); Dichotic listening test: measuring ability to selectively listen to one message from one ear while ignoring the other from the other ear (Cherry, 1953)), and arousal (e.g., Sustained attention to a response task: measuring task maintenance and response inhibition by requiring repetitive responses to nontarget stimuli except for one critical target (Robertson et al., 1997); Continuous performance task: measuring sustained attention by requiring responses only to targets while refraining from responding to nontargets (Braver, 2012; Rosenberg et al., 2013); and Mind wandering: measuring state of mind whether it remains on the current task or shifts away (Schooler et al., 2004; Smallwood & Schooler, 2015)). Two authors (I.K. and H.N.) independently inspected the measured subsets. If judgments were inconsistent, four authors (I.K., H.N., Ri.O., and J.K.) discussed the classification until an agreement was reached.

Outcome measures of attentional performance after the last NFT session were further extracted for the meta-analysis. Here, as outcome measures, we regarded the change in scores from before NFT, and, if such data were unavailable and the scores before NFT were not significantly different between the interventional and control groups, the scores after NFT. We regarded every experiment in each paper as a single trial, from which the following values were extracted from each for the meta-analysis: mean and standard deviation of the attentional outcome measures after the last NFT session and the number of participants in the experimental and control groups. From either these values or t-values, the standardized mean difference (SMD) was calculated using Hedges’ g. If neither SMDs nor t-values were available, we contacted the corresponding author of the study for data request. For consistency across studies, we reversed the sign of SMD if a lower value reflected higher attentional performance (e.g., reaction time, tiredness, or sleepiness), so that higher SMD consistently indicates that NFT increased attentional performance compared to controls.

2.4 Risk of bias assessment

Two authors (I.K. and H.N.) assessed the risk of bias using the Cochrane Collaboration’s tool (Higgins et al., 2011) toward all the studies whose SMDs are available. This tool classifies risk of bias as high, unclear, or low in six domains: selection bias, performance bias, detection bias, attrition bias, reporting bias, and others (including conflict of interest, early trial termination, and incorrect sample size determination or statistical methods) (For the exact criteria for determining high or low in each domain, see Supplementary Table 2). Following Minds Manual Developing Committee (2021), each domain was scored two points for high, one for unclear, or zero for low, and then points were summed to yield an overall risk score. We then classified overall risk as high if the score was more than seven or more than two domains were high, as low if total score was less than five and less than two domains were high, or as moderate in other cases. Individual studies judged to own an overall low or moderate risk of bias were included in the meta-analyses. A summary table on risk of bias evaluation was generated using robvis (https://mcguinlu.shinyapps.io/robvis/; McGuinness & Higgins, 2021).

2.5 Statistics

The basic characteristics of studies that passed the second screening were tabulated, including participant demographics, adverse events, targeted brain location and activity for NFT, type of control condition, and assessed subset of attentional performance (executive function, spatial orientation, and/or arousal) (Table 1). We then performed a standard pairwise meta-analysis using a random-effects model. All results for the same outcome and population were pooled into a single dataset. Publication bias was visualized using a funnel plot and evaluated using Begg’s and Egger’s tests (with P < 0.1 on both tests considered significant following the criterion of Chapter 4 of the Minds manual for clinical practice guideline development (Minds Manual Developing Committee, 2021)). Heterogeneity across studies was assessed using Cochran’s Q and the I2 statistic.

Table 1.

Experimental characteristics of the studies included in the qualitative synthesis.

StudyParticipant demographicsAdverse eventsNeuroimaging modalityIntervention(s) and recording sitesType of control conditionType of feedbackIntervention periodTotal intervention durationSubtype of attentional performance
Cai et al. (2021)  Healthy adults Not reported EEG Increase theta power (4–8 Hz); decrease beta power (12–30 Hz) (Fp1, Fp2) No intervention Visual 5 weeks More than 32 min ×5 sessions 
Demazure et al. (2021)  Healthy adults Not reported EEG Increase beta power (12–30 Hz) and decrease both theta power (4–8 Hz) and alpha power (8–12 Hz) (F3, F4, O1, O2) No intervention Visual Unclear 90 min (number of sessions is unclear) S + A 
Kim et al. (2021)  Healthy adults Not reported EEG Increase or decrease N1 and P1 amplitude (Fz, FCz, FC1, FC2, Cz) sham-NFT Visual 4 weeks 60 min (number of sessions is unclear) 
Mishra et al. (2021)  Healthy adults Not reported EEG Decrease alpha frontal-sensory synchrony (8–12 Hz) sham-NFT Visual 3–5 weeks 40 min ×10 sessions S + A 
Nouchi et al. (2021)  Healthy adults Not reported NIRS Increase bilateral dorsolateral prefrontal cortex activity Block-puzzle game Visual 4 weeks 20 min ×28 sessions S + E 
Shokri and Nosratabadi (2021)  Novice male basketball players Not reported EEG Increase SMR power (13–15 Hz) and alpha power (8–12 Hz); decrease theta power (4–8 Hz) and beta3 power (18–35 Hz) (Cz, Cpz) Biofeedback Unclear 8 weeks 20 min ×24 sessions 
Xia et al. (2021)  Healthy adults Not reported NIRS Increase prefrontal (Ch. 1–22), parietal (Ch. 23–40), and temporal (Ch. 41,42) lobe activities No intervention Visual 3 days more than 30 min ×3 sessions A + E 
Brandmeyer and Delorme (2020)  Healthy adults Not reported EEG Increase frontal-midline theta power (4–6 Hz; Fpz, Fz, F7, F8, Cz, P7, P8, Oz) sham-NFT Visual 2 weeks 30 min ×8 sessions A + E 
Christie et al. (2020)  Ice hockey players Not reported EEG Increase SMR power (12–15 Hz); decrease theta power (4–7 Hz) and high beta power (23–35 Hz) (Cz) No intervention Audio-Visual 4.5 months 75 min ×15 sessions 
Gordon et al. (2020)  Healthy adults Not reported EEG Increase alpha power (8–12 Hz; Pz) Working memory training / Visual search training task / No intervention Visual 5 weeks 20–50 min ×10 sessions 
Maszczyk et al. (2020)  Judo athletes Not reported EEG Increase beta1 power (13–20 Hz); decrease theta power (4–7.5 Hz) and beta2 power (20–30 Hz) (Cz) sham-NFT Audio-Visual 6 weeks (first half) 10 min ×15 sessions → (second half) 4 min ×15 sessions 
Morgenroth et al. (2020)  Females with high anxiety Not reported fMRI Increase functional connectivity between anterior cingulate cortex and left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex sham-NFT Visual around 1 week 14 min ×2 sessions 
Wang et al. (2020)  Healthy adults Not reported fMRI Increase left or right intra-parietal sulcus activity Unclear Visual 3 weeks 90–120 min ×3 sessions 
Balconi et al. (2019)  Healthy adults with a driver’s license Not reported EEG Unclear No intervention Visual 3 weeks 10–20 min ×21 sessions S + A + E 
Crivelli, Fronda, and Balconi (2019)  Athletes and healthy adults Not reported EEG Increase and decrease N2 amplitude (forehead) No intervention Audio 2 weeks 10–20 min ×14 sessions S + E 
Crivelli, Fronda, Venturella, et al. (2019)  Healthy adults with mild stress Not reported EEG Increase or decrease beta power (12–30 Hz) and decrease alpha power (8–12 Hz) (Fz, Cz, Pz) No intervention Visual 4 weeks 10–20 min ×28 sessions S + E 
Kim et al. (2019)  Healthy adults Not reported fMRI Increase default-mode, salience, and executive network activities sham-NFT Visual Unclear 300–390 s ×2 sessions 
Sherwood et al. (2019)  Healthy adults Not reported fMRI Decrease auditory cortex activity sham-NFT Audio 3 weeks 8 sessions (duration per day is unclear) 
Arvaneh et al. (2018)  Healthy adults Not reported EEG Increase P300 power (Fz, C3, Cz, C4, P3, Pz, P4, Oz) No intervention Visual 1 day less than 1 h (including preparation) ×1 session S + A 
Gonçalves et al. (2018)  Healthy adults Not reported EEG Increase SMR power (13–15 Hz); decrease theta power (4–8 Hz) (Cz) NFT in the reverse direction Visual 1 day 1 session (duration per day is unclear) S + A + E 
Mikicin et al. (2018)  Professional soldiers Not reported EEG Increase beta power (12–22 Hz; F3, F4, P3, P4) sham-NFT Unclear Unclear 40 min ×20 sessions 
Chow et al. (2017)  Healthy adults None EEG Increase alpha power (8–12 Hz; Pz) mindfulness meditation / sham-NFT Audio Unclear 18 min (number of sessions is unclear) A + E 
Hudak et al. (2017)  Healthy adults with high impulsivity and subclinical characteristics of ADHD Not reported NIRS Increase frontal lobe activity EMG-based biofeedback Visual 2 weeks 8 sessions (duration per day is unclear) 
Bhayee et al. (2016)  Healthy adults Not reported EEG Unclear (two electrodes on the forehead) No intervention Audio 6 weeks 42 sessions (duration per day is unclear) 
Gadea et al. (2016)  Healthy adult females Not reported EEG Increase SMR power (12–15 Hz; Cz) sham-NFT Audio Unclear 45 min (number of sessions is unclear) 
Hsueh et al. (2016)  Healthy adults None EEG Increase alpha power (8–12 Hz; C3a, C3p, Cza, Czp, C4a, C4p) sham-NFT Visual 4 weeks 36 min ×12 sessions 
Cheng et al. (2015)  Golfers Not reported EEG Increase SMR power (12–15 Hz; Cz) sham-NFT Audio 5 weeks 30–45 min ×8 sessions 
Kober et al. (2015)  Healthy adults Not reported EEG Increase SMR power (12–15 Hz; Cz, FPz) sham-NFT Visual 3–4 weeks 40 min ×10 sessions 
Okazaki et al. (2015)  Healthy adults Not reported MEG Increase alpha power (8–12 Hz) sham-NFT Visual Unclear Unclear 
Enriquez-Geppert et al. (2014)  Healthy adults Not reported EEG Increase frontal-midline theta power (Individualized Peak; Fz, FC1, FC2, FCz, Cz) sham-NFT Visual 2 weeks 30 min ×8 sessions 
Gevensleben et al. (2014)  Healthy adults Not reported EEG Increase and decrease SCP (Cz) sham-NFT Visual 3 weeks 90 min ×8 sessions 
Salari et al. (2014)  Healthy adults Not reported EEG Increase gamma power (40 Hz; PO7, PO8) sham-NFT Visual 3 days 2 h 16 min ×3 sessions 
Studer et al. (2014)  Healthy adults Not reported EEG ① Increase beta power (22–36 Hz); decrease theta power (4–7 Hz) ② Increase and decrease SCP No intervention Visual 5 weeks 50 min ×10 sessions S + A + E 
Ghaziri et al. (2013)  Healthy adults Not reported EEG Increase beta1 power (15–18 Hz; F4, P4) sham-NFT / No intervention Visual 13.5 weeks 30 min ×40 sessions 
Wang and Hsieh (2013)  Adults with high anxiety Not reported EEG Increase alpha power (8–13 Hz); decrease delta power (1–4 Hz) (C3 or C4) sham-NFT Audio 7.5 weeks 27 min ×15 sessions 
Logemann et al. (2010)  Healthy adults with high impulsivity or high inattention Not reported EEG Increase SMR power (Cz, Pz, C3, P3, C4) sham-NFT Unclear 8 weeks 22 min ×16 sessions A + E 
Fritson et al. (2008)  Healthy adults Not reported EEG Increase SMR power (12–15 Hz); decrease theta power (4–7 Hz) and beta power (22–36 Hz) (C3, C4) sham-NFT Audio-Visual Unclear Unclear A + E 
Egner and Gruzelier (2004a)  Healthy adults Not reported EEG ① Increase beta1 power (15–18 Hz); decrease theta power (4–7 Hz) and high beta power (22–30 Hz) (Cz) ② Increase SMR power (12–15 Hz); decrease theta power (4–7 Hz) and high beta power (22–30 Hz) (Cz) Alexander Technique Audio-Visual 10 weeks 15 min ×10 sessions S + A 
Vernon et al. (2003)  Healthy adults Not reported EEG Increase SMR power (12–15 Hz); decrease theta power (4–8 Hz) and beta power (18–22 Hz) (Cz) No intervention / NFT (increase theta power) Audio-Visual 4 weeks 15 min ×8 sessions A + E 
Mikulka et al. (2002)  Healthy adults Not reported EEG Increase beta power (13–30 Hz); decrease theta power (4–7 Hz) and alpha power (8–12 Hz) (F3, F4, O1, O2) sham-NFT / NFT (decrease engagement index) Unclear 1 day 40 min ×1 session 
Hord et al. (1976)  Sleep deprivation (40 h) Not reported EEG Increase alpha power (10 Hz; O2) NFT (decrease alpha and theta powers (4-14 Hz)) Audio 2 days 60 min ×2 sessions 
StudyParticipant demographicsAdverse eventsNeuroimaging modalityIntervention(s) and recording sitesType of control conditionType of feedbackIntervention periodTotal intervention durationSubtype of attentional performance
Cai et al. (2021)  Healthy adults Not reported EEG Increase theta power (4–8 Hz); decrease beta power (12–30 Hz) (Fp1, Fp2) No intervention Visual 5 weeks More than 32 min ×5 sessions 
Demazure et al. (2021)  Healthy adults Not reported EEG Increase beta power (12–30 Hz) and decrease both theta power (4–8 Hz) and alpha power (8–12 Hz) (F3, F4, O1, O2) No intervention Visual Unclear 90 min (number of sessions is unclear) S + A 
Kim et al. (2021)  Healthy adults Not reported EEG Increase or decrease N1 and P1 amplitude (Fz, FCz, FC1, FC2, Cz) sham-NFT Visual 4 weeks 60 min (number of sessions is unclear) 
Mishra et al. (2021)  Healthy adults Not reported EEG Decrease alpha frontal-sensory synchrony (8–12 Hz) sham-NFT Visual 3–5 weeks 40 min ×10 sessions S + A 
Nouchi et al. (2021)  Healthy adults Not reported NIRS Increase bilateral dorsolateral prefrontal cortex activity Block-puzzle game Visual 4 weeks 20 min ×28 sessions S + E 
Shokri and Nosratabadi (2021)  Novice male basketball players Not reported EEG Increase SMR power (13–15 Hz) and alpha power (8–12 Hz); decrease theta power (4–8 Hz) and beta3 power (18–35 Hz) (Cz, Cpz) Biofeedback Unclear 8 weeks 20 min ×24 sessions 
Xia et al. (2021)  Healthy adults Not reported NIRS Increase prefrontal (Ch. 1–22), parietal (Ch. 23–40), and temporal (Ch. 41,42) lobe activities No intervention Visual 3 days more than 30 min ×3 sessions A + E 
Brandmeyer and Delorme (2020)  Healthy adults Not reported EEG Increase frontal-midline theta power (4–6 Hz; Fpz, Fz, F7, F8, Cz, P7, P8, Oz) sham-NFT Visual 2 weeks 30 min ×8 sessions A + E 
Christie et al. (2020)  Ice hockey players Not reported EEG Increase SMR power (12–15 Hz); decrease theta power (4–7 Hz) and high beta power (23–35 Hz) (Cz) No intervention Audio-Visual 4.5 months 75 min ×15 sessions 
Gordon et al. (2020)  Healthy adults Not reported EEG Increase alpha power (8–12 Hz; Pz) Working memory training / Visual search training task / No intervention Visual 5 weeks 20–50 min ×10 sessions 
Maszczyk et al. (2020)  Judo athletes Not reported EEG Increase beta1 power (13–20 Hz); decrease theta power (4–7.5 Hz) and beta2 power (20–30 Hz) (Cz) sham-NFT Audio-Visual 6 weeks (first half) 10 min ×15 sessions → (second half) 4 min ×15 sessions 
Morgenroth et al. (2020)  Females with high anxiety Not reported fMRI Increase functional connectivity between anterior cingulate cortex and left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex sham-NFT Visual around 1 week 14 min ×2 sessions 
Wang et al. (2020)  Healthy adults Not reported fMRI Increase left or right intra-parietal sulcus activity Unclear Visual 3 weeks 90–120 min ×3 sessions 
Balconi et al. (2019)  Healthy adults with a driver’s license Not reported EEG Unclear No intervention Visual 3 weeks 10–20 min ×21 sessions S + A + E 
Crivelli, Fronda, and Balconi (2019)  Athletes and healthy adults Not reported EEG Increase and decrease N2 amplitude (forehead) No intervention Audio 2 weeks 10–20 min ×14 sessions S + E 
Crivelli, Fronda, Venturella, et al. (2019)  Healthy adults with mild stress Not reported EEG Increase or decrease beta power (12–30 Hz) and decrease alpha power (8–12 Hz) (Fz, Cz, Pz) No intervention Visual 4 weeks 10–20 min ×28 sessions S + E 
Kim et al. (2019)  Healthy adults Not reported fMRI Increase default-mode, salience, and executive network activities sham-NFT Visual Unclear 300–390 s ×2 sessions 
Sherwood et al. (2019)  Healthy adults Not reported fMRI Decrease auditory cortex activity sham-NFT Audio 3 weeks 8 sessions (duration per day is unclear) 
Arvaneh et al. (2018)  Healthy adults Not reported EEG Increase P300 power (Fz, C3, Cz, C4, P3, Pz, P4, Oz) No intervention Visual 1 day less than 1 h (including preparation) ×1 session S + A 
Gonçalves et al. (2018)  Healthy adults Not reported EEG Increase SMR power (13–15 Hz); decrease theta power (4–8 Hz) (Cz) NFT in the reverse direction Visual 1 day 1 session (duration per day is unclear) S + A + E 
Mikicin et al. (2018)  Professional soldiers Not reported EEG Increase beta power (12–22 Hz; F3, F4, P3, P4) sham-NFT Unclear Unclear 40 min ×20 sessions 
Chow et al. (2017)  Healthy adults None EEG Increase alpha power (8–12 Hz; Pz) mindfulness meditation / sham-NFT Audio Unclear 18 min (number of sessions is unclear) A + E 
Hudak et al. (2017)  Healthy adults with high impulsivity and subclinical characteristics of ADHD Not reported NIRS Increase frontal lobe activity EMG-based biofeedback Visual 2 weeks 8 sessions (duration per day is unclear) 
Bhayee et al. (2016)  Healthy adults Not reported EEG Unclear (two electrodes on the forehead) No intervention Audio 6 weeks 42 sessions (duration per day is unclear) 
Gadea et al. (2016)  Healthy adult females Not reported EEG Increase SMR power (12–15 Hz; Cz) sham-NFT Audio Unclear 45 min (number of sessions is unclear) 
Hsueh et al. (2016)  Healthy adults None EEG Increase alpha power (8–12 Hz; C3a, C3p, Cza, Czp, C4a, C4p) sham-NFT Visual 4 weeks 36 min ×12 sessions 
Cheng et al. (2015)  Golfers Not reported EEG Increase SMR power (12–15 Hz; Cz) sham-NFT Audio 5 weeks 30–45 min ×8 sessions 
Kober et al. (2015)  Healthy adults Not reported EEG Increase SMR power (12–15 Hz; Cz, FPz) sham-NFT Visual 3–4 weeks 40 min ×10 sessions 
Okazaki et al. (2015)  Healthy adults Not reported MEG Increase alpha power (8–12 Hz) sham-NFT Visual Unclear Unclear 
Enriquez-Geppert et al. (2014)  Healthy adults Not reported EEG Increase frontal-midline theta power (Individualized Peak; Fz, FC1, FC2, FCz, Cz) sham-NFT Visual 2 weeks 30 min ×8 sessions 
Gevensleben et al. (2014)  Healthy adults Not reported EEG Increase and decrease SCP (Cz) sham-NFT Visual 3 weeks 90 min ×8 sessions 
Salari et al. (2014)  Healthy adults Not reported EEG Increase gamma power (40 Hz; PO7, PO8) sham-NFT Visual 3 days 2 h 16 min ×3 sessions 
Studer et al. (2014)  Healthy adults Not reported EEG ① Increase beta power (22–36 Hz); decrease theta power (4–7 Hz) ② Increase and decrease SCP No intervention Visual 5 weeks 50 min ×10 sessions S + A + E 
Ghaziri et al. (2013)  Healthy adults Not reported EEG Increase beta1 power (15–18 Hz; F4, P4) sham-NFT / No intervention Visual 13.5 weeks 30 min ×40 sessions 
Wang and Hsieh (2013)  Adults with high anxiety Not reported EEG Increase alpha power (8–13 Hz); decrease delta power (1–4 Hz) (C3 or C4) sham-NFT Audio 7.5 weeks 27 min ×15 sessions 
Logemann et al. (2010)  Healthy adults with high impulsivity or high inattention Not reported EEG Increase SMR power (Cz, Pz, C3, P3, C4) sham-NFT Unclear 8 weeks 22 min ×16 sessions A + E 
Fritson et al. (2008)  Healthy adults Not reported EEG Increase SMR power (12–15 Hz); decrease theta power (4–7 Hz) and beta power (22–36 Hz) (C3, C4) sham-NFT Audio-Visual Unclear Unclear A + E 
Egner and Gruzelier (2004a)  Healthy adults Not reported EEG ① Increase beta1 power (15–18 Hz); decrease theta power (4–7 Hz) and high beta power (22–30 Hz) (Cz) ② Increase SMR power (12–15 Hz); decrease theta power (4–7 Hz) and high beta power (22–30 Hz) (Cz) Alexander Technique Audio-Visual 10 weeks 15 min ×10 sessions S + A 
Vernon et al. (2003)  Healthy adults Not reported EEG Increase SMR power (12–15 Hz); decrease theta power (4–8 Hz) and beta power (18–22 Hz) (Cz) No intervention / NFT (increase theta power) Audio-Visual 4 weeks 15 min ×8 sessions A + E 
Mikulka et al. (2002)  Healthy adults Not reported EEG Increase beta power (13–30 Hz); decrease theta power (4–7 Hz) and alpha power (8–12 Hz) (F3, F4, O1, O2) sham-NFT / NFT (decrease engagement index) Unclear 1 day 40 min ×1 session 
Hord et al. (1976)  Sleep deprivation (40 h) Not reported EEG Increase alpha power (10 Hz; O2) NFT (decrease alpha and theta powers (4-14 Hz)) Audio 2 days 60 min ×2 sessions 

A qualitative summary was provided for each study according to the top-line items.

Studies included in the meta-analysis.

Abbreviations: NIRS, Near infra-red spectroscopy; EEG, Electroencephalogram; fMRI, Functional magnetic resonance imaging; MEG, Magnetoencephalography; NFT, Neurofeedback training; Ch., Channel; EMG, Electromyography; SMR, Sensorimotor rhythm; SCP, Slow cortical potential; E, Executive function; S, Spatial orientation; A, Arousal.

To confirm the robustness of the result on meta-analysis, sensitivity analyses were performed. Here, we confirmed whether the result of the significance test was the same and whether the magnitude of the estimated SMD was not severely changed by the following conditions: (1) changing the analysis method to a fixed-effects model, (2) including the studies with a high risk of bias, (3) excluding the SMDs calculated from the scores after the last session of NFT instead of the change in scores, and (4) restricting the studies to those performing NFT with EEG. The last one was conducted because the neural mechanisms induced by NFT may be different between NFT with EEG and those with fMRI or NIRS (Nicholson et al., 2020).

To explore dose-response relationship, a meta-regression analysis was applied to examine the correlation between the effect size and total duration (or number of sessions) of NFT in each study. Here, we defined the total duration of NFT as the duration of each session × the number of sessions. If the duration of NFT in each session was described as a range (e.g., 30–40 min), the mid-value was used (e.g., 35 min). Since the effect size may decrease with publication year (Yang et al., 2023), a meta-regression analysis was also performed to investigate the relationship between publication year and effect size.

We exploratory conducted additional subgroup meta-analyses to clarify the efficacy of NFT for each subset of attentional performance (executive function, spatial orientation, or arousal), type of outcome measure (subjective [i.e., a self-assessment through a questionnaire] or objective [i.e., a quantifiable measurement that can be recorded by a device]), type of brain activity targeted for intervention (SMR or alpha power), and type of control (sham-NFT, general training, or no intervention). Because several studies assessed multiple effects with same participant groups, toward all meta- and meta-regression analyses, we applied a cluster-robust variance estimation method (Fisher & Tipton, 2015) with the clubSandwich package. This method can handle multiple interdependent effect sizes within each study.

To validate whether the number of trials in each main and subgroup meta-analysis was sufficient to reliably detect the effect, we performed the power-analysis for each of these analyses. The detailed method was described in Onagawa et al. (2023), but, in brief, the statistical power and required number of sample size were estimated from the SMD and heterogeneity (τ2) calculated from each analysis using the dmetar package. Alpha level was set to 0.05, and we considered statistical power (1 - β) greater than 0.8 as sufficient to detect the effect reliably. All statistical analyses were performed using metafor ver. 3.4.0 (Viechtbauer, 2010), meta ver. 5.5.0 (Balduzzi et al., 2019), clubSandwich ver. 0.5.8, and dmetar ver 0.1.0 in R ver. 4.1.2 (https://www.r-project.org/).

3.1 Search results

The literature review process is illustrated in Figure 1. We retrieved 3,337 articles from six databases using the indicated search strings, of which 3,159 were excluded based on the title or abstract (e.g., a case report (Mroczkowska et al., 2014), a review (Sitaram et al., 2017), an observational study (Santamaría-Vázquez et al., 2022), an interventional study but not with neurofeedback (Falcone et al., 2018), or conference proceedings (Zhang et al., 2019)). The remaining 178 articles were then screened for eligibility by full-text assessment, of which 137 were excluded for the following reasons: study of a clinical population (1 article), age of subjects (17 articles), article type (27 articles), no NFT (10 articles), no control conditions (22 articles), no outcome measure for attentional performance (38 articles), and non-parallel RCT (22 articles). The remaining 41 articles were retained for qualitative synthesis and 15 were further included in meta-analyses. Reasons for exclusion from the meta-analyses were insufficient data reported to calculate effect sizes (24 articles) and high overall risk of bias (2 articles).

Fig. 1.

PRISMA flowchart of article search and inclusion.

Fig. 1.

PRISMA flowchart of article search and inclusion.

Close modal

3.2 Study and sample characteristics

Table 1 summarizes the main characteristics of all studies included in the qualitative synthesis. All were published between 1976 and 2021 (Fig. 2A), with more than half appearing after 2013. Thirteen studies examined executive function alone, five examined spatial orientation alone, and seven examined arousal alone, while three examined both executive function and spatial orientation, six examined both executive function and arousal, four examined both spatial orientation and arousal, and three examined all three subsets of attentional performance (Fig. 2B).

Fig. 2.

Qualitative assessment of NFT studies included in the qualitative synthesis. (A) Histogram of publication year. (B) Subset(s) of attentional performance assessed in NFT studies. (C) Channels recorded for EEG-based NFT. (D) Frequency band(s) targeted to modulate in EEG-based NFT studies. Single and multiple indicate that a band was modulated by intervening single and multiple band(s), respectively. Abbreviations: E, Executive function; S, Spatial orientation; A, Arousal; SMR, sensorimotor rhythm.

Fig. 2.

Qualitative assessment of NFT studies included in the qualitative synthesis. (A) Histogram of publication year. (B) Subset(s) of attentional performance assessed in NFT studies. (C) Channels recorded for EEG-based NFT. (D) Frequency band(s) targeted to modulate in EEG-based NFT studies. Single and multiple indicate that a band was modulated by intervening single and multiple band(s), respectively. Abbreviations: E, Executive function; S, Spatial orientation; A, Arousal; SMR, sensorimotor rhythm.

Close modal

To measure the brain activity, 34 studies used EEG, 4 used fMRI, 2 used NIRS, and 1 used magnetoencephalography (MEG). More than half of the studies provided feedback with visual stimuli (24 studies), followed by audio stimuli (8 studies), and visuo-audio stimuli (5 studies). The stimulus type of feedback was unclear in four studies.

Among studies of EEG-based NFT, the vertex (Cz) was the most frequently applied electrode location, followed by the right frontal (F4), midline frontal (Fz), central (C3, C4), and midline parietal (Pz) locations (Fig. 2C). The band power(s) targeted to modulate in EEG- or MEG-based NFT were alpha, SMR, beta, gamma, delta, and/or theta (Fig. 2D). Most studies targeted to increase alpha power with a single band (5 out of 6 studies), while beta and SMR powers were more often targeted to increase with multiple bands (beta power: 5 out of 7 studies; SMR power: 6 out of 10 studies) than with single band. Two studies targeted to modulate slow cortical potential (SCP). To investigate the NFT effect on executive function, five, four, eight, and three studies applied NFT targeted to increase alpha, beta, SMR, and theta power, respectively, while, to explore the NFT effect on spatial orientation, two, four, three, and one studies applied NFT to increase alpha, beta, SMR, and gamma power, respectively. To evaluate the NFT effect toward arousal, three studies targeted to increase alpha, while six, five, and one studies targeted to increase beta, SMR, and theta power, respectively.

Twenty-six studies compared NFT with sham-NFT, 12 studies with no intervention, 4 studies with general training, and 2 with biofeedback without EEG. The control condition was unknown in one study.

3.3 Adverse events

Only 2 of 41 studies reported the presence or absence of adverse events, and neither reported any such events during or after NFT.

3.4 Risk of bias assessment

Of the 17 parallel RCTs evaluated, overall risk of bias was high in 2 studies, moderate in 13 studies, and low in 2 studies (“Overall” column in Fig. 3). Only three studies explicitly described the random sequence generation method and were evaluated as low risk for selection bias (column D1 in Fig. 3), while the others did not describe the allocation method and were judged as unclear risk. Only one study described the allocation concealment, which was judged as low risk for selection bias (column D2 in Fig. 3). Two studies were single-blinded or unblinded and thus judged as high risk of performance bias (column D3 in Fig. 3), 5 were double-blinded and deemed low risk, and 10 did not describe the blinding procedure and were deemed as unclear risk of performance bias. Most studies did not explicitly mention the blinding status of the evaluator, so the risk of detection bias was unclear (column D4 in Fig. 3). Further, one study was explicitly unblinded. Thirteen studies were judged as low risk of attrition bias (Column D5 in Fig. 3), while four studies were judged as high risk since the drop-out rate was more than 10% or intention-to-treat protocols were violated. Fifteen studies had unclear reporting bias as none performed pre-registration (column D6), while one study was judged as low risk because data were collected according to the pre-registered protocol, and one was judged as high risk because the reported items were incomplete without any explicit reason. Finally, seven studies were rated high risk of other biases due to conflicts of interest (e.g., one of the authors was the employee of or the experiment was funded by a company selling neurofeedback-related devices) (three studies), early trial termination (one study), or inadequate statistical methods (e.g., no considerations for multiple comparisons) (three studies). The other 10 studies were rated unclear risk due to the lack of information any on these items. Two studies with high overall risk of bias were excluded from the subsequent meta-analysis.

Fig. 3.

Risk-of-bias assessment for each study considered for the meta-analysis. The red, yellow, and green symbols in each domain column indicate high, unclear, and low risk of bias, respectively. In the overall column, red, yellow, and green indicate high, moderate, and low risk of bias, respectively. Based on this analysis, two studies (red in the overall column) were eliminated from the meta-analysis.

Fig. 3.

Risk-of-bias assessment for each study considered for the meta-analysis. The red, yellow, and green symbols in each domain column indicate high, unclear, and low risk of bias, respectively. In the overall column, red, yellow, and green indicate high, moderate, and low risk of bias, respectively. Based on this analysis, two studies (red in the overall column) were eliminated from the meta-analysis.

Close modal

3.5 General effects of NFT on attentional performance

A total of 16 trials from 15 published studies were included in the primary meta-analysis. NFT enhanced attentional performance with small effect size (SMD = 0.27, 95% confidence interval (CI) = 0.10–0.45, t = 6.68, P = 0.0076; Figure 5A; See Supplementary Fig. 1 for the forest plot). There was no significant heterogeneity across the trials (Q = 52.5, P = 0.93, I2 = 0%) and no significant publication bias according to the funnel plot (Fig. 4), Begg’s test (z = 1.68, P = 0.093), and Egger’s test (t = 1.24, P = 0.22).

Fig. 4.

Funnel plot showing the standardized mean differences from all experimental results included in the meta-analysis. The relative symmetry suggests low risk of publication bias.

Fig. 4.

Funnel plot showing the standardized mean differences from all experimental results included in the meta-analysis. The relative symmetry suggests low risk of publication bias.

Close modal
Fig. 5.

Forest plot of NFT efficacy for improving attentional performance. (A) Overall effect size estimate. (B–E) Estimated effect sizes for each subtype of attentional performance (B), type of outcome measures (C), type of brain activity targeted for NFT (D), and type of control (E). The squares are the means and error bars are the 95% confidence intervals of estimated effect sizes comparing NFT to control conditions.

Fig. 5.

Forest plot of NFT efficacy for improving attentional performance. (A) Overall effect size estimate. (B–E) Estimated effect sizes for each subtype of attentional performance (B), type of outcome measures (C), type of brain activity targeted for NFT (D), and type of control (E). The squares are the means and error bars are the 95% confidence intervals of estimated effect sizes comparing NFT to control conditions.

Close modal

We performed sensitivity analyses to ensure the robustness of the result. The repeated meta-analysis using a fixed-effects model (instead of the original random-effects model) revealed the small effect size (SMD = 0.27, 95% CI = 0.10–0.44, t = 3.77, P = 0.0076). The result of the meta-analysis also exhibited the small effect size by including the two studies with high overall risk of bias (18 trials, SMD = 0.25, 95% CI = 0.10–0.41, t = 3.80, P = 0.0054), by restricting the studies to those performing NFT with EEG (14 trials, SMD = 0.26, 95% CI = 0.03–0.48, t = 2.74, P = 0.030), or by excluding the effect sizes calculated from the scores after NFT (i.e., not from the change of the score after NFT) (15 trials, SMD = 0.28, 95% CI = 0.10–0.46, t = 3.73, P = 0.0088).

3.6 Meta-regression analyses

Meta-regression analysis revealed no significant relationship between effect size and the total duration of NFT (12 trials; beta = 0.0003, 95% CI = −0.0006–0.0012, t = 1.04, P = 0.37; Supplementary Fig. 2A) or the number of NFT sessions (15 trials; beta = 0.013, 95% CI = −0.013–0.038, t = 1.58, P = 0.21; Supplementary Fig. 2B). We also found no significant correlation between effect size and the publication years (16 trials; beta = 0.0007, 95% CI = −0.035–0.036, t = 0.05, P = 0.96; Supplementary Fig. 2C).

3.7 Subgroup meta-analyses

We performed subgroup meta-analyses to evaluate the effect of NFT on each subtype of attentional performance (Fig. 5B), type of outcome measure (objective or subjective; Fig. 5C), type of targeted brain activity to be increased with NFT (Fig. 5D), and type of control condition (Fig. 5E). The estimated effect on executive function (12 trials) was small (SMD = 0.27, 95% CI = 0.10–0.43, t = 4.06, P = 0.0088; heterogeneity: Q = 45.8, P = 0.72, I2 = 0%) and that on spatial orientation (6 trials) was moderate (SMD = 0.50, 95% CI = 0.034–0.97, t = 2.96, P = 0.041; heterogeneity: Q = 3.12, P = 0.87, I2 = 0%), while that on arousal (7 trials) was not significant (SMD = 0.25, 95% CI = −0.18–0.68, t = 1.58, P = 0.18; heterogeneity: Q = 4.99, P = 0.96, I2 = 0%) (Fig. 5B). However, there was no significant difference in NFT effect size among subtypes (F3, 3.34 = 0.56, P = 0.68). Effect size was small regarding the 15 trials using objective measures (SMD = 0.33, 95% CI = 0.19–0.48, t = 5.67, P = 0.0017; heterogeneity: Q = 42.01, P = 0.93, I2 = 0%), and not significant across the four trials using subjective measures (SMD = −0.040, 95% CI = −0.97–0.89, t = −0.22, P = 0.85; heterogeneity: Q = 6.62, P = 0.83, I2 = 0%) (Fig. 5C). The difference in NFT effect between types of outcome measures was not significant (F1, 2.31 = 4.16, P = 0.16).

When intervention targets were considered separately, NFT demonstrated no significant effect on either alpha power (four trials, SMD = 0.24, 95% CI = −0.45–0.92, t = 1.57, P = 0.26; heterogeneity: Q = 16.3, P = 0.30, I2 = 0%) or SMR power (six trials, SMD = 0.16, 95% CI = −0.58–0.90, t = 0.81, P = 0.49; heterogeneity: Q = 23.8, P = 0.25, I2 = 14.7%). We did not evaluate the effect of NFT using other neuroimaging modalities because only one study used NIRS or fMRI.

Notably, NFT had no significant effect on overall attentional performance when compared to sham-NFT as the control condition (eight trials, SMD = 0.18, 95% CI = −0.18–0.53, t = 1.57, P = 0.21; heterogeneity: Q = 16.9, P = 0.98, I2 = 0%) (Fig. 5E), and also no significant effects on each subtype of attentional performance either (executive function: six trials, SMD = 0.20, 95% CI = −0.16–0.56, t = 2.08, P = 0.15; spatial orientation: two trials, SMD = 0.09, 95% CI = −2.81–3.00, t = 0.39, P = 0.76; and arousal: five trials, SMD = 0.22, 95% CI = −0.49–0.92, t = 1.05, P = 0.38). The effect of NFT was not significantly superior to general training (two trials, SMD = 0.29, 95% CI = −1.05–1.63, t = 2.77, P = 0.22; heterogeneity: Q = 6.50, P = 0.44, I2 = 0%). In contrast, NFT was superior to no intervention (seven trials, SMD = 0.36, 95% CI = 0.003–0.73, t = 2.90, P = 0.049; heterogeneity: Q = 27.5, P = 0.44, I2 = 0%). Again, there was no significant difference in effect among trials using different control conditions (F2, 1.81 = 0.43, P = 0.70). We found that most subgroup meta-analyses (8 out of 10; other than the studies investigating the effect on executive function and on objective measures) in our study were underpowered (i.e., 1−β was less than 0.8; Supplementary Table 3).

The present study evaluated the effects of NFT on attentional performance using both a qualitative synthesis and meta-analysis of parallel RCTs. We also performed subgroup meta-analyses to assess effects of NFT on specific subsets of attentional performance (executive function, spatial orientation, and arousal) as well as types of outcome measure (subjective or objective), targeted brain activity to be increased with NFT (alpha or SMR power), and control condition (sham-NFT, general training, and no intervention). A search of multiple English and Japanese databases identified 41 studies appropriate for qualitative synthesis and 16 trials (15 studies) acceptable for meta-analyses. The main meta-analysis revealed a significant overall effect of NFT on attentional performance, and subgroup meta-analyses revealed significant effects on executive function and spatial orientation but not on arousal. However, neither the overall effect nor the effects on individual subsets were significant when compared to sham-NFT.

4.1 The tendency of the studies revealed from the qualitative synthesis

Since 2017, over half of the studies included in our qualitative synthesis have been published, indicating that research on NFT for improving attentional performance is still growing. This is supported by the fact that most of the studies included here were exploratory, since they did not conduct pre-registration (except for Kim et al. (2019)). However, as the number of publications has increased monotonically over the past 5 years, more results in this area will be gathered to improve the quality of meta-analysis in the next decade.

Approximately 75% of the studies used EEG to measure brain activity in NFT. This pattern was similar to that found in a recent systematic review of NFT (Onagawa et al., 2023). The reason why EEG-based NFT is more common than fMRI- or NIRS-based NFT can be that EEG is relatively inexpensive compared to fMRI and NIRS. Another reason can be the difference in the length of period since its initial report. To the best of our knowledge, the efficacy of EEG-based NFT was shown approximately 30–40 years (Kamiya, 1968) before that of fMRI- (Yoo & Jolesz, 2002) and NIRS-based NFT (Mihara, 2011). Nevertheless, NIRS is as convenient as EEG to measure brain activity, and fMRI has the advantage of high spatial resolution. Moreover, fMRI-based neurofeedback is gaining attention to examine the causal relationship between brain regions and behavior (Finn, 2021). Thus, as the results of fMRI- or NIRS-based NFT accumulate, it would be tempting to compare the effect on attentional performance across these modalities with a meta-analysis.

The most frequent NFT protocol studied to improve attentional performance was NFT targeted to increase SMR power, followed by that to increase beta, alpha, theta, and gamma power. This pattern differs from that revealed in the recent systematic review of NFT to treat patients with ADHD (Van Doren et al., 2019): The three major protocols for ADHD were NFT to increase SMR power and decrease theta power; to increase beta power and decrease theta power; and to regulate SCP. The most notable difference was the proportion of studies using NFT to regulate SCP (2 out of 41 studies in our review, while 3 out of 10 studies in Van Doren et al. (2019)). This might be because of the difference in the initial NFT protocols reported to increase attentional performance. While, in the studies included in our qualitative synthesis, NFT to increase alpha, SMR, and beta were first demonstrated to increase arousal (Hord et al., 1976), executive function (Vernon et al., 2003), and spatial orientation (Egner & Gruzelier, 2004a), respectively, in the systematic review by Van Doren et al. (2019), NFT to regulate SCP was initially reported to alleviate ADHD-related symptoms. Future research will systematically examine which NFT protocol is best for improving attentional performance.

In this systematic review, we highlighted a major concern regarding NFT studies in this field: several studies provided incomplete information on NFT. For example, of the 41 included studies, 6, 6, and 7 studies did not indicate the intervention period, the number of sessions, and the daily intervention duration, respectively. Moreover, most studies did not report whether any adverse events occurred or not. The detailed NFT protocol is crucial for clarifying the optimal protocol for improving attentional performance, while understanding its possible adverse events is important to evaluate the safety of NFT. Future studies should comply with the recent guidelines for reporting of NFT studies (Ros et al., 2020).

4.2 Significant overall NFT effect on attentional performance

Our meta-analysis of 15 studies indicated that NFT was superior to controls for enhancing overall attentional performance but with a small effect size (SMD = 0.27). We confirmed that this result was not severely affected by the choice of the analysis method (random-effects model vs. fixed-effects model) or by the change of the exclusion criteria in the meta-analysis (including the studies with high risk of bias, excluding the effect sizes calculated only from the scores after NFT, or excluding the studies conducting NFT other than EEG). The effect size was comparable to a previous meta-analysis of NFT efficacy for inattention (SMD = 0.38) and hyperactivity-impulsivity (SMD = 0.25) in patients with ADHD compared to controls (Van Doren et al., 2019). In contrast, Da Silva & De Souza reported a larger overall mean effect of NFT on attentional performance compared to our findings (d’ = 0.61; Da Silva & De Souza, 2021). These differences can be explained by the different indices used in these two studies: Hedges’ g used in the current study reflects a relatively small sample size (n = 8–25 in each study), whereas d’ used in Da Silva & De Souza (2021) did not take into account the sample size.

We did not find a significant correlation between the NFT dose and efficacy. Lim et al. (2019) reported that at least 24 sessions are required for NFT to improve attentional performance in patients with ADHD. Considering that only 3 out of 15 studies included in the meta-analysis conducted NFT for more than 24 sessions and the number of sessions of NFT ranged from 1 to 42 sessions, healthy adults might enhance attentional performance with a rather small number of sessions of NFT compared to patients with ADHD.

4.3 Variable NFT effect on each subgroup

The meta-analysis revealed significant effects of NFT on executive function and spatial orientation, consistent with previous systematic reviews showing that NFT can enhance executive function (Da Silva & De Souza, 2021) and visual attention (Ordikhani-Seyedlar et al., 2016). However, in a review by Viviani & Vallesi (2021), only 16% of the included studies reported that NFT improved executive function. These discrepancies may be explained by differences in the definition of success: our study focused on improvement of attentional performance measured by behavioral responses, whereas Viviani & Vallesi (2021) also considered EEG changes compared to its controls. In contrast, we found no significant effect of NFT on arousal compared to controls. We surmise that NFT may not be effective in improving arousal compared to in spatial orientation because the number of studies included in arousal was comparable to that in spatial orientation. Further studies are warranted to address why NFT can improve some attentional domains but not others.

While NFT produced an improvement in attentional performance in general, the meta-analysis revealed no significant effect on individual NFT protocols aimed to increase alpha or SMR power. This is surprising, at least in NFT targeted to increase SMR power, because SMR protocol has long been proposed to enhance attentional performance in healthy adults (Egner & Gruzelier, 2001). These non-significant effects of NFT on alpha and SMR power may be explained by the limited number of studies targeting each band power (four trials for alpha and six for SMR power). Another potential reason is that enhancement of a given band power may not necessarily improve all subsets of attentional performance consistently. For example, several studies have suggested that NFT protocols enhancing low beta and SMR power influence distinct aspects of attentional performance (Egner & Gruzelier, 2004a, 2004b).

The meta-analysis also revealed no significant effect of NFT on attentional performance when compared specifically to sham-NFT or to general training. Arina and colleagues (2017) reported that even sham-NFT can induce unspecific effect such as reducing tension or anxiety. This finding suggests that NFT effects specific to modulation of certain EEG band power cannot be evaluated only by comparison with a non-intervention group. Nevertheless, only about half of the studies (21 out of 41) compared NFT effect with sham-NFT, and the details of the blinding protocols were unclear in most studies. As suggested in other systematic reviews on NFT (Da Silva & De Souza, 2021; Rogala et al., 2016), stringent triple-blind parallel RCTs are needed to compare the effects of NFT with sham-NFT.

4.4 Limitations

The present study has a potential limitation: we cannot assess the NFT effect on each neuroimaging modality. This is because only one study conducted NFT with NIRS (Nouchi et al., 2021) or fMRI (Kim et al., 2019) to enhance attentional performance. NIRS is a convenient tool to measure brain activity as EEG, and several studies suggest that NFT with NIRS is useful for the rehabilitation after stroke (Mihara et al., 2021; Rahman et al., 2020). NFT with fMRI is also a promising tool to modulate brain activity (Koizumi et al., 2020). Future studies are required to determine which neuroimaging modality can best improve attentional performance.

Another limitation of this study was that it was not pre-registered. Therefore, we performed meta-regression analysis and subgroup meta-analysis exploratory. As this might introduce publication bias, these results should be interpreted with caution. We also acknowledge the limited number of trials in our subgroup meta-analyses. If the number of trials included in a meta-analysis is small (e.g., ≤ five trials (Zhou & Shen, 2022)), the result of that analysis must be cautiously interpreted. Further studies will be required to validate which subtype of attentional performance is more benefitted by NFT, which NFT protocol can most effectively improve attentional performance, and whether NFT is beneficial compared to sham-NFT or general training.

Aside from these limitations, it should be noted that only eight trials were included in the meta-analysis that compared the effect of NFT with that of sham-NFT. This is because the effect of the studies that compared with no intervention can include placebo effects, indicating that the effect of NFT in these trials could be influenced by the participant’s motivation or expectation towards neurofeedback (Thibault et al., 2017). In fact, one study reported that the subjective sleep quality scores improved with sham-NFT, even though sham-NFT did not regulate neural activity (Schabus et al., 2017; Thibault et al., 2017). Therefore, a comparison with sham-NFT will be essential to reveal the effects specific to the modulation of the targeted brain activity.

Another thing to note is that only two trials compared NFT effect with general training. Comparison with general training is also crucial to clarify which training (NFT or other) is more useful for enhancing attentional performance. Taken together, more studies comparing the NFT effect with sham-NFT and general training are needed to scientifically validate the NFT effect on attentional performance and the superiority of NFT over alternative methods.

4.5 Conclusion

From the currently available studies, NFT significantly enhances attentional performance compared to its control, without a significant dose–response relationship in healthy adults. Furthermore, NFT has been observed to significantly enhance executive function and spatial orientation, although whether NFT can significantly enhance arousal is still unknown. Unfortunately, approximately half of the studies included in our meta-analysis did not compare NFT to sham-NFT, and the superiority of NFT over sham-NFT and alternative methods for enhancing attentional performance remains uncertain. To address these gaps, future multicenter parallel RCTs with large sample sizes are required to compare NFT effect with sham-NFT in a rigorous triple-blinded design, as well as with non-NFT training.

The data (mean(s) and standard deviation(s) for experimental and control groups and the converted effect size(s) for each study) and codes used in this study were shared in OSF (https://osf.io/5ze7v/).

Ikko Kimura: Methodology, Investigation, Data Curation, Formal analysis, Writing – Original Draft, and Visualization. Hiroki Noyama: Methodology, Investigation, Data Curation, Formal analysis, Writing – Original Draft, and Visualization. Ryoji Onagawa: Investigation, Data Curation, and Writing – Review & Editing. Mitsuaki Takemi: Conceptualization, Project Administration, and Writing – Review & Editing. Rieko Osu: Supervision, Writing – Review & Editing. Jun-ichiro Kawahara: Supervision, Project Administration, and Writing – Review & Editing.

The authors declare that they have no known competing financial interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence the work reported in this paper.

This work was conducted as a part of Braintech Guidebook development in JST Moonshot R&D to MT (Grant Number JPMJMS2012). We thank the members of the Evidence Evaluation Committee for Braintech Guidebook, a specially organized group for the Moonshot R&D project, for their comments on the manuscript.

Supplementary material for this article is available with the online version here: https://doi.org/10.1162/imag_a_00053.

Allport
,
D. A.
,
Antonis
,
B.
, &
Reynolds
,
P.
(
1972
).
On the division of attention: A disproof of the single channel hypothesis
.
Q J Exp Psychol
,
24
,
225
235
. https://doi.org/10.1080/00335557243000102
Arina
,
G.
,
Osina
,
E.
,
Dobrushina
,
O.
, &
Aziatskaya
,
G.
(
2017
).
Sham-neurofeedback as an Intervention: Placebo or Nocebo?
European Psychiatry: The Journal of the Association of European Psychiatrists
,
41
(
S1
),
S253
S254
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eurpsy.2017.02.046
Arvaneh
,
M.
,
Robertson
,
I. H.
, &
Ward
,
T. E.
(
2018
).
A P300-based brain-computer interface for improving attention
.
Front Hum Neurosci
,
12
,
524
. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2018.00524
Balakrishnan
,
J. D.
, &
Ashby
,
F. G.
(
1992
).
Subitizing: Magical numbers or mere superstition
?
Psychol Res
,
54
,
80
90
. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00937136
Balconi
,
M.
,
Crivelli
,
D.
, &
Angioletti
,
L.
(
2019
).
Efficacy of a neurofeedback training on attention and driving performance: Physiological and behavioral measures
.
Front Neurosci
,
13
,
996
. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2019.00996
Balduzzi
,
S.
,
Rücker
,
G.
, &
Schwarzer
,
G.
(
2019
).
How to perform a meta-analysis with R: A practical tutorial
.
Evid Based Ment Health
,
22
,
153
160
. https://doi.org/10.1136/ebmental-2019-300117
Berger
,
H.
(
1929
).
Über das elektrenkephalogramm des menschen
.
Archiv f Psychiatrie
,
87
,
527
570
. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf01797193
Bhayee
,
S.
,
Tomaszewski
,
P.
,
Lee
,
D. H.
,
Moffat
,
G.
,
Pino
,
L.
,
Moreno
,
S.
, &
Farb
,
N. A. S.
(
2016
).
Attentional and affective consequences of technology supported mindfulness training: A randomised, active control, efficacy trial
.
BMC Psychol
,
4
,
60
. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-016-0168-6
Brandmeyer
,
T.
, &
Delorme
,
A.
(
2020
).
Closed-loop frontal midlineθ neurofeedback: A novel approach for training focused-attention meditation
.
Front Hum Neurosci
,
14
,
246
. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2020.00246
Braver
,
T. S.
(
2012
).
The variable nature of cognitive control: A dual mechanisms framework
.
Trends Cogn Sci
,
16
,
106
113
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.12.010
Cai
,
H.
,
Zhang
,
Y.
,
Xiao
,
H.
,
Zhang
,
J.
,
Hu
,
B.
, &
Hu
,
X.
(
2021
).
An adaptive neurofeedback method for attention regulation based on the internet of things
.
IEEE Internet Things J
,
8
,
15829
15838
. https://doi.org/10.1109/JIOT.2021.3083745
Carrasco
,
M.
(
2018
).
How visual spatial attention alters perception
.
Cogn Process
,
19
,
77
88
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-018-0883-4
Cavanagh
,
J. F.
,
Zambrano-Vazquez
,
L.
, &
Allen
,
J. J. B.
(
2012
).
Theta lingua franca: A common mid-frontal substrate for action monitoring processes
.
Psychophysiology
,
49
,
220
238
. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01293.x
Cheng
,
M. Y.
,
Huang
,
C.-J.
,
Chang
,
Y.-K.
,
Koester
,
D.
,
Schack
,
T.
, &
Hung
,
T.-M.
(
2015
).
Sensorimotor rhythm neurofeedback enhances golf putting performance
.
J Sport Exerc Psychol
,
37
,
626
636
. https://doi.org/10.1123/jsep.2015-0166
Cherry
,
E. C.
(
1953
).
Some experiments on the recognition of speech, with one and with two ears
.
J Acoust Soc Am
,
25
,
975
979
. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.1907229
Chow
,
T.
,
Javan
,
T.
,
Ros
,
T.
, &
Frewen
,
P.
(
2017
).
EEG dynamics of mindfulness meditation versus alpha neurofeedback: A sham-controlled study
.
Mindfulness
,
8
,
572
584
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-016-0631-8
Christie
,
S.
,
Bertollo
,
M.
, &
Werthner
,
P.
(
2020
).
The effect of an integrated neurofeedback and biofeedback training intervention on ice hockey shooting performance
.
J Sport Exerc Psychol
,
42
(
1
),
34
47
. https://doi.org/10.1123/jsep.2018-0278
Chun
,
M. M.
,
Golomb
,
J. D.
, &
Turk-Browne
,
N. B.
(
2011
).
A taxonomy of external and internal attention
.
Annu Rev Psychol
,
62
,
73
101
. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100427
Corbetta
,
M.
,
Kincade
,
J. M.
, &
Shulman
,
G. L.
(
2002
).
Neural systems for visual orienting and their relationships to spatial working memory
.
J Cogn Neurosci
,
14
,
508
523
. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892902317362029
Corbetta
,
M.
,
Patel
,
G.
, &
Shulman
,
G. L.
(
2008
).
The reorienting system of the human brain: From environment to theory of mind
.
Neuron
,
58
,
306
324
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2008.04.017
Crivelli
,
D.
,
Fronda
,
G.
, &
Balconi
,
M.
(
2019
).
Neurocognitive enhancement effects of combined mindfulness–neurofeedback training in sport
.
Neuroscience
,
412
,
83
93
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2019.05.066
Crivelli
,
D.
,
Fronda
,
G.
,
Venturella
,
I.
, &
Balconi
,
M.
(
2019
).
Supporting mindfulness practices with brain-sensing devices. Cognitive and electrophysiological evidences
.
Mindfulness
,
10
,
301
311
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-018-0975-3
Cumpston
,
M.
,
Li
,
T.
,
Page
,
M. J.
,
Chandler
,
J.
,
Welch
,
V. A.
,
Higgins
,
J. P.
, &
Thomas
,
J.
(
2019
).
Updated guidance for trusted systematic reviews: A new edition of the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions
.
Cochrane Database Syst Rev
,
10
,
ED000142
. https://doi.org/10.1002/14651858.ED000142
Da Silva
,
J. C.
, &
De Souza
,
M. L.
(
2021
).
Neurofeedback training for cognitive performance improvement in healthy subjects: A systematic review
.
Psychol Neurosci
,
14
,
262
279
. https://doi.org/10.1037/pne0000261
Daneman
,
M.
, &
Carpenter
,
P. A.
(
1980
).
Individual differences in working memory and reading
.
J Verb Learn Verb Behav
,
19
,
450
466
. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(80)90312-6
de Brito
,
M. A.
,
Fernandes
,
J. R.
,
Esteves
,
N. S.
,
Müller
,
V. T.
,
Alexandria
,
D. B.
,
Pérez
,
D. I. V.
,
Slimani
,
M.
,
Brito
,
C. J.
,
Bragazzi
,
N. L.
, &
Miarka
,
B.
(
2022
).
The effect of neurofeedback on the reaction time and cognitive performance of athletes: A systematic review and meta-analysis
.
Front Hum Neurosci
,
16
,
868450
. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2022.868450
Demazure
,
T.
,
Karran
,
A.
,
Léger
,
P. M.
,
Labonté-LeMoyne
,
É.
,
Sénécal
,
S.
,
Fredette
,
M.
, &
Babin
,
G.
(
2021
).
Enhancing sustained attention
.
Bus Inf Syst Eng
,
63
,
653
668
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12599-021-00701-3
Diamond
,
A.
(
2013
).
Executive functions
.
Annu Rev Psychol
,
64
,
135
168
. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750
Draheim
,
C.
,
Pak
,
R.
,
Draheim
,
A. A.
, &
Engle
,
R. W.
(
2022
).
The role of attention control in complex real-world tasks
.
Psychon Bull Rev
,
29
,
1143
1197
. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-021-02052-2
Dux
,
P. E.
, &
Marois
,
R.
(
2009
).
The attentional blink: A review of data and theory
.
Atten Percept Psychophys
,
71
,
1683
1700
. https://doi.org/10.3758/APP.71.8.1683
Egeth
,
H.
, &
Kahneman
,
D.
(
1975
).
Attention and effort
.
Am J Psychol
,
88
,
339
. https://doi.org/10.2307/1421603
Egner
,
T.
, &
Gruzelier
,
J. H.
(
2001
).
Learned self-regulation of EEG frequency components affects attention and event-related brain potentials in humans
.
Neuroreport
,
12
,
4155
4159
. https://doi.org/10.1097/00001756-200112210-00058
Egner
,
T.
, &
Gruzelier
,
J. H.
(
2003
).
Ecological validity of neurofeedback: Modulation of slow wave EEG enhances musical performance
.
Neuroreport
,
14
(
9
),
1221
1224
. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/12824763/
Egner
,
T.
, &
Gruzelier
,
J. H.
(
2004a
).
EEG biofeedback of low beta band components: Frequency-specific effects on variables of attention and event-related brain potentials
.
Clin Neurophysiol
,
115
,
131
139
. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1388-2457(03)00353-5
Egner
,
T.
, &
Gruzelier
,
J. H.
(
2004b
).
The temporal dynamics of electroencephalographic responses to alpha/theta neurofeedback training in healthy subjects
.
J Neurother
,
8
,
43
57
. https://doi.org/10.1300/J184v08n01_04
Enriquez-Geppert
,
S.
,
Huster
,
R. J.
,
Figge
,
C.
, &
Herrmann
,
C. S.
(
2014
).
Self-regulation of frontal-midline theta facilitates memory updating and mental set shifting
.
Front Behav Neurosci
,
8
,
420
. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00420
Eriksen
,
B. A.
, &
Eriksen
,
C. W.
(
1974
).
Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task
.
Percept Psychophys
,
16
,
143
149
. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03203267
Eriksen
,
C. W.
(
1995
).
The flankers task and response competition: A useful tool for investigating a variety of cognitive problems
.
Vis Cogn
,
2
,
101
118
. https://doi.org/10.1080/13506289508401726
Falcone
,
B.
,
Wada
,
A.
,
Parasuraman
,
R.
, &
Callan
,
D. E.
(
2018
).
Individual differences in learning correlate with modulation of brain activity induced by transcranial direct current stimulation
.
PLoS One
,
13
,
e0197192
. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0197192
Fan
,
J.
,
McCandliss
,
B. D.
,
Sommer
,
T.
,
Raz
,
A.
, &
Posner
,
M. I.
(
2002
).
Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks
.
J Cogn Neurosci
,
14
,
340
347
. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892902317361886
Finn
,
E. S.
(
2021
).
Is it time to put rest to rest
?
Trends Cogn Sci
,
25
,
1021
1032
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2021.09.005
Fisher
,
Z.
, &
Tipton
,
E.
(
2015
).
robumeta: An R-package for robust variance estimation in meta-analysis
.
arXiv
. https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.1503.02220
Fritson
,
K. K.
,
Wadkins
,
T. A.
,
Gerdes
,
P.
, &
Hof
,
D.
(
2008
).
The impact of neurotherapy on college students’ cognitive abilities and emotions
.
J Neurother
,
11
,
1
9
. https://doi.org/10.1080/10874200802143998
Gadea
,
M.
,
Aliño
,
M.
,
Garijo
,
E.
,
Espert
,
R.
, &
Salvador
,
A.
(
2016
).
Testing the benefits of neurofeedback on selective attention measured through dichotic listening
.
Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback
,
41
(
2
),
157
164
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10484-015-9323-8
Gevensleben
,
H.
,
Albrecht
,
B.
,
Lütcke
,
H.
,
Auer
,
T.
,
Dewiputri
,
W. I.
,
Schweizer
,
R.
,
Moll
,
G.
,
Heinrich
,
H.
, &
Rothenberger
,
A.
(
2014
).
Neurofeedback of slow cortical potentials: Neural mechanisms and feasibility of a placebo-controlled design in healthy adults
.
Front Hum Neurosci
,
8
,
990
. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00990
Ghaziri
,
J.
,
Tucholka
,
A.
,
Larue
,
V.
,
Blanchette-Sylvestre
,
M.
,
Reyburn
,
G.
,
Gilbert
,
G.
,
Lévesque
,
J.
, &
Beauregard
,
M.
(
2013
).
Neurofeedback training induces changes in white and gray matter
.
Clin EEG Neurosci
,
44
,
265
272
. https://doi.org/10.1177/1550059413476031
Gonçalves
,
Ó. F.
,
Carvalho
,
S.
,
Mendes
,
A. J.
,
Leite
,
J.
, &
Boggio
,
P. S.
(
2018
).
Neuromodulating attention and mind-wandering processes with a single session real time EEG
.
Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback
,
43
,
143
151
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10484-018-9394-4
Gordon
,
S.
,
Todder
,
D.
,
Deutsch
,
I.
,
Garbi
,
D.
,
Alkobi
,
O.
,
Shriki
,
O.
,
Shkedy-Rabani
,
A.
,
Shahar
,
N.
, &
Meiran
,
N.
(
2020
).
Effects of neurofeedback and working memory-combined training on executive functions in healthy young adults
.
Psychol Res
,
84
,
1586
1609
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-019-01170-w
Gronwall
,
D. M.
, &
Sampson
,
H.
(
1974
).
The psychological effects of concussion
.
Auckland University Press; Oxford University Press
. https://wellcomecollection.org/works/ht6dzxn5
Gruzelier
,
J. H.
(
2014
).
EEG-neurofeedback for optimising performance. I: A review of cognitive and affective outcome in healthy participants
.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev
,
44
,
124
141
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.09.015
Hanslmayr
,
S.
,
Sauseng
,
P.
,
Doppelmayr
,
M.
,
Schabus
,
M.
, &
Klimesch
,
W.
(
2005
).
Increasing individual upper alpha power by neurofeedback improves cognitive performance in human subjects
.
Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback
,
30
,
1
10
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10484-005-2169-8
Higgins
,
J. P. T.
,
Altman
,
D. G.
,
Gøtzsche
,
P. C.
,
Jüni
,
P.
,
Moher
,
D.
,
Oxman
,
A. D.
,
Savovic
,
J.
,
Schulz
,
K. F.
,
Weeks
,
L.
,
Sterne
,
J. A. C.
,
Cochrane Bias Methods Group
, &
Cochrane Statistical Methods Group
(
2011
).
The Cochrane Collaboration’s tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials
.
BMJ
,
343
,
d5928
. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.d5928
Hord
,
D. J.
,
Lubin
,
A.
,
Tracy
,
M. L.
,
Jensma
,
B. W.
, &
Johnson
,
L. C.
(
1976
).
Feedback for high EEG alpha does not maintain performance or mood during sleep loss
.
Psychophysiology
,
13
,
58
61
. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1976.tb03338.x
Hsueh
,
J. J.
,
Chen
,
T. S.
,
Chen
,
J. J.
, &
Shaw
,
F. Z.
(
2016
).
Neurofeedback training of EEG alpha rhythm enhances episodic and working memory
.
Hum Brain Mapp
,
37
,
2662
2675
. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23201
Hudak
,
J.
,
Blume
,
F.
,
Dresler
,
T.
,
Haeussinger
,
F. B.
,
Renner
,
T. J.
,
Fallgatter
,
A. J.
,
Gawrilow
,
C.
, &
Ehlis
,
A. C.
(
2017
).
Near-infrared spectroscopy-based frontal lobe neurofeedback integrated in virtual reality modulates brain and behavior in highly impulsive adults
.
Front Hum Neurosci
,
11
,
425
. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2017.00425
Jaeggi
,
S. M.
,
Buschkuehl
,
M.
,
Jonides
,
J.
, &
Perrig
,
W. J.
(
2008
).
Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
,
105
,
6829
6833
. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0801268105
Kamiya
,
J.
(
1968
).
Conscious control of brain waves
.
Psychol Today
,
1
,
56
60
. https://philpapers.org/rec/KAMCCO
Kim
,
H. C.
,
Tegethoff
,
M.
,
Meinlschmidt
,
G.
,
Stalujanis
,
E.
,
Belardi
,
A.
,
Jo
,
S.
,
Lee
,
J.
,
Kim
,
D. Y.
,
Yoo
,
S. S.
, &
Lee
,
J. H.
(
2019
).
Mediation analysis of triple networks revealed functional feature of mindfulness from real-time fMRI neurofeedback
.
Neuroimage
,
195
,
409
432
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.03.066
Kim
,
S.
,
Emory
,
C.
, &
Choi
,
I.
(
2021
).
Neurofeedback training of auditory selective attention enhances speech-in-noise perception
.
Front Hum Neurosci
,
15
,
676992
. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2021.676992
Kirchner
,
W. K.
(
1958
).
Age differences in short-term retention of rapidly changing information
.
J Exp Psychol
,
55
,
352
358
. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0043688
Kober
,
S. E.
,
Witte
,
M.
,
Stangl
,
M.
,
Väljamäe
,
A.
,
Neuper
,
C.
, &
Wood
,
G.
(
2015
).
Shutting down sensorimotor interference unblocks the networks for stimulus processing: An SMR neurofeedback training study
.
Clin Neurophysiol
,
126
,
82
95
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2014.03.031
Koizumi
,
A.
,
Hori
,
T.
,
Maniscalco
,
B.
,
Hayase
,
M.
,
Mishima
,
R.
,
Kawashima
,
T.
,
Miyata
,
J.
,
Aso
,
T.
,
Lau
,
H.
,
Takahashi
,
H.
, &
Amano
,
K.
(
2020
).
Atypical spatial frequency dependence of visual metacognition among schizophrenia patients
.
Neuroimage Clin
,
27
,
102296
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2020.102296
Korchounov
,
A.
, &
Ziemann
,
U.
(
2011
).
Neuromodulatory neurotransmitters influence LTP-like plasticity in human cortex: A pharmaco-TMS study
.
Neuropsychopharmacology
,
36
,
1894
1902
. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2011.75
Lambez
,
B.
,
Harwood-Gross
,
A.
,
Golumbic
,
E. Z.
, &
Rassovsky
,
Y.
(
2020
).
Non-pharmacological interventions for cognitive difficulties in ADHD: A systematic review and meta-analysis
.
J Psychiatr Res
,
120
,
40
55
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2019.10.007
Lim
,
C. G.
,
Poh
,
X. W. W.
,
Fung
,
S. S. D.
,
Guan
,
C.
,
Bautista
,
D.
,
Cheung
,
Y. B.
,
Zhang
,
H.
,
Yeo
,
S. N.
,
Krishnan
,
R.
, &
Lee
,
T. S.
(
2019
).
A randomized controlled trial of a brain-computer interface based attention training program for ADHD
.
PLoS One
,
14
,
e0216225
. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0216225
Logan
,
G. D.
, &
Gordon
,
R. D.
(
2001
).
Executive control of visual attention in dual-task situations
.
Psychol Rev
,
108
,
393
434
. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.108.2.393
Logemann
,
H. N. A.
,
Lansbergen
,
M. M.
,
Van Os
,
T. W. D. P.
,
Böcker
,
K. B. E.
, &
Kenemans
,
J. L.
(
2010
).
The effectiveness of EEG-feedback on attention, impulsivity and EEG: A sham feedback controlled study
.
Neurosci Lett
,
479
,
49
53
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2010.05.026
Lubar
,
J. F.
, &
Shouse
,
M. N.
(
1976
).
EEG and behavioral changes in a hyperkinetic child concurrent with training of the sensorimotor rhythm (SMR): A preliminary report
.
Biofeedback Self Regul
,
1
,
293
306
. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01001170
Luck
,
S. J.
,
Gaspelin
,
N.
,
Folk
,
C. L.
,
Remington
,
R. W.
, &
Theeuwes
,
J.
(
2021
).
Progress toward resolving the attentional capture debate
.
Vis Cogn
,
29
,
1
21
. https://doi.org/10.1080/13506285.2020.1848949
Maszczyk
,
A.
,
Dobrakowski
,
P.
,
Nitychoruk
,
M.
,
Żak
,
M.
,
Kowalczyk
,
M.
, &
Toborek
,
M.
(
2020
).
The effect of neurofeedback training on the visual processing efficiency in Judo Athletes
.
J Hum Kinet
,
71
,
219
227
. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7052722/
Mazur
,
L. M.
,
Campbell
,
A.
,
Comitz
,
E.
,
Planting
,
D.
,
Lowen
,
S.
,
Mosaly
,
P. R.
,
Belger
,
A.
,
Chera
,
B. S.
, &
Marks
,
L. B.
(
2017
).
Positive effects of neurofeedback intervention on radiation oncology physicians’ workload during go/no-go performance test
.
Travail Hum
,
80
,
113
. https://doi.org/10.3917/th.801.0113
McGuinness
,
L. A.
, &
Higgins
,
J. P. T.
(2021).
Risk-of-bias VISualization (robvis): An R package and Shiny web app for visualizing risk-of-bias assessments
.
Research Synthesis Methods
,
12
(
1
),
55
61
. https://doi.org/10.1002/jrsm.1411
Mihara
,
M.
(
2011
).
[Neurorehabilitative intervention with neurofeedback system using functional near-infrared spectroscopy]
.
Rinsho Shinkeigaku
,
51
,
924
926
. https://doi.org/10.5692/clinicalneurol.51.924
Mihara
,
M.
,
Fujimoto
,
H.
,
Hattori
,
N.
,
Otomune
,
H.
,
Kajiyama
,
Y.
,
Konaka
,
K.
,
Watanabe
,
Y.
,
Hiramatsu
,
Y.
,
Sunada
,
Y.
,
Miyai
,
I.
, &
Mochizuki
,
H.
(
2021
).
Effect of neurofeedback facilitation on poststroke gait and balance recovery: A randomized controlled trial
.
Neurology
,
96
,
e2587
e2598
. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000011989
Mikicin
,
M.
,
Sczypanska
,
M.
, &
Skwarek
,
K.
(
2018
).
Neurofeedback needs support! Effects of neurofeedback-EEG training in terms of the level of attention and arousal control in sports shooters
.
Balt J Health Phys Act
,
10
,
8
. https://www.balticsportscience.com/journal/vol10/iss3/8/
Mikulka
,
P. J.
,
Scerbo
,
M. W.
, &
Freeman
,
F. G.
(
2002
).
Effects of a biocybernetic system on vigilance performance
.
Hum Factors
,
44
,
654
664
. https://doi.org/10.1518/0018720024496944
Minds Manual Developing Committee
(
2021
).
Minds Manual for Guideline Development 2020 ver. 3.0
.
Japan Council for Quality Health Care
,
Tokyo
. https://minds.jcqhc.or.jp/docs/various/manual_2020/ver3_0/pdf/chap4_manual_2020ver3_0.pdf
Mishra
,
J.
,
Lowenstein
,
M.
,
Campusano
,
R.
,
Hu
,
Y.
,
Diaz-Delgado
,
J.
,
Ayyoub
,
J.
,
Jain
,
R.
, &
Gazzaley
,
A.
(
2021
).
Closed-loop neurofeedback of α synchrony during goal-directed attention
.
J Neurosci
,
41
,
5699
5710
. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3235-20.2021
Miyake
,
A.
,
Friedman
,
N. P.
,
Emerson
,
M. J.
,
Witzki
,
A. H.
,
Howerter
,
A.
, &
Wager
,
T. D.
(
2000
).
The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis
.
Cogn Psychol
,
41
,
49
100
. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734
Monsell
,
S.
(
2003
).
Task switching
.
Trends Cogn Sci
,
7
,
134
140
. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00028-7
Morgenroth
,
E.
,
Saviola
,
F.
,
Gilleen
,
J.
,
Allen
,
B.
,
Lührs
,
M.
,
W Eysenck
,
M.
, &
Allen
,
P.
(
2020
).
Using connectivity-based real-time fMRI neurofeedback to modulate attentional and resting state networks in people with high trait anxiety
.
Neuroimage Clin
,
25
,
102191
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2020.102191
Mroczkowska
,
D.
,
Białkowska
,
J.
, &
Rakowska
,
A.
(
2014
).
Neurofeedback as supportive therapy after stroke. Case report
.
Postȩpy Psychiatrii i Neurologii
,
23
,
190
201
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pin.2014.09.002
Navon
,
D.
(
1977
).
Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception
.
Cogn Psychol
,
9
,
353
383
. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(77)90012-3
Navon
,
D.
(
2003
).
What does a compound letter tell the psychologist’s mind
?
Acta Psychol (Amst)
,
114
,
273
309
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2003.06.002
Neely
,
J. H.
(
1977
).
Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: Roles of inhibitionless spreading activation and limited-capacity attention
.
J Exp Psychol Gen
,
106
,
226
254
. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.106.3.226
Neisser
,
U.
(
1964
).
Visual search
.
Sci Am
,
210
,
94
103
. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/14183749/
Nicholson
,
A. A.
,
Ros
,
T.
,
Jetly
,
R.
, &
Lanius
,
R. A.
(
2020
).
Regulating posttraumatic stress disorder with neurofeedback: Regaining control of the mind
.
J Mil Veteran Fam Health
,
6
,
3
15
. https://doi.org/10.3138/jmvfh.2019-0032
Nouchi
,
R.
,
Nouchi
,
H.
,
Dinet
,
J.
, &
Kawashima
,
R.
(
2021
).
Cognitive training with neurofeedback using NIRS improved cognitive functions in young adults: Evidence from a randomized controlled trial
.
Brain Sci
,
12
,
5
. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12010005
Nowlis
,
D. P.
, &
Kamiya
,
J.
(
1970
).
The control of electroencephalographic alpha rhythms through auditory feedback and the associated mental activity
.
Psychophysiology
,
6
,
476
484
. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1970.tb01756.x
Oberauer
,
K.
(
2019
).
Working memory and attention—A conceptual analysis and review
.
J Cogn
,
2
,
36
. https://doi.org/10.5334/joc.58
Okazaki
,
Y. O.
,
Horschig
,
J. M.
,
Luther
,
L.
,
Oostenveld
,
R.
,
Murakami
,
I.
, &
Jensen
,
O.
(
2015
).
Real-time MEG neurofeedback training of posterior alpha activity modulates subsequent visual detection performance
.
Neuroimage
,
107
,
323
332
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.12.014
Onagawa
,
R.
,
Muraoka
,
Y.
,
Hagura
,
N.
, &
Takemi
,
M.
(
2023
).
An investigation of the effectiveness of neurofeedback training on motor performance in healthy adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis
.
Neuroimage
,
270
,
120000
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2023.120000
Ordikhani-Seyedlar
,
M.
,
Lebedev
,
M. A.
,
Sorensen
,
H. B. D.
, &
Puthusserypady
,
S.
(
2016
).
Neurofeedback therapy for enhancing visual attention: State-of-the-art and challenges
.
Front Neurosci
,
10
,
352
. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2016.00352
Pashler
,
H.
(
1988
).
Familiarity and visual change detection
.
Percept Psychophys
,
44
,
369
378
. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03210419
Pashler
,
H.
(
1994
).
Overlapping mental operations in serial performance with preview
.
Q J Exp Psychol A
,
47
,
161
91
; discussion 193-9, 201–5. https://doi.org/10.1080/14640749408401148
Pashler
,
H. E.
(
1998
). 1: Introduction. In
Pisella
L.
,
Arzi
M.
, &
Rossetti
Y.
(Eds.),
The psychology of attention
(Vol.
15
).
MIT Press
. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/5677.003.0003
Petersen
,
S. E.
, &
Posner
,
M. I.
(
2012
).
The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after
.
Annu Rev Neurosci
,
35
,
73
89
. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150525
Posner
,
M. I.
(
1978
).
Chronometric explorations of mind
.
Lawrence Erlbaum
. https://psycnet.apa.org/record/1980-02340-000
Posner
,
M. I.
, &
Petersen
,
S. E.
(
1990
).
The attention system of the human brain
.
Annu Rev Neurosci
,
13
,
25
42
. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ne.13.030190.000325
Pylyshyn
,
Z.
(
1989
).
The role of location indexes in spatial perception: A sketch of the FINST spatial-index model
.
Cognition
,
32
,
65
97
. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(89)90014-0
Pylyshyn
,
Z. W.
, &
Storm
,
R. W.
(
1988
).
Tracking multiple independent targets: Evidence for a parallel tracking mechanism
.
Spat Vis
,
3
,
179
197
. https://doi.org/10.1163/156856888x00122
Rahman
,
M. A.
,
Siddik
,
A. B.
,
Ghosh
,
T. K.
,
Khanam
,
F.
, &
Ahmad
,
M.
(
2020
).
A narrative review on clinical applications of fNIRS
.
J Digit Imaging
,
33
,
1167
1184
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10278-020-00387-1
Raymond
,
J. E.
,
Shapiro
,
K. L.
, &
Arnell
,
K. M.
(
1992
).
Temporary suppression of visual processing in an RSVP task: An attentional blink
?
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform
,
18
,
849
860
. https://doi.org/10.1037//0096-1523.18.3.849
Rensink
,
R. A.
(
2003
).
Change detection
.
Annu Rev Psychol
,
53
(
1
),
245
277
. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135125
Ridding
,
M. C.
, &
Ziemann
,
U.
(
2010
).
Determinants of the induction of cortical plasticity by non-invasive brain stimulation in healthy subjects: Induction of cortical plasticity by non-invasive brain stimulation
.
J Physiol
,
588
,
2291
2304
. https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.2010.190314
Robertson
,
I. H.
,
Manly
,
T.
,
Andrade
,
J.
,
Baddeley
,
B. T.
, &
Yiend
,
J.
(
1997
).
“Oops!”: Performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured and normal subjects
.
Neuropsychologia
,
35
,
747
758
. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0028-3932(97)00015-8
Rogala
,
J.
,
Jurewicz
,
K.
,
Paluch
,
K.
,
Kublik
,
E.
,
Cetnarski
,
R.
, &
Wróbel
,
A.
(
2016
).
The do’s and don’ts of neurofeedback training: A review of the controlled studies using healthy adults
.
Front Hum Neurosci
,
10
,
301
. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00301
Ros
,
T.
,
Enriquez-Geppert
,
S.
,
Zotev
,
V.
,
Young
,
K. D.
,
Wood
,
G.
,
Whitfield-Gabrieli
,
S.
,
Wan
,
F.
,
Vuilleumier
,
P.
,
Vialatte
,
F.
,
Van De Ville
,
D.
,
Todder
,
D.
,
Surmeli
,
T.
,
Sulzer
,
J. S.
,
Strehl
,
U.
,
Sterman
,
M. B.
,
Steiner
,
N. J.
,
Sorger
,
B.
,
Soekadar
,
S. R.
,
Sitaram
,
R.
,…
Thibault
,
R. T.
(
2020
).
Consensus on the reporting and experimental design of clinical and cognitive-behavioural neurofeedback studies (CRED-nf checklist)
.
Brain
,
143
,
1674
1685
. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awaa009
Ros
,
T.
,
Moseley
,
M. J.
,
Bloom
,
P. A.
,
Benjamin
,
L.
,
Parkinson
,
L. A.
, &
Gruzelier
,
J. H.
(
2009
).
Optimizing microsurgical skills with EEG neurofeedback
.
BMC Neurosci
,
10
,
87
. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2202-10-87
Rosenberg
,
M.
,
Noonan
,
S.
,
DeGutis
,
J.
, &
Esterman
,
M.
(
2013
).
Sustaining visual attention in the face of distraction: A novel gradual-onset continuous performance task
.
Atten Percept Psychophys
,
75
,
426
439
. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-012-0413-x
Salari
,
N.
,
Büchel
,
C.
, &
Rose
,
M.
(
2014
).
Neurofeedback training of gamma band oscillations improves perceptual processing
.
Exp Brain Res
,
232
,
3353
3361
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-014-4023-9
Santamaría-Vázquez
,
E.
,
Martínez-Cagigal
,
V.
,
Pérez-Velasco
,
S.
,
Marcos-Martínez
,
D.
, &
Hornero
,
R.
(
2022
).
Robust asynchronous control of ERP-based brain-computer interfaces using deep learning
.
Comput Methods Programs Biomed
,
215
,
106623
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cmpb.2022.106623
Schabus
,
M.
,
Griessenberger
,
H.
,
Gnjezda
,
M.-T.
,
Heib
,
D. P. J.
,
Wislowska
,
M.
, &
Hoedlmoser
,
K.
(
2017
).
Better than sham? A double-blind placebo-controlled neurofeedback study in primary insomnia
.
Brain
,
140
,
1041
1052
. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awx011
Schönenberg
,
M.
,
Wiedemann
,
E.
,
Schneidt
,
A.
,
Scheeff
,
J.
,
Logemann
,
A.
,
Keune
,
P. M.
, &
Hautzinger
,
M.
(
2017
).
Neurofeedback, sham neurofeedback, and cognitive-behavioural group therapy in adults with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A triple-blind, randomised, controlled trial
.
Lancet Psychiatry
,
4
,
673
684
. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(17)30291-2
Schooler
,
J. W.
,
Reichle
,
E. D.
, &
Halpern
,
D. V.
(
2004
).
Zoning out while reading: Evidence for dissociations between experience and metaconsciousness
. In
Levin
D. T.
(Ed.),
Thinking and seeing: Visual metacognition in adults and children
(pp.
203
226
).
MIT Press
. https://psycnet.apa.org/fulltext/2004-18818-010.pdf
Scimeca
,
J. M.
, &
Franconeri
,
S. L.
(
2015
).
Selecting and tracking multiple objects
.
Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci
,
6
,
109
118
. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1328
Sherwood
,
M. S.
,
Parker
,
J. G.
,
Diller
,
E. E.
,
Ganapathy
,
S.
,
Bennett
,
K. B.
,
Esquivel
,
C. R.
, &
Nelson
,
J. T.
(
2019
).
Self-directed down-regulation of auditory cortex activity mediated by real-time fMRI neurofeedback augments attentional processes, resting cerebral perfusion, and auditory activation
.
Neuroimage
,
195
,
475
489
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.03.078
Shokri
,
A.
, &
Nosratabadi
,
M.
(
2021
).
Comparison of biofeedback and combined interventions on athlete’s performance
.
Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback
,
46
,
227
234
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10484-020-09498-5
Simon
,
J. R.
, &
Rudell
,
A. P.
(
1967
).
Auditory S-R compatibility: The effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing
.
J Appl Psychol
,
51
,
300
304
. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0020586
Sitaram
,
R.
,
Ros
,
T.
,
Stoeckel
,
L.
,
Haller
,
S.
,
Scharnowski
,
F.
,
Lewis-Peacock
,
J.
,
Weiskopf
,
N.
,
Blefari
,
M. L.
,
Rana
,
M.
,
Oblak
,
E.
,
Birbaumer
,
N.
, &
Sulzer
,
J.
(
2017
).
Closed-loop brain training: The science of neurofeedback
.
Nat Rev Neurosci
,
18
,
86
100
. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2016.164
Smallwood
,
J.
, &
Schooler
,
J. W.
(
2015
).
The science of mind wandering: Empirically navigating the stream of consciousness
.
Annu Rev Psychol
,
66
,
487
518
. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015331
Studer
,
P.
,
Kratz
,
O.
,
Gevensleben
,
H.
,
Rothenberger
,
A.
,
Moll
,
G. H.
,
Hautzinger
,
M.
, &
Heinrich
,
H.
(
2014
).
Slow cortical potential and theta/beta neurofeedback training in adults: Effects on attentional processes and motor system excitability
.
Front Hum Neurosci
,
8
,
555
. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00555
Summerfield
,
C.
, &
Egner
,
T.
(
2009
).
Expectation (and attention) in visual cognition
.
Trends Cogn Sci
,
13
,
403
409
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.06.003
Thibault
,
R. T.
,
Lifshitz
,
M.
, &
Raz
,
A.
(
2017
).
Neurofeedback or neuroplacebo
?
Brain
,
140
(
4
),
862
864
. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awx033
Tombaugh
,
T. N.
(
2004
).
Trail making test A and B: Normative data stratified by age and education
.
Arch Clin Neuropsychol
,
19
,
203
214
. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0887-6177(03)00039-8
Tombaugh
,
T. N.
(
2006
).
A comprehensive review of the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT)
.
Arch Clin Neuropsychol
,
21
,
53
76
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acn.2005.07.006
Turner
,
M. L.
, &
Engle
,
R. W.
(
1989
).
Is working memory capacity task dependent
?
J Mem Lang
,
28
,
127
154
. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(89)90040-5
Van Doren
,
J.
,
Arns
,
M.
,
Heinrich
,
H.
,
Vollebregt
,
M. A.
,
Strehl
,
U.
, &
Loo
,
S. K.
(
2019
).
Sustained effects of neurofeedback in ADHD: A systematic review and meta-analysis
.
Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry
,
28
,
293
305
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-018-1121-4
Vandierendonck
,
A.
,
Liefooghe
,
B.
, &
Verbruggen
,
F.
(
2010
).
Task switching: Interplay of reconfiguration and interference control
.
Psychol Bull
,
136
,
601
626
. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019791
Vernon
,
D.
,
Egner
,
T.
,
Cooper
,
N.
,
Compton
,
T.
,
Neilands
,
C.
,
Sheri
,
A.
, &
Gruzelier
,
J.
(
2003
).
The effect of training distinct neurofeedback protocols on aspects of cognitive performance
.
Int J Psychophysiol
,
47
,
75
85
. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0167-8760(02)00091-0
Viechtbauer
,
W.
(
2010
).
Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package
.
J Stat Softw
,
36
,
1
48
. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v036.i03
Viviani
,
G.
, &
Vallesi
,
A.
(
2021
).
EEG-neurofeedback and executive function enhancement in healthy adults: A systematic review
.
Psychophysiology
,
58
,
e13874
. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13874
Wang
,
J.-R.
, &
Hsieh
,
S.
(
2013
).
Neurofeedback training improves attention and working memory performance
.
Clin Neurophysiol
,
124
,
2406
2420
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2013.05.020
Wang
,
T.
,
Peeters
,
R.
,
Mantini
,
D.
, &
Gillebert
,
C. R.
(
2020
).
Modulating the interhemispheric activity balance in the intraparietal sulcus using real-time fMRI neurofeedback: Development and proof-of-concept
.
Neuroimage Clin
,
28
,
102513
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2020.102513
Welford
,
A. T.
(
1952
).
An apparatus for use in studying serial performance
.
Am J Psychol
,
65
,
91
97
. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/14903214/
Wickens
,
C. D.
(
2002
).
Situation awareness and workload in aviation
.
Curr Dir Psychol Sci
,
11
,
128
133
. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.00184
Wolfe
,
J. M.
(
2020
).
Visual search: How do we find what we are looking for
?
Annu Rev Vis Sci
,
6
,
539
562
. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-vision-091718-015048
Wyrwicka
,
W.
, &
Sterman
,
M. B.
(
1968
).
Instrumental conditioning of sensorimotor cortex EEG spindles in the waking cat
.
Physiol Behav
,
3
,
703
707
. https://doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(68)90139-X
Xia
,
M.
,
Xu
,
P.
,
Yang
,
Y.
,
Jiang
,
W.
,
Wang
,
Z.
,
Gu
,
X.
,
Yang
,
M.
,
Li
,
D.
,
Li
,
S.
,
Dong
,
G.
,
Wang
,
L.
, &
Wang
,
D.
(
2021
).
Frontoparietal connectivity neurofeedback training for promotion of working memory: An fNIRS study in healthy male participants
.
IEEE Access
,
9
,
62316
62331
. https://doi.org/10.1109/ACCESS.2021.3074220
Yang
,
Y.
,
Sánchez-Tójar
,
A.
,
O’Dea
,
R. E.
,
Noble
,
D. W. A.
,
Koricheva
,
J.
,
Jennions
,
M. D.
,
Parker
,
T. H.
,
Lagisz
,
M.
, &
Nakagawa
,
S.
(
2023
).
Publication bias impacts on effect size, statistical power, and magnitude (Type M) and sign (Type S) errors in ecology and evolutionary biology
.
BMC Biol
,
21
,
71
. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12915-022-01485-y
Yoo
,
S.-S.
, &
Jolesz
,
F. A.
(
2002
).
Functional MRI for neurofeedback: Feasibility study on a hand motor task
.
Neuroreport
,
13
,
1377
1381
. https://doi.org/10.1097/00001756-200208070-00005
Zhang
,
Y.
,
Xie
,
J.
,
Xu
,
G.
,
Fang
,
P.
,
Du
,
G.
,
Zhang
,
X.
,
Li
,
M.
,
Zhang
,
S.
,
Tao
,
T.
, &
Yuan
,
H.
(
2019
).
Performance evaluation of a “switch-to-target” based asynchronous SSVEP BCI paradigm
. In:
2019 16th International Conference on Ubiquitous Robots (UR)
(pp.
32
37
).
IEEE
. https://doi.org/10.1109/URAI.2019.8768660
Zhou
,
S.
, &
Shen
,
C.
(
2022
).
Avoiding definitive conclusions in meta-analysis of heterogeneous studies with small sample sizes
.
JAMA Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg
,
148
,
1003
1004
. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamaoto.2022.2847

Author notes

*

Equally contributed co-first authors

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International (CC BY 4.0) license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. For a full description of the license, please visit https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/legalcode.

Supplementary data