Neuromodulation therapies, such as repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS), have shown promise as treatments for tobacco use disorder (TUD). However, the underlying mechanisms of these therapies remain unclear, which may hamper optimization and personalization efforts. In this study, we investigated alteration of brain entropy as a potential mechanism underlying the neural effects of noninvasive brain stimulation by rTMS in people with TUD. We employed sample entropy (SampEn) to quantify the complexity and predictability of brain activity measured using resting-state fMRI data. Our study design included a randomized single-blind study with 42 participants who underwent 2 data collection sessions. During each session, participants received high-frequency (10 Hz) stimulation to the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) or a control region (visual cortex), and resting-state fMRI scans were acquired before and after rTMS. Our findings revealed that individuals who smoke exhibited higher baseline SampEn throughout the brain as compared to previously-published SampEn measurements in control participants. Furthermore, high-frequency rTMS to the dlPFC but not the control region reduced SampEn in the insula and dlPFC, regions implicated in TUD, and also reduced self-reported cigarette craving. These results suggest that brain entropy may serve as a potential biomarker for effects of rTMS, and provide insight into the neural mechanisms underlying rTMS effects on smoking cessation. Our study contributes to the growing understanding of brain-based interventions for TUD by highlighting the relevance of brain entropy in characterizing neural activity patterns associated with smoking. The observed reductions in entropy following dlPFC-targeted rTMS suggest a potential mechanism for the therapeutic effects of this intervention. These findings support the use of neuroimaging techniques to investigate the use of neuromodulation therapies for TUD.

Brain-based neuromodulation therapies, such as repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS), are emerging as a new class of treatments for substance use disorders. A notable milestone has been regulatory (FDA) approval of rTMS to treat tobacco use disorder (TUD). Approval was granted on the basis of a multicenter, double-blind, randomized controlled trial finding higher smoking cessation rates in individuals who received active versus sham rTMS (Zangen et al., 2021). This important finding capitalizes on many previous studies demonstrating that excitatory rTMS to the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) increases smoking abstinence rates relative to sham (Dinur-Klein et al., 2014; X. Li et al., 2020), lowers the rates of relapse to smoking (Sheffer et al., 2018), reduces cigarette craving (X. Li et al., 2013; Pripfl et al., 2014), and reduces the number of cigarettes smoked (Abdelrahman et al., 2021; Amiaz et al., 2009; Huang et al., 2016; X. Li et al., 2020; Prikryl et al., 2014). This body of work has led to clinical recommendations advocating for the use of rTMS as a smoking cessation treatment (Young et al., 2021), and suggests that this promising new brain-based therapeutic can be informed by advances in neuroimaging that shed light on the neural circuitry alterations associated with TUD.

Noninvasive neuromodulation treatments have been designed to capitalize on findings linking the insula to smoking cessation by attempting to stimulate the insula. The neural basis of TUD has been strongly linked to the insula via lesion studies, finding that lesions to the insula per se (Naqvi et al., 2007) and a broader network involving the insula (Joutsa et al., 2022) are associated with higher rates of smoking cessation compared to lesions involving other brain regions. Structural MRI studies also link the insula and other associated brain regions to TUD. People who smoke have smaller gray matter volumes in areas of the prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate (Brody et al., 2004). More cigarette exposure is associated with thinner insular cortex (Morales et al., 2014), and in women, thinner insular cortex is associated with more cigarette craving (Perez Diaz et al., 2021). Both meta-analysis (Hill-Bowen et al., 2022) and mega-analysis (Mackey et al., 2019) have reported smaller amounts of gray matter in both the medial prefrontal cortex and insula in individuals with varying substance use disorders, with a smoking-specific effect (i.e., not found in individuals with other kinds of substance use disorders) of smaller volumes in the posterior cingulate cortex (Hill-Bowen et al., 2022).

Resting-state functional connectivity studies have found that both the insula specifically, and also large-scale network dynamics involving the insula, are implicated in both acute and chronic nicotine use. People who smoke have lower overall functional connectivity in the brain (Cheng et al., 2019), which has also been shown specifically within the executive control and default mode networks (Cole et al., 2010; Sutherland et al., 2012; Weiland et al., 2015). Default mode network connectivity has also been shown to exhibit increased activity and connectivity in response to smoking cues (Claus et al., 2013; Janes et al., 2015), and increases in reported withdrawal as connectivity increases (Huang et al., 2014). Functional connectivity features can be used in machine learning to distinguish between people who do and do not smoke (Wetherill et al., 2019). A triple-network model describing the relationship between the salience, default mode, and executive control networks (Fedota & Stein, 2015) suggests that the relationship between these three networks responds dynamically to nicotine use: smoking increased coupling between the left executive control network and salience network, and decreased the anticorrelation between the default mode network and salience network (Lerman et al., 2014).

Resting-state functional connectivity analyses have also specifically linked the insula to cigarette craving, withdrawal, and relapse. The magnitude of withdrawal correlates positively with the strength of connectivity between the right ventral anterior insula and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (Ghahremani et al., 2021), both hubs of the midcingulo-insular network (also referred to as the salience, cingulo-opercular, or ventral attention network (Uddin et al., 2019)). Stronger connectivity between the insula and cortex surrounding the central gyrus is associated with better cessation outcomes (less relapse) (Addicott et al., 2015); similarly, stronger connectivity between the ventral striatum and a network including the insula is associated with better cessation outcomes (Sweitzer et al., 2016).

The insula is anatomically located underneath the cortical surface, rendering it inaccessible to conventional rTMS devices, but a small proof-of-concept study (Moeller et al., 2022) and electric field modeling work have suggested that deep rTMS machines such as the BrainsWay® H4 coil can penetrate deeply enough to reach the insula (Fiocchi et al., 2018). However, network connectivity may provide an alternate route to stimulate the insula and other brain regions involved in TUD (X. Li et al., 2017) using conventional rTMS devices.

Though stimulating left dlPFC shows promise for smoking cessation treatments, the exact mechanism that it works through remains unclear. Although rTMS undoubtedly produces salutary behavioral effects, and in some cases produces widespread changes in functional connectivity that can extend outside the stimulated network (Beynel et al., 2020), rTMS to most brain regions does not appear to change blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) signal at the stimulation site (Rafiei & Rahnev, 2022). Therefore, the mechanism by which rTMS causes changes in the stimulated region and other associated regions to yield changes in behavior remains ambiguous. Developing, optimizing, and personalizing these techniques may be improved by a more comprehensive understanding of normal brain function, the brain dysfunction associated with substance use, and the brain function that underlies responses to neuromodulation. Measuring brain entropy is an emerging approach that offers the potential to extend existing knowledge of brain features associated with substance use disorders.

Brain entropy quantifies the complexity and unpredictability of brain activity—as opposed to measures such as Pearson correlations, which measure the association between two brain regions, or standard deviation, which assesses variability. Sample entropy (SampEn) is an approach developed in the context of information theory that has recently been applied to understand the structure of neural time series data. By measuring the similarity between two components (subsequences) of a time series, SampEn quantifies regularities and irregularities, and thereby provides information about the complexity and predictability of the time series signal. A validation study has shown that SampEn can be accurately determined for fMRI data on both simulated and actual datasets (Z. Wang et al., 2014). Higher SampEn values reflect time series that are more complex and therefore less predictable, and conversely, lower SampEn values reflect time series that are less complex and more predictable. SampEn is less sensitive to noise and abbreviated data sets than other forms of entropy (e.g., Shannon entropy and Approximate Entropy) as well as showing consistency and independence from data length (Richman & Moorman, 2000), rendering it a good candidate for analysis of fMRI time series data (Richman & Moorman, 2000; Z. Wang et al., 2014; Yentes et al., 2013).

Previous work has suggested that SampEn is both altered by rTMS (Song, Chang, Zhang, Peng, et al., 2019) and is also different in neuropsychiatric populations compared with healthy individuals, although the direction of the effect depends on the population studied. Individuals with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) have lower frontal and occipital entropy compared with controls, and symptom severity correlates negatively with entropy levels (Sokunbi et al., 2013). Similarly, lower SampEn measurements have been observed in patients with Alzheimer’s disease (B. Wang et al., 2017). Notably, machine-learning classifiers that use SampEn to distinguish patients from controls outperform those relying on standard correlation-based measurements (Wu et al., 2021). Likewise, SampEn has been shown to correlate with fractional amplitude of low-frequency fluctuation (fALFF) measurements (Song, Chang, Zhang, Ge, et al., 2019; Zhang et al., 2021), network coherence frequency ranges (D. J. J. Wang et al., 2018), and power spectrum measures (Bruce et al., 2009) showing that changes in SampEn capture a diverse set of neural mechanisms, which is a desired feature in a biomarker.

In contrast to the relatively lower SampEn observed in individuals with ADHD and Alzheimer’s disease, higher SampEn has been observed in people who use nicotine, marijuana, and alcohol (Jiang et al., 2023), replicating and extending an earlier finding showing higher entropy in people who smoke cigarettes (Z. Li et al., 2016). A small pilot study suggested that noninvasive neuromodulation using repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) can reduce both entropy and cigarette craving in these individuals (Chang et al., 2018). Demonstrating that rTMS can influence entropy in people who smoke could fill this gap in knowledge about rTMS mechanisms, so we sought to test the hypotheses that (1) high-frequency rTMS to the dlPFC would reduce SampEn in the dlPFC and insular cortex, and (2) greater reductions in SampEn in these regions would correspond with greater reductions in smoking craving.

2.1 Participants

To test the above hypotheses, data were collected from 42 participants. All participants were recruited from the greater Los Angeles community using online ads via Craigslist. Study procedures were approved by the UCLA IRB and written, informed consent was obtained from all participants for being included in the study. Initial eligibility assessments were made by telephone, and participants who met criteria according to their self-report were scheduled for further in-person eligibility screening, which included baseline neuroimaging measurements (see below).

To be included, participants were required to be right-handed, between the ages of 18 to 45, smoking on average 5 or more cigarettes per day, and not seeking or receiving treatment for smoking cessation. Exclusion of those below the age of 18 was due to the low prevalence of daily smoking and those above 45 due to the average range of perimenopause onset being between 46 and 50 years of age. Restrictions for seeking or receiving treatment were made for reasons of beneficence (i.e., because the study did not include a standard-of-care treatment arm). For those seeking treatment, we referred them to treatment centers/programs so that no assumption of treatment was implied by this study and for those receiving treatment the effects of said treatment would confound the results seen for stimulation. The relatively low number of cigarettes per day threshold was set so that the sample would include individuals with a range of smoking characteristics. Participants were excluded from participation if they were left-handed, met criteria for any other substance use disorder, met criteria for other psychiatric conditions as assessed by the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview version 7.0.2 (Sheehan et al., 1998); tested positive for other substances of abuse by urinalysis or breathalyzer; if they reported or tested positive for pregnancy; or if they were determined to have safety contraindications for rTMS or MRI, including non-removable metal implants or any factor that could lower the seizure threshold. Table 1 shows the demographic characteristics of the participants included in this study.

Table 1.

Study project demographics with number of participants included, N, the mean age of the participants in years, the number of males and females, and the average duration of abstinence prior to their test session for each stimulation site.

Demographic table
45 
Years of age (mean ± SEM) 33 ±
Years of smoking (mean ± SD) 15.2 ± 1.24 
Sex Males: 33 (73.3%) 
Females: 12 (26.7%) 
Hours of abstinence dlPFC: 16 ±
v5: 16 ± 0.95 
Demographic table
45 
Years of age (mean ± SEM) 33 ±
Years of smoking (mean ± SD) 15.2 ± 1.24 
Sex Males: 33 (73.3%) 
Females: 12 (26.7%) 
Hours of abstinence dlPFC: 16 ±
v5: 16 ± 0.95 

2.2 Study design

Participants who remained eligible after in-person assessments were scheduled for data collection sessions. Each session was identical except for the region stimulated (left dlPFC or visual cortex [v5]). The order of each session type was randomized and counterbalanced, and participants were instructed to remain abstinent from smoking for >12 hours before their data collection sessions. Sessions were scheduled at least 24 hours apart to ensure adequate washout of the treatment. Participants were blinded as to which region investigators intended to be active versus control targets, although they would have been able to sense the location that the magnet was positioned in.

Upon arriving in the laboratory for each data collection session, urine samples were collected to confirm abstinence from illicit substances, a breathalyzer was administered to confirm abstinence from alcohol, and vital signs were obtained. Expired carbon monoxide was measured to confirm >12 hours abstinence from cigarette smoking.

To assess baseline withdrawal and craving, participants completed the Shiffman-Jarvik Withdrawal Questionnaire (Shiffman & Jarvik, 1976) and Urge to Smoke scales (Jarvik et al., 2000) via self-report. Baseline resting-state functional images (see below for sequence details) were collected. On the first stimulation day only, the participant’s active motor threshold was measured and recorded. On each stimulation day, neuronavigation was used to position the rTMS coil. rTMS was delivered (see details below), followed immediately, approximately 5-15 minutes, by a post-rTMS resting-state neuroimaging scan, then post-rTMS self-report of craving and withdrawal measurements. Figure 1 shows a CONSORT diagram of the study.

Fig. 1.

CONSORT Diagram. Our study design was a randomized one-way blind study. Eligible participants were randomized to either dlPFC first and v5 second, or vice versa. Participant data were not excluded if they only completed one session. Nine participants were lost to follow-up between the two stimulation sessions. Three sessions of data were excluded due to corrupted data or normalization errors.

Fig. 1.

CONSORT Diagram. Our study design was a randomized one-way blind study. Eligible participants were randomized to either dlPFC first and v5 second, or vice versa. Participant data were not excluded if they only completed one session. Nine participants were lost to follow-up between the two stimulation sessions. Three sessions of data were excluded due to corrupted data or normalization errors.

Close modal

2.3 Behavioral data collection

Participants were required to abstain from smoking for >12 hours before each testing day began to produce a state of acute withdrawal. Withdrawal is associated with a range of subjective experiences, including a heightened sense of craving as well as other somatic and affective symptoms. Although often linked together, withdrawal and craving are separate but related components of TUD (Baker et al., 2012; Shiffman et al., 2004). To access both variables, we used two questionnaires: Shiffman-Jarvik Withdrawal Questionnaire (SJWS, Shiffman & Jarvik, 1976) and Urge to Smoke scales (Jarvik et al., 2000). Psychometric properties of the UTS have not been published but we judged the scale to have high face validity, and noted that it has been widely used in other neuroimaging studies (e.g., Brody et al., 2007; Faulkner et al., 2019; Galván et al., 2011). The SJWS assesses multiple domains of withdrawal, including a sub-scale specific to craving, and has been subjected to factor analysis to determine its subscales, and found to have high overall reliability (Patten & Martin, 1996). For this study, we examined the SJWS overall score and the craving subscale scores in participants to capture both aspects in our participants. The craving sub-scale has two scores reported, the average and total scores, calculated from the questions specific to cigarette craving in the SJWS.

2.4 Brain imaging data collection & preprocessing

Whole-brain structural and functional MR imaging was conducted on a 3 Tesla Siemens Prisma Fit MRI scanner with a 32-channel head coil at the UCLA Staglin Center for Cognitive Neuroscience. A single T1-weighted structural scan (TE = 2.24 ms; TR = 2400 ms; voxel resolution = 0.8 x 0.8 x 0.8 mm) was collected during the intake session as well as an 8 minute baseline T2*-weighted multi-band sequence resting-state functional scan. Resting-state functional scans (TE = 37 ms; TR = 800 ms; FoV = 208 mm; Slice Thickness = 2 mm; Number of Slices = 72, voxel resolution = 2 x 2 x 2 mm) were performed twice on test days (pre- and post-rTMS). Prior to all resting-state functional scans, two spin echo fieldmaps were collected in opposite directions (AP and PA).

FMRI data processing was carried out using FEAT (FMRI Expert Analysis Tool) Version 6.00, part of FSL (FMRIB's Software Library, www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl). The following pre-statistics processing was applied; motion correction using MCFLIRT(Jenkinson et al., 2002); B0 unwarping using boundary-based registration via FUGUE (Jenkinson, 2003, 2004); slice-timing correction using Fourier-space time-series phase-shifting; non-brain removal using BET (Smith, 2002); spatial smoothing using a Gaussian kernel of FWHM 4.0 mm; grand-mean intensity normalization of the entire 4D dataset by a single multiplicative factor; and high-pass temporal filtering (Gaussian-weighted least-squares straight line fitting, with sigma = 50.0 seconds). ICA-based exploratory data analysis was carried out using MELODIC (Beckmann & Smith, 2004), in order to investigate the possible presence of unexpected artifacts or activation. ICA-FIX was trained on a set of 20 scans that were hand-classified into noise and non-noise components, with the scans randomly selected from 5 bins sorting scans by the amount of average motion present to have high and low motion data in the trained set. The component classification derived from the trained data was then used in ICA-FIX to classify noise and non-noise components from all subject data and non-aggressively remove the noise components. After denoising, ICA-FIX applied a high-pass filter to each subject’s data. Registration to high-resolution structural and/or standard space images was carried out using FLIRT (Jenkinson & Smith, 2001; Jenkinson et al., 2002). Registration from high-resolution structural to standard space was then further refined using FNIRT nonlinear registration (Andersson et al., 2007a, 2007b). Lastly, average time series were extracted from each subject’s data based on brain nodes specified by the atlas, then detrended for cubic trends, and finally z-score normalized.

2.5 Brain atlas, dlPFC, and insula

For this study, we used the brain parcellation proposed by Van De Ville et al. (2021) to extract time series from all imaging data. Briefly, this parcellation includes a Schaefer 400 brain region cortical parcellation ((Schaefer et al., 2018), https://github.com/ThomasYeoLab/CBIG/tree/master/stable_projects/brain_parcellation/Schaefer2018_LocalGlobal) combined with 16 subcortical regions and 3 cerebellar regions from the HCP release for a total of 419 nodes. This study focused on changes in dlPFC and insula; therefore, to determine which nodes correlated to those regions, we used the Harvard-Oxford probability atlas and the dlPFC ROI mask obtained from Neurovault to determine which nodes were primarily in these regions. Insula was determined to overlay with nodes 35, 98 to 100, and 143 in the left hemisphere and nodes 234-236, 302-305, and 340 in the right hemisphere. Left dlPFC was determined to overlay with nodes 137 to 142.

2.6 Neuromodulation

2.6.1 rTMS

TMS sessions were conducted using the Magstim Super Rapid2 Plus1 (MagStim, UK, https://www.magstim.com/row-en/) system equipped with a figure-8 coil. We stimulated two regions, dlPFC and v5, during separate sessions as shown in Figure 2. Stimulation to dlPFC was considered the active treatment region, while v5 served as a control region. Both stimulation sessions used the same stimulation sequence of 10 Hz stimulation for 60 trains, each train lasting 5 seconds and followed by 10 seconds of no stimulation, for a total of 3000 pulses over approximately 15 minutes at 100% motor threshold.

Fig. 2.

Stimulation Targets. (A) The left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) was used as the target region for this project. (B) Counter to dlPFC, the left visual cortex (v5) was used as a control region.

Fig. 2.

Stimulation Targets. (A) The left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) was used as the target region for this project. (B) Counter to dlPFC, the left visual cortex (v5) was used as a control region.

Close modal

2.6.2 Neuronavigation

Neuronavigation was conducted to personalize target engagement using the ANT Neuro visor2 system (ANT Neuro, NL, https://www.ant-neuro.com/products/visor2). Target coordinates for neuronavigation were determined using the participant’s resting-state fMRI. The centroid of all individualized dlPFC targets was [MNI -46, 23, 33] and that of individualized v5 targets was [MNI -44, -78, 4]. Individualized targets were selected using the below procedure, but importantly, we do not make any claims in this manuscript as to whether this targeting produces different outcomes compared to stimulating at the centroid or elsewhere within the ROI. Using FSL’s MELODIC, scans would split into 20 independent components. These components would then be overlaid with one of two masks to extract the time series and look for which component had the max activity for each mask ROI. The component with the max activity for a particular node would then be considered the network component for the network associated with that particular node. For this study we used the inferior frontal gyrus to determine the executive network and the visual cortex to determine the visual network. Once the peak component for each network was determined, we used the dlPFC and v5 masks shown in Figure 2 to determine the peak active voxel in each ROI for their respective networks. This peak voxel was designated as the participant’s neuronavigation target for stimulation.

2.7 Analyses

2.7.1 Brain entropy calculations & analysis

Information entropy is the measure of randomness or uncertainty of a series of data without knowledge of the data series origin. One way to calculate this information entropy is called Sample Entropy. Given a data series, for example a time series extracted from a voxel or region of the brain, the process of Sample Entropy first divides the time series up into smaller vectors of length m. Next, for each of the vectors it calculates the distance between the two vectors, with the requirement that they are not the same vector, i.e. i ≠ j. If both values are less than the distance threshold (r), also can be called a noise filter as it determines the possibility of the pair, then the pairing is counted as a possible. The sum of all possible vector comparisons creates B(r), which is the probability that two sequences are similar for m points. This process is then repeated for vectors of m+1 size to determine the number of matches and sum those together and create A(r), which is the probability that two sequences are similar for m+1 points. Taking the ratio of the number of matches to the number of possibles, we find how much of the signal is uncertain/random. We then take the negative log of this ratio since information measurements are made on the logarithmic scale. The mathematical representation of this process is:

For this study, sample entropy of each node was calculated using the Brain Entropy Mapping Toolbox (BENtbx, Z. Wang et al., 2014). For the parameters, we set m = 3 and r = 0.3 based on a previous study examining the effects of these parameters on sample entropy (Yentes et al., 2013). Extracted mean node time series were organized into matrices with dimension timepoints by nodes; for the study, this would generate a 588 x 419 matrix for 419 nodes each having 588 timepoints. The BENtbx would then take these matrices and calculate the entropy per node per participant.

3.1 Brain entropy prior to rTMS

We examined the distribution of average SampEn across the brain at baseline before examining changes in SampEn due to brain stimulation. Using baseline (pre-rTMS) images, values for each node were collected from each participant and then averaged to determine the average resting SampEn for people who smoke and are in withdrawal. Likewise, all node values were averaged for each participant to determine their brain’s average SampEn. We then took the brain averages and compared them to each node's values to determine if a region was significantly above or below the global average.

Considering both (1) the node average SampEn and (2) node average SampEn relative to brain average SampEn, we observed that gyral nodes had lower SampEn than the sulci nodes and the subcortical regions. Figure 3A shows the contrast between these areas of the brain. We also found that the majority of the outer cortical regions and the cerebellum were significantly below the global average (pFDR < 0.05), and that the subcortical and orbital frontal regions were significantly above the cortex (pFDR < 0.05). These observations and results complement and support previous findings by (Z. Wang et al., 2014). Figure 3B shows the regions found to be above (red) and below (blue) the average brain SampEn in people who smoke. Contrary to Wang et al. (2014), who found that in healthy adults the range of SampEn values across the brain spanned from 0.44 to 0.608, in this study, we found that SampEn ranges across the brain spanning from 1 to 1.75. Although we did not directly compare people who do not smoke, this finding is broadly consistent with previous studies explicitly demonstrating that people who smoke have a “hyper-resting brain entropy” state (Z. Li et al., 2016).

Fig. 3.

Average Sample Entropy Maps. (A) Average sample entropy per region across the brain. Regions are defined by the Schaefer 400 parcellation and 16 sub-cortical regions & 3 cerebellum regions from the Human Connectome Project. Red indicates the highest levels of SampEn observed, and purple indicates the lowest. (B) Regions with SampEn above (red) and below (blue) the average SampEn across the brain for people who smoke pre-rTMS.

Fig. 3.

Average Sample Entropy Maps. (A) Average sample entropy per region across the brain. Regions are defined by the Schaefer 400 parcellation and 16 sub-cortical regions & 3 cerebellum regions from the Human Connectome Project. Red indicates the highest levels of SampEn observed, and purple indicates the lowest. (B) Regions with SampEn above (red) and below (blue) the average SampEn across the brain for people who smoke pre-rTMS.

Close modal

3.2 Changes in craving

Participant self-reported craving measurements were collected before and after stimulation to determine if rTMS has an immediate effect on an individual’s cigarette craving. Self-reported measures were compared using a paired-samples t-test and corrected for multiple comparisons using the Bonferroni correction. Craving as measured by the Shiffman-Jarvik Withdrawal Scale craving subscale was found to be significantly different for stimulation to left dlPFC (Pre: 22 (8.25); Post: 20.45 (6.7), t(df) = 2.36(37), p = 0.005, Cohen’s d = 0.31). No significant differences were found for the Urge to Smoke or withdrawal as measured by the SJWS for stimulation to left dlPFC. No craving or withdrawal measures were found to be significantly different for stimulation to left v5. Figure 4 & Table 2 show the results for the SJWS-Craving scores for both sessions. Results for the Urge to Smoke are in the Supplementary Materials.

Fig. 4.

Stimulation to left dlPFC reduced craving in participants. (Left) Change score for Shiffman-Jarvik Withdrawal Craving measure from pre-rTMS minus post-rTMS values, resulting in larger positive values corresponding to larger reductions in craving. (Right) Violin individual point distribution plot, showing each participant’s individual change from pre- to post-rTMS.

Fig. 4.

Stimulation to left dlPFC reduced craving in participants. (Left) Change score for Shiffman-Jarvik Withdrawal Craving measure from pre-rTMS minus post-rTMS values, resulting in larger positive values corresponding to larger reductions in craving. (Right) Violin individual point distribution plot, showing each participant’s individual change from pre- to post-rTMS.

Close modal
Table 2.

Pre- and Post-rTMS to both targets showing both their total craving scores and average craving scores before and after treatment.

Shiffman-Jarvik withdrawal scale measurements, Mean (SD)
dlPFCv5
Total craving Pre-rTMS 22 (8.25) 21.79 (7.74) 
 Post-rTMS 20.45 (6.7) 21.07 (7.14) 
Average craving Pre-rTMS 4.57 (1.7) 4.44 (1.57) 
 Post-rTMS 4.22 (1.4) 4.26 (1.43) 
Shiffman-Jarvik withdrawal scale measurements, Mean (SD)
dlPFCv5
Total craving Pre-rTMS 22 (8.25) 21.79 (7.74) 
 Post-rTMS 20.45 (6.7) 21.07 (7.14) 
Average craving Pre-rTMS 4.57 (1.7) 4.44 (1.57) 
 Post-rTMS 4.22 (1.4) 4.26 (1.43) 

3.3 Brain entropy changes in left dlPFC and insula

Extracted time series using the atlas previously described were normalized and entered into the BENtbx to calculate each node's sample entropy. Node sample entropy was compared for pre- and post-rTMS for all 419 regions using a paired t-test. All results were corrected for multiple comparisons using the False-Discovery Rate method.

Examining the 19 nodes that include bilateral insula and left dlPFC, we found that 17 out of 19 nodes had significant changes in their SampEn measurements from pre- to post-rTMS to left dlPFC. All nodes showed lower SampEn in post-rTMS scans compared to pre-rTMS. Table 3 shows the results for each node. No insula or dlPFC nodes were found to change significantly after stimulation to v5. Figures 5 & 6 show the mean SampEn measurements before and after stimulation in the nodes with the most significant change for each stimulation site, and a t-statistic map for those nodes. Figures for all other nodes in these regions can be found in the Supplementary Materials.

Table 3.

Pre vs. post T-stat, FDR-corrected p-value, and Cohen’s d for all a priori nodes.

Sample entropy statistical results
Schaefer Atlas NodeT-stat (df = 41)pFDRCohen’s d
35 (L Insula) -9.71 5.06E-11 1.82 
98 (L Insula) -4.76 4.92E-05 0.73 
99 (L Insula) -9.30 1.47E-10 1.74 
100 (L Insula) -9.84 3.92E-11 1.84 
143 (L Insula) -11.51 1.07E-12 3.02 
234 (R Insula) -10.2 1.42E-11 1.89 
235 (R Insula) -10.72 4.49E-12 1.83 
236 (R Insula) -10.39 9.45E-12 1.66 
302 (R Insula) -6.39 4.26E-07 1.17 
303 (R Insula) -5.02 2.32E-05 0.97 
304 (R Insula) -7.28 3.63E-08 1.28 
305 (R Insula) -8.03 4.60E-09 1.63 
340 (R Insula) -6.69 1.86E-07 1.22 
137 (L dlPFC) -2.34 0.03 0.41 
138 (L dlPFC) -3.06 0.01 0.54 
139 (L dlPFC) -1.84 0.09 0.44 
140 (L dlPFC) -2.2 0.04 0.37 
141 (L dlPFC) -5.85 2.16E-06 1.02 
142 (L dlPFC) -9.71 5.06E-11 1.82 
Sample entropy statistical results
Schaefer Atlas NodeT-stat (df = 41)pFDRCohen’s d
35 (L Insula) -9.71 5.06E-11 1.82 
98 (L Insula) -4.76 4.92E-05 0.73 
99 (L Insula) -9.30 1.47E-10 1.74 
100 (L Insula) -9.84 3.92E-11 1.84 
143 (L Insula) -11.51 1.07E-12 3.02 
234 (R Insula) -10.2 1.42E-11 1.89 
235 (R Insula) -10.72 4.49E-12 1.83 
236 (R Insula) -10.39 9.45E-12 1.66 
302 (R Insula) -6.39 4.26E-07 1.17 
303 (R Insula) -5.02 2.32E-05 0.97 
304 (R Insula) -7.28 3.63E-08 1.28 
305 (R Insula) -8.03 4.60E-09 1.63 
340 (R Insula) -6.69 1.86E-07 1.22 
137 (L dlPFC) -2.34 0.03 0.41 
138 (L dlPFC) -3.06 0.01 0.54 
139 (L dlPFC) -1.84 0.09 0.44 
140 (L dlPFC) -2.2 0.04 0.37 
141 (L dlPFC) -5.85 2.16E-06 1.02 
142 (L dlPFC) -9.71 5.06E-11 1.82 
Fig. 5.

Reductions in entropy in a priori selected ROIs. Regions in blue (insula and left dlPFC) were selected a priori as nodes that were expected to show reductions in SampEn as a result of rTMS. The t-statistic value for node is shown in blue, with darker values indicating a t-statistic closer to zero, and lighter blues showing increasingly more negative t-statistics as a result of stimulation to left dlPFC, indicating greater rTMS-induced decreases in SampEn.

Fig. 5.

Reductions in entropy in a priori selected ROIs. Regions in blue (insula and left dlPFC) were selected a priori as nodes that were expected to show reductions in SampEn as a result of rTMS. The t-statistic value for node is shown in blue, with darker values indicating a t-statistic closer to zero, and lighter blues showing increasingly more negative t-statistics as a result of stimulation to left dlPFC, indicating greater rTMS-induced decreases in SampEn.

Close modal
Fig. 6.

Stimulation to left dlPFC reduced sample entropy in L/R Insula and L dlPFC nodes. Plots show change group in sample entropy (left) and individual changes/distribution (right) for each node that had the lowest p-value and the region that correlated with the node. Change values were calculated by subtracting post-rTMS entropy values from pre-rTMS values.

Fig. 6.

Stimulation to left dlPFC reduced sample entropy in L/R Insula and L dlPFC nodes. Plots show change group in sample entropy (left) and individual changes/distribution (right) for each node that had the lowest p-value and the region that correlated with the node. Change values were calculated by subtracting post-rTMS entropy values from pre-rTMS values.

Close modal

To determine if changes in SampEn influenced the observed changes in an individual’s craving, we correlated each participant’s change in SampEn measurements with their change in craving scores and restricted correlation values to be above r = 0.2. No correlations were found for any of the a priori nodes.

3.4 Potential confounding variables

To determine whether other participant characteristics may have influenced SampEn analyses, five variables (sex, ethnicity, age, years of smoking, and education) were examined for relationships with SampEn. Pre-rTMS SampEn and change in SampEn were compared between male and female participants using an independent-samples t-test. Pre-rTMS SampEn was compared between all 8 ethnicity categories using a one-way ANOVA. For sex differences, nodes 99, 100, 303, and 304 were found to have differences between male and female participants with females having higher entropy in all nodes. These differences did not survive multiple comparison corrections, but warrant exploration in future studies. No significant differences were found between ethnicity groups. Supplementary Table S4 showing the average SampEn per node for each ethnic group can be found in the Supplementary Materials. Pearson correlations were calculated for Age/Years of Smoking/Education level versus Pre-rTMS SampEn to determine if any of the variables influenced the entropy measurement in our participant population. No significant correlations (p > 0.05) were found between age, years of smoking or education level, and Pre-rTMS SampEn in left dlPFC, left or right Insula. A table of non-significant correlation values with each variable and the corresponding p-values can be found in the Supplementary Materials.

3.5 Exploratory findings

After the above a priori ROI results were obtained and determined to have no correlation with observed changes in craving, we decided to examine other nodes for significant changes and correlation with behavior. We observed that for left dlPFC stimulation, entropy changed significantly across the majority of the brain. Figure 7 shows a t-statistic map, thresholded at pFDR < 0.05, showing the t-statistic associated with the comparison of each region’s SampEn before and after stimulation. Three nodes (133, 314, and 318) were found to have significant changes in SampEn (Cohen’s d = 0.56,1.06,0.58; pFDR = 0.0016,2·104,6.1·106, respectively) and have a moderate correlation with craving. In node 133, which overlaps with the inferior temporal gyrus (ITG), changes in SampEn correlated with the changes in SJWS-Craving (r(40) = 0.36). Nodes 314 and 318, both in the right superior frontal gyrus (SFG), had moderate correlations between their changes in SampEn and changes in UTS (r(34) = 0.39 and r(34) = 0.43, respectively). All tables and figures for these results are in the Supplementary Materials.

Fig. 7.

Brainwide changes in SampEn as a result of rTMS to dlPFC show widespread reductions in entropy. Changes in SampEn from pre- to post-dlPFC stimulation are shown here as t-statistics; thresholded at pFDR < 0.05. Red indicates increased entropy following rTMS, gray indicates no change, and increasingly darker blues indicate increasingly greater reductions in entropy as a result of dlPFC stimulation. These reductions in entropy can be observed throughout the brain, with only a few small regions of increased entropy.

Fig. 7.

Brainwide changes in SampEn as a result of rTMS to dlPFC show widespread reductions in entropy. Changes in SampEn from pre- to post-dlPFC stimulation are shown here as t-statistics; thresholded at pFDR < 0.05. Red indicates increased entropy following rTMS, gray indicates no change, and increasingly darker blues indicate increasingly greater reductions in entropy as a result of dlPFC stimulation. These reductions in entropy can be observed throughout the brain, with only a few small regions of increased entropy.

Close modal

We also looked at the effect of stimulation on network SampEn using the network designation of the Schaefer atlas. For this we took the SampEn values from all nodes in each network and compared them as a group for pre- and post-stimulation to calculate the effect size using Cohen’s d. We found that stimulation to the left dlPFC affected the SampEn in all networks except for subcortical, but stimulation to v5 had only small to no effect on network entropy. Stimulation to dlPFC showed large effects in visual, somatomotor, and limbic networks (Cohen’s d = 0.8, 1.08, and 0.96 respectively); medium effects in dorsal attention and salience networks (Cohen’s d = 0.69 for both); and small effects in executive and default mode networks (Cohen’s d = 0.35 and 0.37 respectively). Figure 8 shows the difference in effects of each stimulation and the direction of those effects. No correlations were found between changes in network SampEn and craving/withdrawal.

Fig. 8.

Cohen’s d showing SampEn network effects of stimulation showing large effects of left dlPFC stimulation. Effects of stimulation to both sites are shown as Cohen’s d values, with positive values indicating reductions in SampEn and negative values indicating increases in SampEn. Left dlPFC stimulation (blue) had large effects on visual, somatomotor, and limbic networks; medium effects on dorsal attention and salience networks; and small effects on executive and default mode networks. Left v5 stimulation (red) had only a small effect on visual and somatic motor networks.

Fig. 8.

Cohen’s d showing SampEn network effects of stimulation showing large effects of left dlPFC stimulation. Effects of stimulation to both sites are shown as Cohen’s d values, with positive values indicating reductions in SampEn and negative values indicating increases in SampEn. Left dlPFC stimulation (blue) had large effects on visual, somatomotor, and limbic networks; medium effects on dorsal attention and salience networks; and small effects on executive and default mode networks. Left v5 stimulation (red) had only a small effect on visual and somatic motor networks.

Close modal

4.1 dlPFC & insula

In this study, we used excitatory rTMS to the left dlPFC to reduce cigarette craving and sample entropy in the bilateral insula and left dlPFC. Although no correlation was found between the magnitude of SampEn changes in these regions and the magnitude of craving changes, these data suggest that one mechanism by which neuromodulation produces craving relief may include reducing regional brain entropy. This is strengthened by our exploratory results showing that changes in regional SampEn for SFG and ITG did have moderate correlations with craving changes.

This investigation builds on ample previous work strongly relating left dlPFC stimulation to reductions in cigarette craving, consumption, and ultimately, cessation. Although the evidence base for this treatment is mounting, the mechanism by which dlPFC stimulation produces its effects are not well-understood. One small (N = 10) investigation showed that dlPFC stimulation reduces fractional amplitude of low-frequency fluctuations in the insula, and also reduced connectivity between the stimulation site and medial prefrontal cortex (X. Li et al., 2017), suggesting that modulation of insula activity by dlPFC stimulation may be the mechanism by which rTMS alleviates craving.

Our finding that dlPFC stimulation reduces entropy in the insula is consistent with previous evidence suggesting that rTMS to the dlPFC produces its salutary effects. However, based on the current findings, the insula’s link to the observed rTMS-induced reductions in craving still remains to be seen. One explanation for this may be that single-session stimulation of left dlPFC does not induce enough of a reduction in withdrawal and craving for a correlation to be observed, and successive stimulation sessions may be required to increase the effect of stimulation to left dlPFC on craving and withdrawal so that behavioral changes are more apparent. Alternatively, the link between insula entropy and craving may be reflected in entropy changes in other regions that are functionally connected to the insula, like the SFG. Because the magnitude of craving reduction corresponds to the magnitude of entropy reduction in the SFG, even though the difference between pre- and post-Urge to Smoke scores was minimal and inconsistent in participants, these findings suggest that rTMS to the dlPFC may be a viable target, but not the most effective. Direct stimulation to SFG may prove to be more effective.

Both the SFG and ITG have been shown to be positively connected to the addiction remission network (Joutsa et al., 2022). The superior frontal gyrus has been linked to smoking through a previous brain stimulation study. Rose et al., 2011 showed in a small study of 15 participants that excitatory stimulation to SFG resulted in immediate reductions in self-reported craving and reductions in craving due to neutral cues. Two previous studies showed that in people who smoke, SFG demonstrated higher levels of spontaneous activity (Niu et al., 2023) and lower resting functional connectivity (Zhou et al., 2017) relative to controls. These findings could be broadly consistent with our observations of entropy reductions, as rTMS may reduce entropy in SFG, thus causing the region to stabilize its activity and thereby reduce craving.

4.2 Limitations

This investigation delivered only single-session rTMS, and therefore conclusions about long-term effects cannot be drawn. Notably, however, in a pivotal multi-center trial of rTMS for smoking cessation, acute (single-session) reductions in craving did predict successful smoking cessation (Zangen et al., 2021). Additionally, our control condition in this investigation involved stimulation to a different brain site that was delivered to our test population of people with TUD. The lack of a control group (people who do not smoke) prevents any conclusions about entropy in people with and without TUD from being drawn from this data; however, previous work has performed this comparison and found higher brain entropy in people who smoke compared to controls (Z. Li et al., 2016). The size of our sample, the age range of 18 to 45, and the difference in the number of males versus females limit our ability to generalize these results and thus this study should be repeated in a larger sample with less of a difference in the number of each sex included in the sample.

Brain entropy was calculated in this study using fMRI collected data, which has a lower temporal resolution than other methods of neuroimaging, such as electroencephalography. This restriction on temporal resolution potentially limits the results determined here and should be validated using a neuroimaging method with higher resolution so that more accurate measures of entropy can be determined due to finer time scales. Likewise, although extensive denoising of data was carried out, residual noise could remain in the data and therefore influence the results.

4.3 Conclusion

In this study, we were able to replicate previous findings that rTMS can reduce sample entropy in the brain, and extended these findings in people who smoke, showing that the effect of rTMS on sample entropy is consistent in a population other than healthy controls. We also replicated previous observations about the distribution of brain entropy across the brain and observed evidence of potentially increased resting entropy in people who smoke. Although changes to insula and left DLPFC SampEn did not correlate with changes in behavior, we did find that post-TMS reductions in entropy in two other regions, the SFG and ITG, correlated with rTMS-induced reductions in craving. This result provides additional (although indirect) evidence that entropy is higher in people who smoke than people who do not, and suggests that by reducing entropy in specific regions associated with smoking, we can reduce cigarette craving. This work shows that sample entropy may be a potential biomarker for measuring efficacy of rTMS-based smoking cessation treatments.

4.4 Future directions

Future studies should examine this effect in larger populations using more substantial doses of rTMS. Moreover, future investigations may test the effect of using baseline entropy in regions associated with smoking, specifically insula and SFG, to adjust individual treatments. These studies should also seek to measure entropy at time points further out from completion of stimulation protocols such as days and weeks later to determine delayed/long-term effects on entropy. Next, we will also need to explore the functional connectivity changes in these participants to see if the regions found with significant changes in SampEn also have changes in functional connectivity and compare them separately and together as predictors of behavior changes. Expanding upon this work, further investigations into brain complexity should be examined outside of just regional complexity. These should include measures of complexity of functional connections using functional entropy (Yao et al., 2013), entropy states and directional influences of entropy (Varley et al., 2023), and community mapping entropy (Betzel et al., 2019). By developing our understanding of how these measures of entropy change due to TMS, entropy can be better applied as a biomarker for treatments.

The data and code that support the results of this study are available on Github (https://github.com/humanbrainzappingatucla). Any additional information required to reanalyze the data used in this paper is available upon request and use agreement with the corresponding author

Conceptualization, T.J. and N.P.; Methodology, T.J. and J.N.; Software, T.J.; Formal Analysis, T.J.; Investigation, M.R.A., T.J., and N.P.; Data Curation, T.J.; Writing—Original Draft, T.J. and N.P.; Writing—Review & Editing, T.J. M.R.A. J.N., and N.P.; Visualization, T.J.; Supervision, T.J. and N.P.; Project Administration, M.R.A. and N.P.; Funding Acquisition, N.P.

This study was supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health (NIDA, R00DA045749 to N.P.) and the Friends of Semel Scholars (N.P.).

The authors declare no competing interests.

We thank the staff of the Center for Cognitive Neuroscience for providing aid and support for all fMRI imaging sessions. We thank the UCLA TMS Clinical and Research Services for providing technical and medical support during all stimulation sessions. We are grateful to Lucina Uddin for helpful discussions and feedback. We thank Anthony Sun, Melanie Beltran, and Riley Russell for assisting the investigators.

Supplemental figures and tables can be found here: https://github.com/HumanBrainZappingatUCLA/Entropy. Supplementary material for this article is available with the online version here: https://doi.org/10.1162/imag_a_00061.

Abdelrahman
,
A. A.
,
Noaman
,
M.
,
Fawzy
,
M.
,
Moheb
,
A.
,
Karim
,
A. A.
, &
Khedr
,
E. M.
(
2021
).
A double-blind randomized clinical trial of high frequency rTMS over the DLPFC on nicotine dependence, anxiety and depression
.
Scientific Reports
,
11
(
1
),
1640
. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-80927-5
Addicott
,
M. A.
,
Sweitzer
,
M. M.
,
Froeliger
,
B.
,
Rose
,
J. E.
, &
McClernon
,
F. J.
(
2015
).
Increased functional connectivity in an insula-based network is associated with improved smoking cessation outcomes
.
Neuropsychopharmacology
,
40
(
11
),
Article 11
. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2015.114
Amiaz
,
R.
,
Levy
,
D.
,
Vainiger
,
D.
,
Grunhaus
,
L.
, &
Zangen
,
A.
(
2009
).
Repeated high-frequency transcranial magnetic stimulation over the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex reduces cigarette craving and consumption
.
Addiction
,
104
(
4
),
653
660
. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2008.02448.x
Andersson
,
J. L.
,
Jenkinson
,
M.
, &
Smith
,
S.
(
2007a
).
Non-linear optimisation. FMRIB technical report TR07JA1
.
Practice
. https://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/datasets/techrep/
Andersson
,
J. L.
,
Jenkinson
,
M.
, &
Smith
,
S.
(
2007b
).
Non-linear registration, aka Spatial normalisation FMRIB technical report TR07JA2
.
FMRIB Analysis Group of the University of Oxford
,
2
(
1
),
e21
. https://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/datasets/techrep/
Baker
,
T. B.
,
Breslau
,
N.
,
Covey
,
L.
, &
Shiffman
,
S.
(
2012
).
DSM criteria for tobacco use disorder and tobacco withdrawal: A critique and proposed revisions for DSM-5*
.
Addiction
,
107
(
2
),
263
275
. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2011.03657.x
Beckmann
,
C. F.
, &
Smith
,
S. M.
(
2004
).
Probabilistic independent component analysis for functional magnetic resonance imaging
.
IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging
,
23
(
2
),
137
152
. https://doi.org/10.1109/tmi.2003.822821
Betzel
,
R. F.
,
Bertolero
,
M. A.
,
Gordon
,
E. M.
,
Gratton
,
C.
,
Dosenbach
,
N. U. F.
, &
Bassett
,
D. S.
(
2019
).
The community structure of functional brain networks exhibits scale-specific patterns of inter- and intra-subject variability
.
NeuroImage
,
202
,
115990
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.07.003
Beynel
,
L.
,
Powers
,
J. P.
, &
Appelbaum
,
L. G.
(
2020
).
Effects of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation on resting-state connectivity: A systematic review
.
NeuroImage
,
211
,
116596
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116596
Brody
,
A. L.
,
Mandelkern
,
M. A.
,
Olmstead
,
R. E.
,
Jou
,
J.
,
Tiongson
,
E.
,
Allen
,
V.
,
Scheibal
,
D.
,
London
,
E. D.
,
Monterosso
,
J. R.
,
Tiffany
,
S. T.
,
Korb
,
A.
,
Gan
,
J. J.
, &
Cohen
,
M. S.
(
2007
).
Neural substrates of resisting craving during cigarette cue exposure
.
Biological Psychiatry
,
62
(
6
),
642
651
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.10.026
Brody
,
A. L.
,
Olmstead
,
R. E.
,
London
,
E. D.
,
Farahi
,
J.
,
Meyer
,
J. H.
,
Grossman
,
P.
,
Lee
,
G. S.
,
Huang
,
J.
,
Hahn
,
E. L.
, &
Mandelkern
,
M. A.
(
2004
).
Smoking-induced ventral striatum dopamine release
.
American Journal of Psychiatry
,
161
(
7
),
1211
1218
. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.161.7.1211
Bruce
,
E. N.
,
Bruce
,
M. C.
, &
Vennelaganti
,
S.
(
2009
).
Sample entropy tracks changes in EEG power spectrum with sleep state and aging
.
Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology : Official Publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society
,
26
(
4
),
257
266
. https://doi.org/10.1097/wnp.0b013e3181b2f1e3
Chang
,
D.
,
Zhang
,
J.
,
Peng
,
W.
,
Shen
,
Z.
,
Gao
,
X.
,
Du
,
Y.
,
Ge
,
Q.
,
Song
,
D.
,
Shang
,
Y.
, &
Wang
,
Z.
(
2018
).
Smoking cessation with 20 Hz repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) applied to two brain regions: A pilot study
.
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience
,
12
,
344
. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2018.00344
Cheng
,
W.
,
Rolls
,
E. T.
,
Robbins
,
T. W.
,
Gong
,
W.
,
Liu
,
Z.
,
Lv
,
W.
,
Du
,
J.
,
Wen
,
H.
,
Ma
,
L.
, &
Quinlan
,
E. B.
(
2019
).
Decreased brain connectivity in smoking contrasts with increased connectivity in drinking
.
Elife
,
8
,
e40765
. https://doi.org/10.7554/elife.40765
Claus
,
E. D.
,
Blaine
,
S. K.
,
Filbey
,
F. M.
,
Mayer
,
A. R.
, &
Hutchison
,
K. E.
(
2013
).
Association between nicotine dependence severity, BOLD response to smoking cues, and functional connectivity
.
Neuropsychopharmacology
,
38
(
12
),
Article 12
. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2013.134
Cole
,
D. M.
,
Beckmann
,
C. F.
,
Long
,
C. J.
,
Matthews
,
P. M.
,
Durcan
,
M. J.
, &
Beaver
,
J. D.
(
2010
).
Nicotine replacement in abstinent smokers improves cognitive withdrawal symptoms with modulation of resting brain network dynamics
.
NeuroImage
,
52
(
2
),
590
599
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.251
Dinur-Klein
,
L.
,
Dannon
,
P.
,
Hadar
,
A.
,
Rosenberg
,
O.
,
Roth
,
Y.
,
Kotler
,
M.
, &
Zangen
,
A.
(
2014
).
Smoking cessation induced by deep repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the prefrontal and insular cortices: A prospective, randomized controlled trial
.
Biological Psychiatry
,
76
(
9
),
742
749
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.05.020
Faulkner
,
P.
,
Ghahremani
,
D. G.
,
Tyndale
,
R. F.
,
Paterson
,
N. E.
,
Cox
,
C.
,
Ginder
,
N.
,
Hellemann
,
G.
, &
London
,
E. D.
(
2019
).
Neural basis of smoking-induced relief of craving and negative affect: Contribution of nicotine
.
Addiction Biology
,
24
(
5
),
1087
1095
. https://doi.org/10.1111/adb.12679
Fedota
,
J. R.
, &
Stein
,
E. A.
(
2015
).
Resting-state functional connectivity and nicotine addiction: Prospects for biomarker development
.
Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences
,
1349
(
1
),
64
82
. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.12882
Fiocchi
,
S.
,
Chiaramello
,
E.
,
Luzi
,
L.
,
Ferrulli
,
A.
,
Bonato
,
M.
,
Roth
,
Y.
,
Zangen
,
A.
,
Ravazzani
,
P.
, &
Parazzini
,
M.
(
2018
).
Deep transcranial magnetic stimulation for the addiction treatment: Electric field distribution modeling
.
IEEE Journal of Electromagnetics, RF and Microwaves in Medicine and Biology
,
2
(
4
),
242
248
. https://doi.org/10.1109/jerm.2018.2874528
Galván
,
A.
,
Poldrack
,
R. A.
,
Baker
,
C. M.
,
McGlennen
,
K. M.
, &
London
,
E. D.
(
2011
).
Neural correlates of response inhibition and cigarette smoking in late adolescence
.
Neuropsychopharmacology
,
36
(
5
),
Article 5
. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2010.235
Ghahremani
,
D. G.
,
Pochon
,
J.-B.
,
Perez Diaz
,
M.
,
Tyndale
,
R. F.
,
Dean
,
A. C.
, &
London
,
E. D.
(
2021
).
Functional connectivity of the anterior insula during withdrawal from cigarette smoking
.
Neuropsychopharmacology
,
46
(
12
),
2083
2089
. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-021-01036-z
Hill-Bowen
,
L. D.
,
Riedel
,
M. C.
,
Salo
,
T.
,
Flannery
,
J. S.
,
Poudel
,
R.
,
Laird
,
A. R.
, &
Sutherland
,
M. T.
(
2022
).
Convergent gray matter alterations across drugs of abuse and network-level implications: A meta-analysis of structural MRI studies
.
Drug and Alcohol Dependence
,
240
,
109625
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2022.109625
Huang
,
W.
,
Fang
,
S.
,
Zhang
,
J.
, &
Baoping
,
X.
(
2016
).
Effect of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation on cigarette smoking in patients with schizophrenia
.
Shanghai Archives of Psychiatry
,
28
(
6
),
309
. https://doi.org/10.11919/j.issn.1002-0829.216044
Huang
,
W.
,
King
,
J. A.
,
Ursprung
,
W. W. S.
,
Zheng
,
S.
,
Zhang
,
N.
,
Kennedy
,
D. N.
,
Ziedonis
,
D.
, &
DiFranza
,
J. R.
(
2014
).
The development and expression of physical nicotine dependence corresponds to structural and functional alterations in the anterior cingulate-precuneus pathway
.
Brain and Behavior
,
4
(
3
),
408
417
. https://doi.org/10.1002/brb3.227
Janes
,
A. C.
,
Farmer
,
S.
,
Peechatka
,
A. L.
,
Frederick
,
B. de B.
, &
Lukas
,
S. E.
(
2015
).
Insula–dorsal anterior cingulate cortex coupling is associated with enhanced brain reactivity to smoking cues
.
Neuropsychopharmacology
,
40
(
7
),
Article 7
. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2015.9
Jarvik
,
M. E.
,
Madsen
,
D. C.
,
Olmstead
,
R. E.
,
Iwamoto-Schaap
,
P. N.
,
Elins
,
J. L.
, &
Benowitz
,
N. L.
(
2000
).
Nicotine blood levels and subjective craving for cigarettes
.
Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior
,
66
(
3
),
553
558
. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0091-3057(00)00261-6
Jenkinson
,
M.
(
2003
).
Fast, automated, N-dimensional phase-unwrapping algorithm
.
Magnetic Resonance in Medicine
,
49
(
1
),
193
197
.https://doi.org/10.1002/mrm.10354
Jenkinson
,
M.
(
2004
).
Improving the registration of B0-disorted EPI images using calculated cost function weights
. In
Tenth International Conference on Functional Mapping of the Human Brain
.
Jenkinson
,
M.
,
Bannister
,
P.
,
Brady
,
M.
, &
Smith
,
S.
(
2002
).
Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images
.
NeuroImage
,
17
(
2
),
825
841
. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.1132
Jenkinson
,
M.
, &
Smith
,
S.
(
2001
).
A global optimisation method for robust affine registration of brain images
.
Medical Image Analysis
,
5
(
2
),
143
156
. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1361-8415(01)00036-6
Joutsa
,
J.
,
Moussawi
,
K.
,
Siddiqi
,
S. H.
,
Abdolahi
,
A.
,
Drew
,
W.
,
Cohen
,
A. L.
,
Ross
,
T. J.
,
Deshpande
,
H. U.
,
Wang
,
H. Z.
,
Bruss
,
J.
,
Stein
,
E. A.
,
Volkow
,
N. D.
,
Grafman
,
J. H.
,
van Wijngaarden
,
E.
,
Boes
,
A. D.
, &
Fox
,
M. D.
(
2022
).
Brain lesions disrupting addiction map to a common human brain circuit
.
Nature Medicine
,
28
(
6
),
1249
1255
,
Article 6
. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41591-022-01834-y
Lerman
,
C.
,
Gu
,
H.
,
Loughead
,
J.
,
Ruparel
,
K.
,
Yang
,
Y.
, &
Stein
,
E. A.
(
2014
).
Large-scale brain network coupling predicts acute nicotine abstinence effects on craving and cognitive function
.
JAMA Psychiatry
,
71
(
5
),
523
530
. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.4091
Li
,
X.
,
Du
,
L.
,
Sahlem
,
G. L.
,
Badran
,
B. W.
,
Henderson
,
S.
, &
George
,
M. S.
(
2017
).
Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex reduces resting-state insula activity and modulates functional connectivity of the orbitofrontal cortex in cigarette smokers
.
Drug and Alcohol Dependence
,
174
,
98
105
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2017.02.002
Li
,
X.
,
Hartwell
,
K. J.
,
Henderson
,
S.
,
Badran
,
B. W.
,
Brady
,
K. T.
, &
George
,
M. S.
(
2020
).
Two weeks of image-guided left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation improves smoking cessation: A double-blind, sham-controlled, randomized clinical trial
.
Brain Stimulation
,
13
(
5
),
1271
1279
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brs.2020.06.007
Li
,
X.
,
Hartwell
,
K. J.
,
Owens
,
M.
,
LeMatty
,
T.
,
Borckardt
,
J. J.
,
Hanlon
,
C. A.
,
Brady
,
K. T.
, &
George
,
M. S.
(
2013
).
Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex reduces nicotine cue craving
.
Biological Psychiatry
,
73
(
8
),
714
720
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.01.003
Li
,
Z.
,
Fang
,
Z.
,
Hager
,
N.
,
Rao
,
H.
, &
Wang
,
Z.
(
2016
).
Hyper-resting brain entropy within chronic smokers and its moderation by Sex
.
Scientific Reports
,
6
(
1
),
Article 1
. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep29435
Mackey
,
S.
,
Allgaier
,
N.
,
Chaarani
,
B.
,
Spechler
,
P.
,
Orr
,
C.
,
Bunn
,
J.
,
Allen
,
N. B.
,
Alia-Klein
,
N.
,
Batalla
,
A.
, &
Blaine
,
S.
(
2019
).
Mega-analysis of gray matter volume in substance dependence: General and substance-specific regional effects
.
American Journal of Psychiatry
,
176
(
2
),
119
128
. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2018.17040415
Moeller
,
S. J.
,
Gil
,
R.
,
Weinstein
,
J. J.
,
Baumvoll
,
T.
,
Wengler
,
K.
,
Fallon
,
N.
,
Van Snellenberg
,
J. X.
,
Abeykoon
,
S.
,
Perlman
,
G.
,
Williams
,
J.
,
Manu
,
L.
,
Slifstein
,
M.
,
Cassidy
,
C. M.
,
Martinez
,
D. M.
, &
Abi-Dargham
,
A.
(
2022
).
Deep rTMS of the insula and prefrontal cortex in smokers with schizophrenia: Proof-of-concept study
.
Schizophrenia (Heidelberg, Germany)
,
8
(
1
),
6
. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41537-022-00224-0
Morales
,
A. M.
,
Ghahremani
,
D.
,
Kohno
,
M.
,
Hellemann
,
G. S.
, &
London
,
E. D.
(
2014
).
Cigarette exposure, dependence, and craving are related to insula thickness in young adult smokers
.
Neuropsychopharmacology
,
39
(
8
),
1816
1822
. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2014.48
Naqvi
,
N. H.
,
Rudrauf
,
D.
,
Damasio
,
H.
, &
Bechara
,
A.
(
2007
).
Damage to the insula disrupts addiction to cigarette smoking
.
Science
,
315
(
5811
),
531
534
. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1135926
Niu
,
X.
,
Gao
,
X.
,
Lv
,
Q.
,
Zhang
,
M.
,
Dang
,
J.
,
Sun
,
J.
,
Wang
,
W.
,
Wei
,
Y.
,
Cheng
,
J.
,
Han
,
S.
, &
Zhang
,
Y.
(
2023
).
Increased spontaneous activity of the superior frontal gyrus with reduced functional connectivity to visual attention areas and cerebellum in male smokers
.
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience
,
17
,
1153976
. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2023.1153976
Patten
,
C. A.
, &
Martin
,
J. E.
(
1996
).
Measuring tobacco withdrawal: A review of self-report questionnaires
.
Journal of Substance Abuse
,
8
(
1
),
93
113
. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0899-3289(96)90115-7
Perez Diaz
,
M.
,
Pochon
,
J.-B.
,
Ghahremani
,
D. G.
,
Dean
,
A. C.
,
Faulkner
,
P.
,
Petersen
,
N.
,
Tyndale
,
R. F.
,
Donis
,
A.
,
Paez
,
D.
, &
Cahuantzi
,
C.
(
2021
).
Sex differences in the association of cigarette craving with insula structure
.
International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology
,
24
(
8
),
624
633
. https://doi.org/10.1093/ijnp/pyab015
Prikryl
,
R.
,
Ustohal
,
L.
,
Kucerova
,
H. P.
,
Kasparek
,
T.
,
Jarkovsky
,
J.
,
Hublova
,
V.
,
Vrzalova
,
M.
, &
Ceskova
,
E.
(
2014
).
Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation reduces cigarette consumption in schizophrenia patients
.
Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry
,
49
,
30
35
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2013.10.019
Pripfl
,
J.
,
Tomova
,
L.
,
Riecansky
,
I.
, &
Lamm
,
C.
(
2014
).
Transcranial magnetic stimulation of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex decreases cue-induced nicotine craving and EEG delta power
.
Brain Stimulation
,
7
(
2
),
226
233
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brs.2013.11.003
Rafiei
,
F.
, &
Rahnev
,
D.
(
2022
).
TMS does not increase BOLD activity at the site of stimulation: A review of all concurrent TMS-fMRI studies
.
eNeuro
,
9
(
4
), ENEURO.0163-22.2022. https://doi.org/10.1523/eneuro.0163-22.2022
Richman
,
J. S.
, &
Moorman
,
J. R.
(
2000
).
Physiological time-series analysis using approximate entropy and sample entropy
.
American Journal of Physiology. Heart and Circulatory Physiology
,
278
(
6
),
H2039
2049
. https://doi.org/10.1152/ajpheart.2000.278.6.h2039
Rose
,
J. E.
,
McClernon
,
F. J.
,
Froeliger
,
B.
,
Behm
,
F. M.
,
Preud’homme
,
X.
, &
Krystal
,
A. D.
(
2011
).
Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the superior frontal gyrus modulates craving for cigarettes
.
Biological Psychiatry
,
70
(
8
),
794
799
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.05.031
Schaefer
,
A.
,
Kong
,
R.
,
Gordon
,
E. M.
,
Laumann
,
T. O.
,
Zuo
,
X.-N.
,
Holmes
,
A. J.
,
Eickhoff
,
S. B.
, &
Yeo
,
B. T. T.
(
2018
).
Local-global parcellation of the human cerebral cortex from intrinsic functional connectivity MRI
.
Cerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y.: 1991)
,
28
(
9
),
3095
3114
. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhx179
Sheehan
,
D. V.
,
Lecrubier
,
Y.
,
Sheehan
,
K. H.
,
Amorim
,
P.
,
Janavs
,
J.
,
Weiller
,
E.
,
Hergueta
,
T.
,
Baker
,
R.
, &
Dunbar
,
G. C.
(
1998
).
The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): The development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10
.
The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry
,
59
Suppl 20
,
22
33
;quiz 34–57.
Sheffer
,
C. E.
,
Bickel
,
W. K.
,
Brandon
,
T. H.
,
Franck
,
C. T.
,
Deen
,
D.
,
Panissidi
,
L.
,
Abdali
,
S. A.
,
Pittman
,
J. C.
,
Lunden
,
S. E.
, &
Prashad
,
N.
(
2018
).
Preventing relapse to smoking with transcranial magnetic stimulation: Feasibility and potential efficacy
.
Drug and Alcohol Dependence
,
182
,
8
18
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2017.09.037
Shiffman
,
S.
,
West
,
R. J.
, &
Gilbert
,
D. G.
(
2004
).
Recommendation for the assessment of tobacco craving and withdrawal in smoking cessation trials
.
Nicotine & Tobacco Research
,
6
(
4
),
599
614
. https://doi.org/10.1080/14622200410001734067
Shiffman
,
S. M.
, &
Jarvik
,
M. E.
(
1976
).
Smoking withdrawal symptoms in two weeks of abstinence
.
Psychopharmacology
,
50
(
1
),
35
39
. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf00634151
Smith
,
S. M.
(
2002
).
Fast robust automated brain extraction
.
Human Brain Mapping
,
17
(
3
),
143
155
. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.10062
Sokunbi
,
M. O.
,
Fung
,
W.
,
Sawlani
,
V.
,
Choppin
,
S.
,
Linden
,
D. E. J.
, &
Thome
,
J.
(
2013
).
Resting state fMRI entropy probes complexity of brain activity in adults with ADHD
.
Psychiatry Research
,
214
(
3
),
341
348
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2013.10.001
Song
,
D.
,
Chang
,
D.
,
Zhang
,
J.
,
Ge
,
Q.
,
Zang
,
Y.-F.
, &
Wang
,
Z.
(
2019
).
Associations of brain entropy (BEN) to cerebral blood flow and fractional amplitude of low-frequency fluctuations in the resting brain
.
Brain Imaging and Behavior
,
13
(
5
),
1486
1495
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-018-9963-4
Song
,
D.
,
Chang
,
D.
,
Zhang
,
J.
,
Peng
,
W.
,
Shang
,
Y.
,
Gao
,
X.
, &
Wang
,
Z.
(
2019
).
Reduced brain entropy by repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation on the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in healthy young adults
.
Brain Imaging and Behavior
,
13
(
2
),
421
429
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-018-9866-4
Sutherland
,
M. T.
,
McHugh
,
M. J.
,
Pariyadath
,
V.
, &
Stein
,
E. A.
(
2012
).
Resting state functional connectivity in addiction: Lessons learned and a road ahead
.
NeuroImage
,
62
(
4
),
2281
2295
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.117
Sweitzer
,
M. M.
,
Geier
,
C. F.
,
Addicott
,
M. A.
,
Denlinger
,
R.
,
Raiff
,
B. R.
,
Dallery
,
J.
,
McClernon
,
F. J.
, &
Donny
,
E. C.
(
2016
).
Smoking abstinence-induced changes in resting state Functional connectivity with ventral striatum predict lapse during a quit attempt
.
Neuropsychopharmacology
,
41
(
10
),
2521
2529
. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2016.56
Uddin
,
L. Q.
,
Yeo
,
B. T.
, &
Spreng
,
R. N.
(
2019
).
Towards a universal taxonomy of macro-scale functional human brain networks
.
Brain Topography
,
32
(
6
),
926
942
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10548-019-00744-6
Van De Ville
,
D.
,
Farouj
,
Y.
,
Preti
,
M. G.
,
Liégeois
,
R.
, &
Amico
,
E.
(
2021
).
When makes you unique: Temporality of the human brain fingerprint
.
Science Advances
,
7
(
42
),
eabj0751
. https://doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.abj0751
Varley
,
T. F.
,
Pope
,
M.
,
Faskowitz
,
J.
, &
Sporns
,
O.
(
2023
).
Multivariate information theory uncovers synergistic subsystems of the human cerebral cortex
.
Communications Biology
,
6
(
1
),
Article 1
. https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-023-04843-w
Wang
,
B.
,
Niu
,
Y.
,
Miao
,
L.
,
Cao
,
R.
,
Yan
,
P.
,
Guo
,
H.
,
Li
,
D.
,
Guo
,
Y.
,
Yan
,
T.
,
Wu
,
J.
,
Xiang
,
J.
, &
Zhang
,
H.
(
2017
).
Decreased complexity in Alzheimer’s disease: Resting-state fMRI evidence of brain entropy mapping
.
Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience
,
9
,
378
. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2017.00378
Wang
,
D. J. J.
,
Jann
,
K.
,
Fan
,
C.
,
Qiao
,
Y.
,
Zang
,
Y.-F.
,
Lu
,
H.
, &
Yang
,
Y.
(
2018
).
Neurophysiological basis of multi-scale entropy of brain complexity and its relationship with functional connectivity
.
Frontiers in Neuroscience
,
12
,
352
. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2018.00352
Wang
,
Z.
,
Li
,
Y.
,
Childress
,
A. R.
, &
Detre
,
J. A.
(
2014
).
Brain entropy mapping using fMRI
.
PLOS ONE
,
9
(
3
),
e89948
. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0089948
Weiland
,
B. J.
,
Sabbineni
,
A.
,
Calhoun
,
V. D.
,
Welsh
,
R. C.
, &
Hutchison
,
K. E.
(
2015
).
Reduced executive and default network functional connectivity in cigarette smokers
.
Human Brain Mapping
,
36
(
3
),
872
882
. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22672
Wetherill
,
R. R.
,
Rao
,
H.
,
Hager
,
N.
,
Wang
,
J.
,
Franklin
,
T. R.
, &
Fan
,
Y.
(
2019
).
Classifying and characterizing nicotine use disorder with high accuracy using machine learning and resting-state fMRI
.
Addiction Biology
,
24
(
4
),
811
821
. https://doi.org/10.1111/adb.12644
Wu
,
Y.
,
Zhou
,
Y.
, &
Song
,
M.
(
2021
).
Classification of patients with AD from healthy controls using entropy-based measures of causality brain networks
.
Journal of Neuroscience Methods
,
361
,
109265
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2021.109265
Yao
,
Y.
,
Lu
,
W. L.
,
Xu
,
B.
,
Li
,
C. B.
,
Lin
,
C. P.
,
Waxman
,
D.
, &
Feng
,
J. F.
(
2013
).
The increase of the functional entropy of the human brain with age
.
Scientific Reports
,
3
(
1
),
Article 1
. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep02853
Yentes
,
J. M.
,
Hunt
,
N.
,
Schmid
,
K. K.
,
Kaipust
,
J. P.
,
McGrath
,
D.
, &
Stergiou
,
N.
(
2013
).
The appropriate use of approximate entropy and sample entropy with short data sets
.
Annals of Biomedical Engineering
,
41
(
2
),
349
365
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10439-012-0668-3
Young
,
J. R.
,
Galla
,
J. T.
, &
Appelbaum
,
L. G.
(
2021
).
Transcranial magnetic stimulation treatment for smoking cessation: An introduction for primary care clinicians
.
The American Journal of Medicine
,
134
(
11
),
1339
1343
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.amjmed.2021.06.037
Zangen
,
A.
,
Moshe
,
H.
,
Martinez
,
D.
,
Barnea-Ygael
,
N.
,
Vapnik
,
T.
,
Bystritsky
,
A.
,
Duffy
,
W.
,
Toder
,
D.
,
Casuto
,
L.
,
Grosz
,
M. L.
,
Nunes
,
E. V.
,
Ward
,
H.
,
Tendler
,
A.
,
Feifel
,
D.
,
Morales
,
O.
,
Roth
,
Y.
,
Iosifescu
,
D. V.
,
Winston
,
J.
,
Wirecki
,
T.
,…
George
,
M. S.
(
2021
).
Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation for smoking cessation: A pivotal multicenter double-blind randomized controlled trial
.
World Psychiatry
,
20
(
3
),
397
404
. https://doi.org/10.1002/wps.20905
Zhang
,
S.
,
Spoletini
,
L. J.
,
Gold
,
B. P.
,
Morgan
,
V. L.
,
Rogers
,
B. P.
, &
Chang
,
C.
(
2021
).
Interindividual signatures of fMRI temporal fluctuations
.
Cerebral Cortex
,
31
(
10
),
4450
4463
. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhab099
Zhou
,
S.
,
Xiao
,
D.
,
Peng
,
P.
,
Wang
,
S.-K.
,
Liu
,
Z.
,
Qin
,
H.-Y.
,
Li
,
S.-S.
, &
Wang
,
C.
(
2017
).
Effect of smoking on resting-state functional connectivity in smokers: An fMRI study
.
Respirology
,
22
(
6
),
1118
1124
. https://doi.org/10.1111/resp.13048
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International (CC BY 4.0) license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. For a full description of the license, please visit https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/legalcode.

Supplementary data