Advances in ultra-high field 7 Tesla functional magnetic resonance imaging (7 T fMRI) have provided unprecedented opportunities to gain insights into the neural underpinnings supporting human memory. The hippocampus, a heterogeneous brain structure comprising several subfields, plays a central role during vivid re-experiencing of autobiographical memories (AM). However, due to technical limitations, how hippocampal subfields differentially support AM, whether this contribution is specific to one portion along the hippocampal long-axis, and how subfields are functionally connected with other brain regions typically associated with AM retrieval remains elusive. Here, we leveraged technical advances of parallel imaging and employed a submillimeter Echo Planar Imaging sequence over the whole brain while participants re-experienced vivid, detail-rich AM. We found that all hippocampal subfields along the long-axis were engaged during AM retrieval. Nonetheless, only the pre/parasubiculum within the anterior body of the hippocampus contributed over and above to AM retrieval. Moreover, whole-brain functional connectivity analyses of the same data revealed that this part of the hippocampus was the only one that was strongly connected to other brain regions typically associated with AM, such as the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) and medial/lateral parietal regions. In the context of the broader literature, our results support recent proposals that the anterior body of the pre/parasubiculum may play an important role in scene-based cognition, such as its engagement during the re-experiencing of personal past events.

Neuropsychological and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies have firmly established the hippocampus as a central structure underpinning vivid autobiographical memory (AM, i.e., memories of personal past events). The hippocampus is a heterogeneous brain structure comprising several subfields, including the dentate gyrus (DG), cornu ammonis (CA) 1-4, subiculum, presubiculum, and parasubiculum (hereafter referred to collectively as the pre/parasubiculum). The hippocampus interacts with a broader set of brain areas that together comprise the AM network. This network includes areas in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) and medial/lateral parietal cortices (Addis et al., 2007; McCormick et al., 2015, 2020; Moscovitch et al., 2005; Rosenbaum et al., 2008; Scoville & Milner, 1957). While we have a broad understanding that the hippocampus works within this network to support AM retrieval, we lack a detailed understanding of how hippocampal subfields interact with cortical areas of the AM network during AM retrieval. To address this gap, we leveraged recent advances in ultra-high field 7-Tesla functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), to: (i) examine the contributions of hippocampal subfields to AM; (ii) assess how this differs along the anterior-posterior axis of hippocampal subfields; and (iii) characterize their associated functional connectivity with the neocortex.

AM retrieval is a complex cognitive process supported by a dynamic interplay between brain areas within the AM network (Conway, 2009; Conway & Pleydell-Pearce, 2000; McCormick et al., 2015; Sekeres et al., 2018). It is widely acknowledged that the hippocampus plays a central role in this network and is consistently and reliably activated during AM retrieval, including at the single-subject level. Several studies have investigated the relationship between hippocampal subfields and AM, albeit with mixed results (Barry et al., 2021; Bartsch et al., 2011; Bonnici et al., 2013; Chadwick et al., 2014; Miller et al., 2020; Palombo et al., 2018). For example, Bonnici et al. (2013), Chadwick et al. (2014), and Miller et al. (2020) reported evidence that the CA3 region may be particularly involved in AM retrieval while Bartsch et al. (2011) speculated a key role of the CA1 region. Furthermore, Palombo et al. (2018) and Barry et al. (2021) found support for a central position of the subiculum and pre/parasubiculum in AM retrieval. However, these mixed observations may result from methodological differences across these studies which used different subfield segmentation protocols (e.g., Bonnici et al. (2013) did not segment the pre/parasubiculum), different imaging modalities/analysis (i.e., structural versus functional MRI), and different measures of AM retrieval success (i.e., interview-based markers versus task-based fMRI, etc). Taking this into consideration, our understanding of how hippocampal subfields preferentially engage during AM retrieval remains limited.

In addition to its subfields, functional differentiation has also been observed along the longitudinal axis of the hippocampus (Poppenk et al., 2013, Strange et al., 2014, Zeidman et al., 2016). Interestingly, an increasing number of 3 T fMRI studies have observed that a specific region in the anterior medial hippocampus is consistently engaged during AM tasks (see Zeidman et al., 2016 for review). While the majority of these studies did not have the spatial resolution to explicitly examine hippocampal subfields, activation patterns consistently align with the medial most portion of the anterior body of the hippocampus, aligning with the location of the pre/parasubiculum and distal subiculum. Indeed, the pre/parasubiculum has recently been proposed as crucial hub for scene-based cognition (Dalton & Maguire, 2017) and subsequent experimental work has provided empirical support that this specific region preferentially engages during scene-based cognition (Dalton et al., 2018; Grande et al., 2022). In addition, previous research has shown that this region of the anterior medial body of the hippocampus (aligning with the location of the pre/parasubiculum) was functionally connected with parts of the AM network, including the vmPFC and medial/lateral parietal cortices during the vivid re-experiencing of autobiographical memories (McCormick et al., 2015). Despite these preliminary insights, we do not know how different portions of hippocampal subfields along their anterior-posterior axis engage during AM retrieval.

As noted above, traditional 3 T fMRI sequences at a whole-brain level and a reasonable repetition time are usually limited to a voxel size of approximately 3 mm isotropic, thus prohibiting accurate functional imaging of small brain structures like hippocampal subfields in a whole-brain setting (Willems & Henke, 2021). Technical advances in high-resolution 3 T fMRI have facilitated an increase in spatial resolution that can be used to capture dissociable activity of small adjacent brain structures of the medial temporal lobes, including hippocampal subfields (Bonnici, Chadwick, Lutti, et al., 2012; Dalton, McCormick, & Maguire, 2019; Dalton, McCormick, De Luca, et al., 2019). However, these sequences require a reduced field-of-view for a reasonable spatiotemporal resolution, thus precluding detailed examination of hippocampal subfield interactions with the broader AM network. Recent innovations in ultra-high field 7 Tesla (7 T) fMRI sequence development including 2D high parallel imaging acceleration capabilities now permit submillimeter voxel sizes at a whole-brain level while keeping temporal resolution high (Stirnberg & Stöcker, 2021; Stirnberg et al., 2017). In the current study, we leveraged these technological advances to conduct a fine-grained examination of functional connectivity between hippocampal subfields along their longitudinal axis and the neocortex during AM retrieval.

To date, a few studies have investigated memory signals using 7 T fMRI (Berron et al., 2016; Grande et al., 2019; Risius et al., 2013). For example, recent reduced field-of-view 7 T fMRI studies examined hippocampal subfield contributions to distinguish between or combine similar experiences (Berron et al., 2016; Grande et al., 2019). Other neuroimaging studies have exploited advances of 7 T fMRI to study targeted layer-specific effects of mnemonic processes (Finn et al., 2019; Maass et al., 2014; Norris & Polimeni, 2019). However, to our knowledge, no study has hitherto leveraged both increased speed and spatial resolution of 7 T fMRI to examine differential hippocampal subfield–neocortical interactions during AM retrieval.

Here, we deployed ultra-high field 7 T fMRI with a dedicated AM retrieval task to achieve two primary goals: (1) to investigate the differential activation of hippocampal subfields along their longitudinal axis during AM retrieval, and (2) to examine hippocampal subfield functional connectivity with neocortical brain regions associated with the AM network.

2.1 Participants

Twenty-four healthy young individuals (right-handed, age: 26.66 ± 4.15 years old, Males: 12, Females: 12) with no history of neurological or psychiatric disorders and normal or corrected-to-normal vision were recruited. All participants completed secondary level of education (at least 12 years of education). All participants provided oral and written informed consent in accordance with the local ethics board.

2.2 Autobiographical memory and visual imagery assessment

In order to examine vivid, detail-rich AM retrieval, we only included participants who reported being able to recall detail-rich personal memories and construct mental images with ease. Each participant was first asked to assess their ability to recall vivid AMs on a Likert scale from 1 to 6 (1 = able to recall detail-rich memories; 6 = unable to recall any personal events). Participants were also asked to assess their ability to construct vivid mental images on a Likert scale from 1 to 6 (1 = able to create detail-rich mental images; 6 = lack of visual imagery). This procedure was adapted from Clark and Maguire (2020) in which the authors demonstrated that these questions effectively captured the ability to engage in AM retrieval.

2.3 Experimental fMRI task

The experimental task was adapted from a previous protocol by McCormick et al. (2015). The experimental procedure was clarified to the participants prior to the scanning. Participants were presented with a set of 40 randomized trials consisting of an AM retrieval task and a simple mental arithmetic (MA) task. As MA task generally does not involve the activation of hippocampus or memory, it was chosen to serve as a baseline. Each trial lasted a maximum of 17 s with a jittered inter-stimulus interval (ISI) between 1 and 4 s. The AM trials consisted of word cues of various general events, for example, birthday celebration. Once the stimulus appeared on the screen, participants were instructed to search covertly for a relevant personal event which was specific in time and place and more than 1 year ago and press a response button once a memory had been chosen without verbally describing it. Participants were then asked to re-experience the memory in their mind by re-living the event with as many perceptual details as possible. In comparison, the MA trials consisted of simple addition or subtraction problems, for example, 13 + 53. After the MA problem was solved, participants were instructed to press a response button and add 3 to the solution iteratively, for example, (13 + 53) + 3 + 3n. Following all AM trials, participants were asked to indicate with a button press whether the AM had been re-experienced in a detailed manner or whether the retrieval was faint. Following all MA trials, participants were asked to indicate with a button press whether the MA problem had been easy or difficult.

2.4 MRI data acquisition

MRI data were acquired using a MAGNETOM 7 T Plus ultra-high field scanner (Siemens Healthineers, Erlangen, Germany). Participants viewed the stimulus screen placed at the back end of the scanner bore through a mirror mounted between the inner 32 channel receiver head coil and the outer circular polarized transmit coil. The MRI protocol consisted of the following scans:

2.4.1 Whole-brain T1-weighted structural image

A 0.6 mm isotropic whole-brain T1-weighted multi-echo MP-RAGE scan was acquired using a custom sequence optimized for scanning efficiency (Brenner et al., 2013) and minimal geometric distortions (van der Kouwe et al., 2008) (TI = 1.1 s, TR = 2.5 s, TEs = 1.84/3.55/5.26/6.92 ms, FA = 7°, TA = 7:12, readout pixel bandwidth: 970 Hz, matrix size: 428 x 364 x 256, elliptical sampling, sagittal slice orientation, CAIPIRINHA (Breuer et al., 2006) 1 x 2z1 parallel imaging undersampling with on-line 2D GRAPPA reconstruction, turbofactor: 218). Finally, the four echo time images were collapsed into a single high-SNR image using root-mean-squares combination.

2.4.2 Reduced hippocampus field-of-view T2-weighted structural image

For motion-robust hippocampal subfield-segmentation, three rapid, 0.4 mm x 0.4 mm x 1.0 mm T2-weighted, slice-selective TSE scans were acquired consecutively on a reduced hippocampus field-of-view (TE = 76 ms, TR = 8090 ms, FA = 60° using Hyperecho (Hennig & Scheffler, 2001), TA = 2:59, readout pixel bandwidth: 150 Hz, matrix size: 512 x 512, 55 oblique-coronal slices of 1 mm thickness orthogonal to the long hippocampus axis, 3-fold parallel imaging undersampling with online 1D GRAPPA reconstruction, turbofactor: 9). The RF transmit power reference voltage was varied across the scans (200 V, 240 V, 280 V) such that the nominal refocusing flip angles of the protocol were approximately reached in all brain regions in at least one of the scans. Finally, the three images from each participant were coregistered, denoised following the Rician noise estimation (Coupé et al., 2010), and averaged.

2.4.3 Rapid whole-brain submillimeter fMRI

A custom interleaved multishot 3D echo planar imaging (EPI) sequence was used (Stirnberg & Stöcker, 2021) with the following paramaters: TE = 21.6 ms, TRvol = 3.4 s, FA = 15°, 6/8 partial Fourier sampling along the primary phase encode direction, oblique-axial slice orientation along the anterior-posterior commissure line, readout pixel bandwidth: 1136 Hz, matrix size: 220 x 220 x 140. To obtain both a high nominal spatial resolution of 0.9 mm isotropic at TRvol = 3.4 s while imaging the whole brain at sufficient signal-to-noise-ratio (SNR) with a BOLD-optimal TE, several unique sequence features were combined for this work at 7 T. (A) Skipped-CAIPI 3.1 x 7z2 sampling (SNR-optimized 7-fold CAIPIRINHA undersampling combined with interleaved 3-shot segmentation) (Stirnberg & Stöcker, 2021) with on-line 2D GRAPPA reconstruction. (B) One externally acquired phase correction scan per volume instead of typically integrated phase correction scans per shot (Stirnberg & Stöcker, 2021). (C) Variable echo train lengths, skipping only the latest EPI echoes outside of a semi-elliptical k-space mask that defines 0.9 mm isotropic voxel resolution (Stirnberg et al., 2017). (D) Rapid slab-selective binomial-121 water excitation instead of time-consuming fat saturation (Stirnberg et al., 2016). A 3-min fMRI practice-run was performed before the two main functional sessions, which lasted approximately 15 min each. The MRI session was concluded by a standard 3 mm isotropic two-echo gradient-echo field-mapping scan acquired within 35 s. A maximum number of 264 imaging volumes were acquired from each functional session. The first five images consisting of the waiting period of 17 s prior to the beginning of the first trial were excluded to rule out non-steady-state signals.

2.5 MRI data processing

2.5.1 Segmentation of hippocampal subfields

Manual segmentation of hippocampal subfields was performed on the averaged and denoised native space T2-weighted structural scans according to the protocol described by Dalton et al. (2017). ROI masks were created for six hippocampal subfields, including DG/CA4, CA3/2, CA1, subiculum, pre/parasubiculum, and uncus using the software application ITK-SNAP 3.8 (Yushkevich et al., 2006). We excluded the uncus from our analyses because this region contains a mix of different hippocampal subfields (Ding & Van Hoesen, 2015) that are difficult to differentiate on structural MRI scans (Dalton et al., 2017) even with the high-resolution achieved in the current study. To assess intra- and inter-rater reliability, five hippocampi were segmented by two independent raters (P.L. and M.A.D) and again 6 months after initial segmentation. The inter-rater reliability as measured by the DICE overlap metric (Dice, 1945) was in accordance with those reported in the existent literature (Bonnici, Chadwick, Kumaran, et al., 2012; Palombo et al., 2013): DG/CA4 = 0.87 (aim 0.86-0.80), CA3/2 = 0.73 (aim of 0.74-0.67), CA1 = 0.80 (aim of 0.81-0.67), subiculum = 0.80 (aim of 0.79-0.57), and pre/parasubiculum = 0.64 (aim of 0.67-0.57). Intra-rater reliability was measured 8 months apart and also showed high concordance between segmentations at the two different time points (0.92 for DG/CA4, 0.79 for CA3/2, 0.84 for CA1, 0.84 for subiculum, and 0.86 for pre/parasubiculum).

2.5.2 Segmentation of hippocampal subfields in anterior, anterior body, posterior body, and tail

Each hippocampal subfield ROI mask was divided into four portions along the longitudinal axis of the hippocampus (anterior, anterior body, posterior body, and tail) using the software application ITK-SNAP 3.8, according to the protocol described by Dalton et al. (2017, 2019). The anterior masks encompassed the first slice on which the hippocampus was visible up to the slice preceding the first slice of the DG. The mean number of slices in the anterior mask was 7.67 (SD = 2.01). The remaining slices, beginning with the first slice of the DG until the last slice of the hippocampus, were summed and divided into three parts to create equivalent slices in the anterior body, posterior body, and tail, resulting in a mean of 11.04 (SD = 1.01), 10.73 (SD = 0.98), and 10.54 (SD = 0.94) slices respectively. The average total number of slices covering the hippocampus was 39.98 (SD = 2.57).

2.5.3 Functional MRI preprocessing

To address both research aims, two steps of MRI preprocessing were performed using SPM12 (Statistical Parametric Mapping 12) software package (www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/) on MATLAB v17a (MathWorks) computing platform (https://matlab.mathworks.com/). For both steps, the anatomical and the functional scans obtained from each subject were reoriented to be in line with the anterior-posterior commissure axis. The field maps, including the phase and magnitude images, were used to calculate the voxel displacement maps (VDM) to geometrically correct the distorted EPI images. The VDMs were then applied to the functional scans during realignment and unwarping. The averaged anatomical scans (and hippocampal subfield masks) were co-registered with the functional scans (see Fig. S1). After motion correction and co-registration, the preprocessing pipelines for the two research aims diverged: For the first aim (to examine differential activation of hippocampal subfields), fMRI data were kept in native space to allow maximum spatial precision. Only a sparse Gaussian smoothing kernel of 1 mm full-width half-maximum (FWHM) to reduce excess noise (Yoo et al., 2018) and a temporal high-pass filter of 128 s was applied to the function data. Then, one-sample T-Test contrasts (T-contrasts) were calculated to test the effects of AM retrieval versus baseline (MA). For the second aim (to examine functional connectivity of hippocampal subfields to other neocortical regions), motion-corrected and co-registered fMRI data were normalized to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space and smoothed with a slightly smaller than standard 6 mm FWHM to examine activation and functional connectivity at the group level.

Additionally, in order to mitigate the artifacts produced by unsolicited movements during the functional tasks for further exclusion criteria, we screened for motion artifact outliers in the time series using the ARtifact detection Tools (ART) software package (v2015). The standard threshold of outlier detection if motion exceeds the 97th percentile of the global mean intensity (relating to under 1 mm motion) for more than 10% of the number of the scans. No excessive motion was detected; hence, no participant was excluded from the study.

2.6 Statistical analyses

2.6.1 Hippocampal subfield activation extraction

For the native space fMRI analyses, we followed the standard GLM procedure in SPM12 with trials designated as mini blocks and covering the elaboration period fixed at the last 8 s of the stimulus time and prior to the display of the vividness rating. Since only a few trials were rated as faint/difficult, we included all trials in our analyses. Furthermore, motion correction parameters were included in the GLM as covariate of no interest. The contrasts of interest of the first level were specified as (1) AM versus Baseline and (2) MA versus Baseline. Signal intensity values were extracted for both contrasts for the five right and five left hippocampal subfield ROIs covering the entire length of the hippocampus. We then subtracted the signal intensities for MA from the signal intensities for AM for each of the ROIs for each participant. In a second step, we extracted signal intensities for AM and MA of the five right and five left hippocampal subfields for the anterior body, posterior body, and tail portions separately. Signal intensities were extracted for each participant in native space using MATLAB-based Response Exploration (REX) toolbox (https://www.nitrc.org/projects/rex) by applying the segmented ROI masks. Since we found no evidence of laterality effects (t = 1.04, df = 23, p = 0.3568, see Table S1), signal intensities for bilateral subfields were collapsed. Differential signal intensity values were subjected to a 1-way-RM-ANOVA with Tukey’s multiple-comparison test. A significance threshold of p < 0.05 was applied. Furthermore, the temporal signal-to-noise ratios (tSNR) across the fMRI time series along the longitudinal axis of the hippocampal subfields were examined and the 1-way-RM-ANOVA with Dunnett’s multiple-comparison tests were applied (see Fig. S2).

2.6.2 Group analyses of whole-brain activation and hippocampal subfield functional connectivity

First, to assess differences between AM and MA, a multivariate mean-centered partial least squares (PLS) group analysis was performed. Detailed descriptions of PLS can be found elsewhere (Krishnan et al., 2011; McIntosh & Lobaugh, 2004). In brief, PLS uses singular value decomposition to extract ranked latent variables (LVs) from the covariance matrix of brain activity and conditions in a data-driven manner. These LVs express patterns of brain activity associated with each condition. Statistical significance of the LVs was assessed using permutation testing. In this procedure, each participant’s data were randomly reassigned (without replacement) to different experimental conditions, and a null distribution was derived from 500 permutated solutions. We considered LVs as significant at p < 0.05. Furthermore, we assessed the reliability of each voxel that contributed to a specific LV’s activity pattern using a bootstrapped estimation of the standard error (bootstrap ratio, BSR). For each bootstrapped solution (100 in total), participants were sampled randomly with replacement and a new analysis was performed. In the current study, we considered clusters of 50 or more voxels with BSRs greater than 3 (approximately equal to a p < 0.001) to represent reliable patterns of activation. Of note, PLS uses two re-sampling techniques that (1) scramble the data of each participant’s conditions so that small but reliable differences between true experimental conditions can be detected, and (2) exclude whole datasets of participants, so that outliers who may drive significant effects can be detected.

To assess functional connectivity between hippocampal subfields and the rest of the brain, seed PLS, an extension of the mean-centered PLS was used (McIntosh & Lobaugh, 2004). Seed PLS examines the relationship between a target region (seed region) and signal intensities in all other brain voxels as a function of the experimental conditions (Krishnan et al., 2011). The main difference to the mean-centered PLS is that, in seed PLS, the covariance matrix used in the single value decomposition stems from correlation values between the seed region and all other voxels for each experimental condition. Thus, seed PLS offered us to examine the multivoxel patterns which correlate with fMRI signal extracted from individual hippocampal subfields. Signal intensities of individual subfields (extracted in native space) were used as seeds for the group analyses (performed in MNI space). We conducted three seed PLS analyses (1. anterior body, 2. posterior body, and 3. tail) containing all five hippocampal subfields and contrasting AM versus MA trials. A significance threshold for the LV’s (500 permutations) was p < 0.05. After establishing whether the functional connectivity pattern differed between AM and MA across all five subfields for a specific portion of the hippocampal long-axis, we then examined this portion more closely with follow-up PLS analyses. In these post hoc analyses, functional connectivity of each of the five hippocampal subfields was assessed separately and a Bonferroni multiple-comparison correction was applied so that we considered statistical significance for the LV’s at p < 0.01 (see for a similar approach (McCormick et al., 2021)). Boot strap ratios (100 bootstraps) of <3 and >3 (corresponding approximately to a p < 0.01) were considered significant.

3.1 Behavioral results

All 24 participants reported being able to recall detail-rich autobiographical memories (average of 1.92 ± 0.67, 1 = able to recall detail-rich memories; 6 = unable to recall any personal events) and construct vivid mental images (average of 1.83 ± 0.69, 1 = able to create detail-rich mental images; 6 = lack of visual imagery). During scanning, the participants spent 3.53 s (±0.98) on average to select a memory and around 3.66 s (±0.97) to solve the MA problem. Whereas some trials were excluded from the analyses due to missing button presses, no significant difference was found between the speed of AM retrieval and MA solving (t = 0.8601, p = 0.3958). Participants indicated in 35.67 (±4.67) trials out of 40 trials that their memories were vivid (t = 19.36, df = 23, p < 0.0001) and 28.13 (±6.15) trials out of 40 MA problems were reported as easy (t = 8.553, df = 23, p < 0.0001).

3.2 Hippocampal subfield activation during AM

Greater bilateral hippocampal activation during AM retrieval than MA solving was found in all participants (see Fig. 1). Furthermore, all hippocampal subfields showed greater activation during AM retrieval than solving an MA problem (DG/CA4: t = 6.140, df = 23, p < 0.001, CA2/3: t = 5.217, df = 23, p < 0.001, CA1: t = 3.768, df = 23, p = 0.001, subiculum: t = 3.068, df = 23, p = 0.005, pre/parasubiculum: t = 4.637, df = 23, p < 0.001, Bonferroni corrected). Further analyses revealed differences in hippocampal subfield activity associated with AM retrieval (F = 5.887, df = 5, p = 0.017, Fig. 1 and Table S1 for an overview of % signal changes). In line with our hypothesis (Fig. 1E), we found that activation during AM was much stronger in the pre/parasubiculum compared with CA1 (df = 23, p = 0.001), the subiculum (df = 23, p = 0.001), CA2/3 (df = 23, p = 0.049), and at a non-significant trend level in DG/CA4 (df = 23, p = 0.075). There were no other significant differences between subfield activation associated with AM retrieval.

Fig. 1 (Color).

Differential hippocampal subfield engagement during AM retrieval. (A) Overlaid segmentation of labeled hippocampal subfields, including the DG, CA1-4, subiculum, and pre/parasubiculum on high-resolution structural T2-weighted scan. (B) Examples of AM versus MA activation along the longitudinal axis of the hippocampus (shown in red) from three participants (Y coordinates from upper to lower panels of 20, 17, and 27, beginning from rostral to caudal of 55 slices, respectively). (C) Examples of manual hippocampal subfields segmentation for signals extraction. The subfields along the longitudinal axis are divided into four portions of anterior, anterior body, posterior body, and tail (Y coordinates of 16, 21, 38, and 46, respectively). (D) Hippocampus subfields along the longitudinal axis. (E) The comparison between the % signal change during AM and MA in hippocampal subfields (DG/CA4, CA1, CA2/3, subiculum, and pre/parasubiculum). The pre/parasubiculum showed stronger differentiation between AM and MA than most other subfields. (F) This effect was driven by the anterior body part of the hippocampus. (G) The anterior body of the pre/parasubiculum shows greater differentiation between AM and MA than all other subfields, whereas no significant difference was found in the posterior body (H) nor the tail (I). *** p < 0.001, ** p < 0.01, and * p < 0.05, and + p < 0.1 (non-significant trend level).

Fig. 1 (Color).

Differential hippocampal subfield engagement during AM retrieval. (A) Overlaid segmentation of labeled hippocampal subfields, including the DG, CA1-4, subiculum, and pre/parasubiculum on high-resolution structural T2-weighted scan. (B) Examples of AM versus MA activation along the longitudinal axis of the hippocampus (shown in red) from three participants (Y coordinates from upper to lower panels of 20, 17, and 27, beginning from rostral to caudal of 55 slices, respectively). (C) Examples of manual hippocampal subfields segmentation for signals extraction. The subfields along the longitudinal axis are divided into four portions of anterior, anterior body, posterior body, and tail (Y coordinates of 16, 21, 38, and 46, respectively). (D) Hippocampus subfields along the longitudinal axis. (E) The comparison between the % signal change during AM and MA in hippocampal subfields (DG/CA4, CA1, CA2/3, subiculum, and pre/parasubiculum). The pre/parasubiculum showed stronger differentiation between AM and MA than most other subfields. (F) This effect was driven by the anterior body part of the hippocampus. (G) The anterior body of the pre/parasubiculum shows greater differentiation between AM and MA than all other subfields, whereas no significant difference was found in the posterior body (H) nor the tail (I). *** p < 0.001, ** p < 0.01, and * p < 0.05, and + p < 0.1 (non-significant trend level).

Close modal

3.3 Hippocampal subfield activation along its longitudinal axis during AM

Next, we assessed differential subfield engagement of the anterior body, posterior body, and tail portions separately. Strikingly, the RM-ANOVA found a main effect of subfield activation levels in the anterior body of the hippocampus (F = 4.440, df = 4, p = 0.024, see Fig. 1G) but not in the posterior body (F = 1.650, df = 4, p = 0.1895) or tail (F = 1.157, df = 4, p = 0.3286) of the hippocampus (Fig. 1H and 1I, respectively). The anterior (the first 7.67 slices on average) was omitted from these analyses due to (1) significantly less number of slices and voxel count compared to the other portions and (2) not all subfields being present in this portion of the hippocampus (e.g., the DG). No signal intensity was detected in the anterior portion in one participant, possibly due to signal drop out. Post hoc analyses revealed that, in the anterior body portion of the hippocampus, activation relating to AM versus MA was much stronger in the pre/parasubiculum than in CA1 (df = 23, p < 0.001), CA2/3 (df = 23, p = 0.019), subiculum (df = 23, p = 0.034), and DG/CA4 (df = 23, p = 0.034). There were no other significant differences between anterior body subfields in activation associated with AM retrieval (see 1 for signal change during AM versus MA tasks). Further comparison (F(2, 23) = 9.878, p < 0.001) revealed that the activation difference (AM versus MA) of the pre/parasubiculum in the anterior body was stronger than the pre/parasubiculum in the posterior body (df = 23, p < 0.001) and the tail (df = 23, p = 0.020).

3.4 Hippocampal subfield-neocortical interactions during AM

On a whole-brain, whole-group basis, we found greater activation during AM retrieval than MA in all regions typically associated with AM retrieval, including bilateral hippocampal activation as well as vmPFC and medial/lateral parietal activation (LV1, p < 0.0001, see Fig. 2 and Table S3 for peak MNI coordinates).

Fig. 2.

Hippocampal subfield functional connectivity during AM. (A) Greater activation for AM versus MA is shown in bootstraps ratios (BSR). All regions typically associated with AM show greater activation of bilateral hippocampi, vmPFC, and medial/lateral parietal cortex for AM. The whole-brain fMRI activation is overlaid on a standard T1-weighted MRI image. (B) Greater functional connectivity of the anterior body of the pre/parasubiculum during AM than MA is shown. All regions typically associated with AM retrieval show strong functional connectivity, including vmPFC and medial/lateral parietal cortices.

Fig. 2.

Hippocampal subfield functional connectivity during AM. (A) Greater activation for AM versus MA is shown in bootstraps ratios (BSR). All regions typically associated with AM show greater activation of bilateral hippocampi, vmPFC, and medial/lateral parietal cortex for AM. The whole-brain fMRI activation is overlaid on a standard T1-weighted MRI image. (B) Greater functional connectivity of the anterior body of the pre/parasubiculum during AM than MA is shown. All regions typically associated with AM retrieval show strong functional connectivity, including vmPFC and medial/lateral parietal cortices.

Close modal

Next, we evaluated patterns of functional connectivity along the long-axis of the hippocampus. We found that only functional connectivity patterns of the anterior body of the hippocampus differed between AM and MA (anterior body: LV 1, p = 0.027, posterior body: LV 1, p = 0.51, tail: LV 1, p = 0.64). In Bonferroni corrected post hoc analyses (thus applying a threshold of p < 0.01), we found that only the functional connectivity of the pre/parasubiculum differed between both conditions (pre/parasubiculum: p < 0.001), whereas the other subfields did not (DG/CA4: p = 0.53, CA2/3: p = 0.16, CA1: p = 0.36, subiculum: p = 0.28). Figure 2 illustrates the brain pattern associated with functional connectivity of the anterior body of the pre/parasubiculum during AM, including all canonical regions typically associated with AM, including the vmPFC and medial/lateral parietal cortices (see Table S4 for peak MNI coordinates and Table S5 for peak MNI coordinates of MA). In addition to these areas that are traditionally associated with the AM network, the anterior body of the pre/parasubiculum also showed significant functional connectivity with posterior visual-perceptual cortices (i.e., right lingual gyrus and bilateral fusiform gyrus) during AM than MA.

By exploiting a novel whole-brain 7 T fMRI sequence with submillimeter voxel size, this study provides two novel insights into hippocampal memory processes. First, we found evidence that the anterior body of the pre/parasubiculum was significantly more engaged during the vivid re-experience of AMs than other hippocampal subfields. Second, during AM, only the anterior body of the pre/parasubiculum showed stronger functional connectivity to neocortical brain regions typically engaged during AM retrieval over and above other hippocampal subfields. We discuss these findings in turn.

We observed that all hippocampal subfields differentiated between AM and MA with the pre/parasubiculum differentiating between cognitive tasks to a greater extent than the neighboring subiculum and CA fields. While our AM task was not designed to specifically target different cognitive states (i.e., scene-specific content), our finding indicates that overall AM retrieval preferentially engages the pre/parasubiculum. Examining our study design closer, we only included participants who reported to be able to retrieve visually detail-rich AMs easily and we focused our analyses on the last 8 s of the AM trials. This served to highlight the period in which participants were most likely engaged in re-experiencing visual-perceptual imagery. One explanation for our findings, therefore, is that the pre/parasubiculum is preferentially engaged in tasks which rely on vivid scene-based cognition (Dalton & Maguire, 2017). For example, previous work suggests that the pre/parasubiculum is more strongly activated during scene-imagery than object-imagery (Dalton et al., 2018; Zeidman et al., 2015), and scene/object discrimination (Hodgetts et al., 2017). Furthermore, we know from rodents and nonhuman primates that the pre/parasubiculum contains an abundance of head, grid, and border cells (Boccara et al., 2010; Lever et al., 2009; Robertson et al., 1999), which has recently been extended to human goal direction cells (Shine et al., 2019). Arguably, AM retrieval relies heavily on scene-based cognition since AMs tend to unfold on a visuo-spatial stage (Greenberg & Knowlton, 2014). In fact, when participants are asked to imagine personal events, they tend to place the event onto a spatial stage (Robin et al., 2018), indicating that visuo-spatial imagery plays a fundamental role in episodic memory retrieval. Further strengthening the tight link between the ability to recall AMs and visual imagery, people with little or no ability to experience visual imagery, commonly referred to as aphantasics, also tend to have difficulties recalling AM (Dawes et al., 2020; Milton et al., 2021; Zeman et al., 2015, 2020). Interestingly, aphantasics not only have difficulties to recall visual-perceptual details to their AMs, but they seem to have a worse ability to retrieve personal memories in general (Dawes et al., 2020) and show poorer verbal and non-verbal memory function (Monzel et al., 2021). In fact, a recent phenomenon called severe deficient autobiographical memory or SDAM (Palombo, Sheldon, et al., 2018; Watkins, 2018) has also been associated with aphantasia (Pearson, 2019). This line of thought meshes well with the scene construction theory positing that a dominant function of the hippocampus is to construct spatially coherent internal models of the environment (Dalton & Maguire, 2017; Maguire & Mullally, 2013; McCormick, Ciaramelli, et al., 2018; Zeidman & Maguire, 2016), and the pre/parasubiculum may be of special significance to this process (Dalton & Maguire, 2017; Dalton et al., 2018). In line with the scene construction theory, patients with hippocampal damage have been shown to use less scene-based cognition in their mind-wandering episodes (McCormick, Rosenthal, et al., 2018), moral decision-making (McCormick et al., 2016), and scene-based judgements (McCormick et al., 2017). Therefore, our results point towards a potential role of the pre/parasubiculum in tasks relying heavily on vivid visuo-spatial imagery, such as vivid AM retrieval.

Having highlighted the role of the pre/parasubiculum in vivid AM retrieval, we further found that all other hippocampal subfields also showed stronger activation during AM retrieval than MA. This result is not surprising since there might be many factors differentiating the cognitive process of recalling AMs from solving MA problems. Especially, since AM retrieval is a complex cognitive task with a magnitude of different operations (such as detail integration, and discrimination), it is likely that the other hippocampal subfields contribute to different processes which we could not dissociate in the current study. In fact, recent investigations show different contributions of hippocampal subfields to mnemonic processes. Although there is somewhat mixed evidence in the current literature, CA fields have been implicated in the integration or associations of memory details, such as external and internal (Chadwick et al., 2014; Grande et al., 2019; Miller et al., 2020; Newman & Hasselmo, 2014), whereas the DG/CA4 region (Baker et al., 2016; Berron et al., 2016; Newman & Hasselmo, 2014; van Dijk & Fenton, 2018) may support the separation or discrimination of mnemonic information. The aim of the current study was to evaluate a submillimeter 7 T fMRI sequence during a robust, reliable, and established AM paradigm. Future studies will now have to experimentally target specific subfield functions, such as examining AMs with and without visual imagery. Our newly developed 7 T fMRI sequence will allow the innovative investigation of differential hippocampal subfield contributions to cognition.

The second major goal of the current study was to examine functional connectivity of hippocampal subfields to the neocortex during AM retrieval. We found that the anterior body of the pre/parasubiculum, over and above other subfields, has strong functional connectivity to neocortical regions known to support AM retrieval. In addition, this effect was specific to the anterior body and not evident in the posterior body or tail portions of the hippocampus. This finding adds new detail to several lines of research. For example, both 3 T and 7 T fMRI resting-state studies have identified the subiculum (in which the pre/parasubiculum was included) as a functional connectivity hub correlating with activity in the default mode network which has overlapping brain regions to the AM network (Ezama et al., 2021; Shah et al., 2018). In addition, previous task-based 3 T fMRI revealed that hippocampal functional connectivity during AM of a seed region in the vicinity of the pre/parasubiculum (McCormick et al., 2015) was strongly connected to a brain-wide network comprising the vmPFC and medial/lateral parietal cortices, as well as visual-perceptual regions of the occipital cortex. Furthermore, the same region of the anterior medial hippocampus was more strongly connected to frontal and parietal cortices during scene construction than object construction (Zeidman et al., 2015). Additionally, from a neuroanatomical perspective, the pre/parasubiculum is a primary target of the parieto-medial temporal visual pathway carrying information about visuo-spatial representations of the environment (Dalton & Maguire, 2017). The parieto-medial temporal pathway directly links the pre/parasubiculum with the inferior parietal lobule, posterior cingulate cortex, retrosplenial cortex, and parahippocampal gyrus (Ding, 2013; Ding & Van Hoesen, 2015; Kravitz et al., 2011). Each of these regions has been heavily associated with visuo-spatial cognition (Auger & Floresco, 2014; Epstein et al., 2007) and AM (Svoboda et al., 2006) and connect directly with the pre/parasubiculum, giving it privileged access to visuo-spatial information. While most of this evidence stems from anatomical connectivity studies in rodent and nonhuman primates, a recent diffusion-weighted imaging study supports this framework by showing, for the first time in the human brain, that circumscribed regions along the anterior-posterior axis of the pre/parasubiculum, including a specific portion in the anterior body of the hippocampus, have dense patterns of anatomical connectivity with distributed cortical brain areas implicated in AM (Dalton et al., 2022). The anterior portion was shown to exhibit greater connectivity with temporal, medial parietal, and occipital regions. The posterior hippocampus, more intense in the tail, was partially found to be connected with medial parietal and occipital cortices. Our results dovetail nicely with this collection of structural and functional data and provide new evidence that the pre/parasubiculum in the anterior body of the hippocampus may be an important hippocampal hub for scene-based cognition.

In summary, here we utilized a novel submillimeter 7 T fMRI sequence which enabled us to examine functional connectivity between hippocampal subfields and neocortical regions during vivid AM retrieval. We enhanced our knowledge of hippocampal subfield contributions to cognition by showing that the anterior body of the pre/parasubiculum was more engaged during AM than other neighboring hippocampal subfields and that this part of the hippocampus was strongly functionally connected to regions typically recruited during AM. In context of the broader literature, these observations correspond well with multiple lines of evidence, suggesting that the anterior body of the pre/parasubiculum may be a central component of the networks underpinning AM retrieval.

The data are available upon request by contacting the Lead Contact, Cornelia McCormick ([email protected]).

P.L.: Data curation, formal analysis, and original draft writing. M.A.D.: Methodology, visualization, writing, reviewing, and editing. R.S.: Methodology. T.S.: Methodology. A.Sp.: Reviewing, editing. A.Sc.: Reviewing, editing. C.M.: Conceptualization, methodology, data curation, visualization, supervision, writing, reviewing, and editing.

The authors have no competing interests to declare.

C.M. and P.L. are supported by a BONFOR fellowship (2019-2-06, 2021-2A-01) and the German Research Foundation (MC-244/3-1). We thank Sascha Brunheim, Anke Rühling, and Jennifer Schlee for their technical support during scanning and all participants for their time.

Supplementary material for this article is available with the online version here: https://doi.org/10.1162/imag_a_00105.

Addis
,
D. R.
,
Wong
,
A. T.
, &
Schacter
,
D. L.
(
2007
).
Remembering the past and imagining the future: Common and distinct neural substrates during event construction and elaboration
.
Neuropsychologia
,
45
,
1363
1377
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.10.016
Auger
,
M. L.
, &
Floresco
,
S. B.
(
2014
).
Prefrontal cortical GABA modulation of spatial reference and working memory
.
Int J Neuropsychopharmacol
,
18
,
pyu013
. https://doi.org/10.1093/ijnp/pyu013
Baker
,
S.
,
Vieweg
,
P.
,
Gao
,
F.
,
Gilboa
,
A.
,
Wolbers
,
T.
,
Black
,
S. E.
, &
Rosenbaum
,
R. S.
(
2016
).
The human dentate gyrus plays a necessary role in discriminating new memories
.
Curr Biol
,
26
,
2629
2634
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2016.07.081
Barry
,
D. N.
,
Clark
,
I. A.
, &
Maguire
,
E. A.
(
2021
).
The relationship between hippocampal subfield volumes and autobiographical memory persistence
.
Hippocampus
,
31
,
362
374
. https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.23293
Bartsch
,
T.
,
Döhring
,
J.
,
Rohr
,
A.
,
Jansen
,
O.
, &
Deuschl
,
G.
(
2011
).
CA1 neurons in the human hippocampus are critical for autobiographical memory, mental time travel, and autonoetic consciousness
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
,
108
(
42
),
17562
17567
. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1110266108
Berron
,
D.
,
Schütze
,
H.
,
Maass
,
A.
,
Cardenas-Blanco
,
A.
,
Kuijf
,
H. J.
,
Kumaran
,
D.
, &
Düzel
,
E.
(
2016
).
Strong evidence for pattern separation in human dentate gyrus
.
J Neurosci
,
36
,
7569
7579
. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.0518-16.2016
Boccara
,
C. N.
,
Sargolini
,
F.
,
Thoresen
,
V. H.
,
Solstad
,
T.
,
Witter
,
M. P.
,
Moser
,
E. I.
, &
Moser
,
M. B.
(
2010
).
Grid cells in pre- and parasubiculum
.
Nat Neurosci
,
13
,
987
994
. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2602
Bonnici
,
H. M.
,
Chadwick
,
M. J.
,
Kumaran
,
D.
,
Hassabis
,
D.
,
Weiskopf
,
N.
, &
Maguire
,
E. A.
(
2012
).
Multi-voxel pattern analysis in human hippocampal subfields
.
Front Hum Neurosci
,
6
,
290
. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00290
Bonnici
,
H. M.
,
Chadwick
,
M. J.
,
Lutti
,
A.
,
Hassabis
,
D.
,
Weiskopf
,
N.
, &
Maguire
,
E. A.
(
2012
).
Detecting representations of recent and remote autobiographical memories in vmPFC and hippocampus
.
J Neurosci
,
32
,
16982
16991
. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.2475-12.2012
Bonnici
,
H. M.
,
Chadwick
,
M. J.
, &
Maguire
,
E. A.
(
2013
).
Representations of recent and remote autobiographical memories in hippocampal subfields
.
Hippocampus
,
23
(
10
),
849
854
. https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.22155
Brenner
,
D.
,
Stirnberg
,
R.
,
Pracht
,
E. D.
, &
Stöcker
,
T.
(
2013
).
ESMRMB 2013, 30th Annual Scientific Meeting, Toulouse, France, 3–5 October: Abstracts, Friday
.
Magn Reson Mater Phy
,
26
,
151
301
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10334-013-0383-6
Breuer
,
F. A.
,
Blaimer
,
M.
,
Mueller
,
M. F.
,
Seiberlich
,
N.
,
Heidemann
,
R. M.
,
Griswold
,
M. A.
, &
Jakob
,
P. M.
(
2006
).
Controlled aliasing in volumetric parallel imaging (2D CAIPIRINHA)
.
Magn Reson Med
,
55
,
549
556
. https://doi.org/10.1002/mrm.20787
Chadwick
,
M. J.
,
Bonnici
,
H. M.
, &
Maguire
,
E. A.
(
2014
).
CA3 size predicts the precision of memory recall
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
,
111
,
10720
10725
. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1319641111
Clark
,
I. A.
, &
Maguire
,
E. A.
(
2020
).
Do questionnaires reflect their purported cognitive functions
?
Cognition
,
195
,
104114
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2019.104114
Conway
,
M. A.
(
2009
).
Episodic memories
.
Neuropsychologia
,
47
,
2305
2313
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.02.003
Conway
,
M. A.
, &
Pleydell-Pearce
,
C. W.
(
2000
).
The construction of autobiographical memories in the self-memory system
.
Psychol Rev
,
107
,
261
288
. https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-295x.107.2.261
Coupé
,
P.
,
Manjón
,
J. V.
,
Gedamu
,
E.
,
Arnold
,
D.
,
Robles
,
M.
, &
Collins
,
D. L.
(
2010
).
Robust Rician noise estimation for MR images
.
Med Image Anal
,
14
,
483
493
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.media.2010.03.001
Dalton
,
M. A.
,
D'Souza
,
A.
,
Lv
,
J.
, &
Calamante
,
F.
(
2022
).
New insights into anatomical connectivity along the anterior–posterior axis of the human hippocampus using in vivo quantitative fibre tracking
.
Elife
,
11
,
e76143
. https://doi.org/10.7554/elife.76143
Dalton
,
M. A.
, &
Maguire
,
E. A.
(
2017
).
The pre/parasubiculum: A hippocampal hub for scene-based cognition
?
Curr Opin Behav Sci
,
17
,
34
40
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.06.001
Dalton
,
M. A.
,
McCormick
,
C.
,
De Luca
,
F.
,
Clark
,
I. A.
, &
Maguire
,
E. A.
(
2019
).
Functional connectivity along the anterior-posterior axis of hippocampal subfields in the ageing human brain
.
Hippocampus
,
29
,
1049
1062
. https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.23097
Dalton
,
M. A.
,
McCormick
,
C.
, &
Maguire
,
E. A.
(
2019
).
Differences in functional connectivity along the anterior-posterior axis of human hippocampal subfields
.
Neuroimage
,
192
,
38
51
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.02.066
Dalton
,
M. A.
,
Zeidman
,
P.
,
Barry
,
D. N.
,
Williams
,
E.
, &
Maguire
,
E. A.
(
2017
).
Segmenting subregions of the human hippocampus on structural magnetic resonance image scans: An illustrated tutorial
.
Brain Neurosci Adv
,
1
,
2398212817701448
. https://doi.org/10.1177/2398212817701448
Dalton
,
M. A.
,
Zeidman
,
P.
,
McCormick
,
C.
, &
Maguire
,
E. A.
(
2018
).
Differentiable processing of objects, associations, and scenes within the hippocampus
.
J Neurosci
,
38
,
8146
8159
. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.0263-18.2018
Dawes
,
A. J.
,
Keogh
,
R.
,
Andrillon
,
T.
, &
Pearson
,
J.
(
2020
).
A cognitive profile of multi-sensory imagery, memory and dreaming in aphantasia
.
Sci Rep
,
10
,
10022
. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-65705-7
Dice
,
L. R.
(
1945
).
Measures of the amount of ecologic association between species
.
Ecology
,
26
,
297
. https://doi.org/10.2307/1932409
Ding
,
S.-L.
(
2013
).
Comparative anatomy of the prosubiculum, subiculum, presubiculum, postsubiculum, and parasubiculum in human, monkey, and rodent
.
J Comp Neurol
,
521
,
4145
4162
. https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.23416
Ding
,
S.-L.
, &
Van Hoesen
,
G. W.
(
2015
).
Organization and detailed parcellation of human hippocampal head and body regions based on a combined analysis of cyto- and chemoarchitecture
.
J Comp Neurol
,
523
,
2233
2253
. https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.23786
Epstein
,
R. A.
,
Parker
,
W. E.
, &
Feiler
,
A. M.
(
2007
).
Where am I now? Distinct roles for parahippocampal and retrosplenial cortices in place recognition
.
J Neurosci
,
27
,
6141
6149
. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.0799-07.2007
Ezama
,
L.
,
Hernández-Cabrera
,
J. A.
,
Seoane
,
S.
,
Pereda
,
E.
, &
Janssen
,
N.
(
2021
).
Functional connectivity of the hippocampus and its subfields in resting-state networks
.
Eur J Neurosci
,
53
,
3378
3393
. https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.15213
Finn
,
E. S.
,
Huber
,
L.
,
Jangraw
,
D. C.
,
Molfese
,
P. J.
, &
Bandettini
,
P. A.
(
2019
).
Layer-dependent activity in human prefrontal cortex during working memory
.
Nat Neurosci
,
22
,
1687
1695
. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-019-0487-z
Grande
,
X.
,
Berron
,
D.
,
Horner
,
A. J.
,
Bisby
,
J. A.
,
Düzel
,
E.
, &
Burgess
,
N.
(
2019
).
Holistic recollection via pattern completion involves hippocampal subfield CA3
.
J Neurosci
,
39
,
8100
8111
. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.0722-19.2019
Grande
,
X.
,
Sauvage
,
M. M.
,
Becke
,
A.
,
Düzel
,
E.
, &
Berron
,
D.
(
2022
).
Transversal functional connectivity and scene-specific processing in the human entorhinal-hippocampal circuitry
.
Elife
,
11
,
e76479
. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.76479
Greenberg
,
D. L.
, &
Knowlton
,
B. J.
(
2014
).
The role of visual imagery in autobiographical memory
.
Mem Cognit
,
42
,
922
934
. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-014-0402-5
Hennig
,
J.
, &
Scheffler
,
K.
(
2001
).
Hyperechoes
.
Magn Reson Med
,
46
,
6
12
. https://doi.org/10.1002/mrm.1153
Hodgetts
,
C. J.
,
Voets
,
N. L.
,
Thomas
,
A. G.
,
Clare
,
S.
,
Lawrence
,
A. D.
, &
Graham
,
K. S.
(
2017
).
Ultra-high-field fMRI reveals a role for the subiculum in scene perceptual discrimination
.
J Neurosci
,
37
,
3150
3159
. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.3225-16.2017
Kravitz
,
D. J.
,
Saleem
,
K. S.
,
Baker
,
C. I.
, &
Mishkin
,
M.
(
2011
).
A new neural framework for visuospatial processing
.
Nat Rev Neurosci
,
12
,
217
230
. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3008
Krishnan
,
A.
,
Williams
,
L. J.
,
McIntosh
,
A. R.
, &
Abdi
,
H.
(
2011
).
Partial least squares (PLS) methods for neuroimaging: A tutorial and review
.
Neuroimage
,
56
,
455
475
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.034
Lever
,
C.
,
Burton
,
S.
,
Jeewajee
,
A.
,
O’Keefe
,
J.
, &
Burgess
,
N.
(
2009
).
Boundary vector cells in the subiculum of the hippocampal formation
.
J Neurosci
,
29
,
9771
9777
. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.1319-09.2009
Maass
,
A.
,
Schütze
,
H.
,
Speck
,
O.
,
Yonelinas
,
A.
,
Tempelmann
,
C.
,
Heinze
,
H.-J.
,
Berron
,
D.
,
Cardenas-Blanco
,
A.
,
Brodersen
,
K. H.
,
Stephan
,
K. E.
, &
Düzel
,
E.
(
2014
).
Laminar activity in the hippocampus and entorhinal cortex related to novelty and episodic encoding
.
Nat Commun
,
5
,
5547
. https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms6547
Maguire
,
E. A.
, &
Mullally
,
S. L.
(
2013
).
The hippocampus: A manifesto for change
.
J Exp Psychol Gen
,
142
,
1180
1189
. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033650
McCormick
,
C.
,
Barry
,
D.N, Jafarian, A.
,
Barnes
,
G. R.
, &
Maguire
,
E. A.
(
2020
).
vmPFC drives hippocampal processing during autobiographical memory recall regardless of remoteness
.
Cereb Cortex
,
30
,
5972
5987
. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhaa172
McCormick
,
C.
,
Ciaramelli
,
E.
,
De Luca
,
F.
, &
Maguire
,
E. A.
(
2018
).
Comparing and contrasting the cognitive effects of hippocampal and ventromedial prefrontal cortex damage: A review of human lesion studies
.
Neuroscience
,
374
,
295
318
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.07.066
McCormick
,
C.
,
Dalton
,
M. A.
,
Zeidman
,
P.
, &
Maguire
,
E. A.
(
2021
).
Characterising the hippocampal response to perception, construction and complexity
.
Cortex
,
137
,
1
17
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2020.12.018
McCormick
,
C.
,
Rosenthal
,
C. R.
,
Miller
,
T. D.
, &
Maguire
,
E. A.
(
2016
).
Hippocampal damage increases deontological responses during moral decision making
.
J Neurosci
,
36
,
12157
12167
. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.0707-16.2016
McCormick
,
C.
,
Rosenthal
,
C. R.
,
Miller
,
T. D.
, &
Maguire
,
E. A.
(
2017
).
Deciding what is possible and impossible following hippocampal damage in humans
.
Hippocampus
,
27
,
303
314
. https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.22694
McCormick
,
C.
,
Rosenthal
,
C. R.
,
Miller
,
T. D.
, &
Maguire
,
E. A.
(
2018
).
Mind-Wandering in people with hippocampal damage
.
J Neurosci
,
38
,
2745
2754
. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.1812-17.2018
McCormick
,
C.
,
St-Laurent
,
M.
,
Ty
,
A.
,
Valiante
,
T.A
& .,
McAndrews
,
M. P.
(
2015
).
Functional and effective hippocampal-neocortical connectivity during construction and elaboration of autobiographical memory retrieval
.
Cereb Cortex
,
25
,
1297
1305
. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bht324
McIntosh
,
A. R.
, &
Lobaugh
,
N. J.
(
2004
).
Partial least squares analysis of neuroimaging data: Applications and advances
.
Neuroimage
,
23
Suppl 1
,
S250
S263
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.020
Miller
,
T. D.
,
Chong
,
T. T.-J.
,
Aimola Davies
,
A. M.
,
Johnson
,
M. R.
,
Irani
,
S. R.
,
Husain
,
M.
,
Ng
,
T. W.
,
Jacob
,
S.
,
Maddison
,
P.
,
Kennard
,
C.
,
Gowland
,
P. A.
, &
Rosenthal
,
C. R.
(
2020
).
Human hippocampal CA3 damage disrupts both recent and remote episodic memories
.
Elife
,
9
,
e41836
. https://doi.org/10.7554/elife.41836
Milton
,
F.
,
Fulford
,
J.
,
Dance
,
C.
,
Gaddum
,
J.
,
Heuerman-Williamson
,
B.
,
Jones
,
K.
,
Knight
,
K. F.
,
MacKisack
,
M.
,
Winlove
,
C.
, &
Zeman
,
A.
(
2021
).
Behavioral and neural signatures of visual imagery vividness extremes: Aphantasia versus hyperphantasia
.
Cereb Cortex Commun
,
2
,
tgab035
. https://doi.org/10.1093/texcom/tgab035
Monzel
,
M.
, &
Vetterlein
,
A.
,
Reuter
,
M.
(
2021
).
Memory deficits in aphantasics are not restricted to autobiographical memory—Perspectives from the dual coding approach
.
J Neuropsychol
,
16
(
2
),
444
461
. https://doi.org/10.1111/jnp.12265
Moscovitch
,
M.
,
Rosenbaum
,
R. S.
,
Gilboa
,
A.
,
Addis
,
D. R.
,
Westmacott
,
R.
,
Grady
,
C.
,
McAndrews
,
M. P.
,
Levine
,
B.
,
Black
,
S.
,
Winocur
,
G.
, &
Nadel
,
L.
(
2005
).
Functional neuroanatomy of remote episodic, semantic and spatial memory: A unified account based on multiple trace theory
.
J Anat
,
207
,
35
66
. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7580.2005.00421.x
Newman
,
E. L.
, &
Hasselmo
,
M. E.
(
2014
).
CA3 sees the big picture while dentate gyrus splits hairs
.
Neuron
,
81
,
226
228
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2014.01.004
Norris
,
D. G.
, &
Polimeni
,
J. R.
(
2019
).
Laminar (f)MRI: A short history and future prospects
.
Neuroimage
,
197
,
643
649
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.04.082
Palombo
,
D. J.
,
Amaral
,
R. S. C.
,
Olsen
,
R. K.
,
Müller
,
D. J.
,
Todd
,
R. M.
,
Anderson
,
A. K.
, &
Levine
,
B.
(
2013
).
KIBRA polymorphism is associated with individual differences in hippocampal subregions: Evidence from anatomical segmentation using high-resolution MRI
.
J Neurosci
,
33
,
13088
13093
. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.1406-13.2013
Palombo
,
D. J.
,
Bacopulos
,
A.
,
Amaral
,
R. S. C.
,
Olsen
,
R. K.
,
Todd
,
R. M.
,
Anderson
,
A. K.
, &
Levine
,
B.
(
2018
).
Episodic autobiographical memory is associated with variation in the size of hippocampal subregions
.
Hippocampus
,
28
,
69
75
. https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.22818
Palombo
,
D. J.
,
Sheldon
,
S.
, &
Levine
,
B.
(
2018
).
Individual differences in autobiographical memory
.
Trends Cogn Sci (Regul Ed)
,
22
,
583
597
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2018.04.007
Pearson
,
J.
(
2019
).
The human imagination: The cognitive neuroscience of visual mental imagery
.
Nat Rev Neurosci
,
20
,
624
634
. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41583-019-0202-9
Poppenk
,
J.
,
Evensmoen
,
H. R.
,
Moscovitch
,
M.
, &
Nadel
,
L.
(
2013
).
Long-axis specialization of the human hippocampus
.
Trends Cogn Sci
,
17
(
5
),
230
-
240
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.03.005
Risius
,
U.-M.
,
Staniloiu
,
A.
,
Piefke
,
M.
,
Maderwald
,
S.
,
Schulte
,
F. P.
,
Brand
,
M.
, &
Markowitsch
,
H. J.
(
2013
).
Retrieval, monitoring, and control processes: A 7 tesla FMRI approach to memory accuracy
.
Front Behav Neurosci
,
7
,
24
. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00024
Robertson
,
R. G.
,
Rolls
,
E. T.
,
Georges-François
,
P.
, &
Panzeri
,
S.
(
1999
).
Head direction cells in the primate pre-subiculum
.
Hippocampus
,
9
,
206
219
. https://doi.org/10.1002/(sici)1098-1063(1999)9:3<206::aid-hipo2>3.3.co;2-8
Robin
,
J.
,
Buchsbaum
,
B. R.
, &
Moscovitch
,
M.
(
2018
).
The primacy of spatial context in the neural representation of events
.
J Neurosci
,
38
,
2755
2765
. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.1638-17.2018
Rosenbaum
,
R. S.
,
Moscovitch
,
M.
,
Foster
,
J. K.
,
Schnyer
,
D. M.
,
Gao
,
F.
,
Kovacevic
,
N.
,
Verfaellie
,
M.
,
Black
,
S. E.
, &
Levine
,
B.
(
2008
).
Patterns of autobiographical memory loss in medial-temporal lobe amnesic patients
.
J Cogn Neurosci
,
20
,
1490
1506
. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2008.20105
Scoville
,
W. B.
, &
Milner
,
B.
(
1957
).
Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions
.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry
,
20
,
11
21
. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.20.1.11
Sekeres
,
M. J.
,
Winocur
,
G.
, &
Moscovitch
,
M.
(
2018
).
The hippocampus and related neocortical structures in memory transformation
.
Neurosci Lett
,
680
,
39
53
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2018.05.006
Shah
,
P.
,
Bassett
,
D.S
,
Wisse
,
L. E. M.
,
Detre
,
J. A.
,
Stein
,
J. M.
,
Yushkevich
,
P. A.
,
Shinohara
,
R. T.
,
Pluta
,
J. B.
,
Valenciano
,
E.
,
Daffner
,
M.
,
Wolk
,
D. A.
,
Elliott
,
M. A.
,
Litt
,
B.
,
Davis
,
K. A.
, &
Das
,
S. R.
(
2018
).
Mapping the structural and functional network architecture of the medial temporal lobe using 7 T MRI
.
Hum Brain Mapp
,
39
,
851
865
. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23887
Shine
,
J. P.
,
Valdés-Herrera
,
J. P.
,
Tempelmann
,
C.
, &
Wolbers
,
T.
(
2019
).
Evidence for allocentric boundary and goal direction information in the human entorhinal cortex and subiculum
.
Nat Commun
,
10
,
4004
. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-019-11802-9
Stirnberg
,
R.
,
Brenner
,
D.
,
Stöcker
,
T.
, &
Shah
,
N. J.
(
2016
).
Rapid fat suppression for three-dimensional echo planar imaging with minimized specific absorption rate
.
Magn Reson Med
,
76
,
1517
1523
. https://doi.org/10.1002/mrm.26063
Stirnberg
,
R.
,
Huijbers
,
W.
,
Brenner
,
D.
,
Poser
,
B. A.
,
Breteler
,
M.
, &
Stöcker
,
T.
(
2017
).
Rapid whole-brain resting-state fMRI at 3 T: Efficiency-optimized three-dimensional EPI versus repetition time-matched simultaneous-multi-slice EPI
.
Neuroimage
,
163
,
81
92
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.08.031
Stirnberg
,
R.
, &
Stöcker
,
T.
(
2021
).
Segmented K-space blipped-controlled aliasing in parallel imaging for high spatiotemporal resolution EPI
.
Magn Reson Med
,
85
,
1540
1551
. https://doi.org/10.1002/mrm.28486
Strange
,
B. A.
,
Witter
,
M. P.
,
Lein
,
E. S.
, &
Moser
,
E. I.
(
2014
).
Functional organization of the hippocampal longitudinal axis
.
Nat Rev Neurosci
,
15
(
10
),
655
669
. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3785
Svoboda
,
E.
,
McKinnon
,
M. C.
, &
Levine
,
B.
(
2006
).
The functional neuroanatomy of autobiographical memory: A meta-analysis
.
Neuropsychologia
,
44
,
2189
2208
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.05.023
van der Kouwe
,
A. J. W.
,
Benner
,
T.
,
Salat
,
D. H.
, &
Fischl
,
B.
(
2008
).
Brain morphometry with multiecho MPRAGE
.
Neuroimage
,
40
,
559
569
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.12.025
van Dijk
,
M. T.
, &
Fenton
,
A. A.
(
2018
).
On how the dentate gyrus contributes to memory discrimination
.
Neuron
,
98
,
832.e5
845.e5
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2018.04.018
Watkins
,
N. W.
(
2018
).
(A)phantasia and severely deficient autobiographical memory: Scientific and personal perspectives
.
Cortex
,
105
,
41
52
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2017.10.010
Willems
,
T.
, &
Henke
,
K.
(
2021
).
Imaging human engrams using 7 Tesla magnetic resonance imaging
.
Hippocampus
,
31
,
1257
1270
. https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.23391
Yoo
,
P. E.
,
John
,
S. E.
,
Farquharson
,
S.
,
Cleary
,
J. O.
,
Wong
,
Y. T.
,
Ng
,
A.
,
Mulcahy
,
C. B.
,
Grayden
,
D. B.
,
Ordidge
,
R. J.
,
Opie
,
N. L.
,
O'Brien
,
T. J.
,
Oxley
,
T. J.
, &
Moffat
,
B. A.
(
2018
).
7 T-fMRI: Faster temporal resolution yields optimal BOLD sensitivity for functional network imaging specifically at high spatial resolution
.
Neuroimage
,
164
,
214
229
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.03.002
Yushkevich
,
P. A.
,
Piven
,
J.
,
Hazlett
,
H. C.
,
Smith
,
R.G
,
Ho
,
S.
,
Gee
,
J. C.
, &
Gerig
,
G.
(
2006
).
User-guided 3D active contour segmentation of anatomical structures: Significantly improved efficiency and reliability
.
Neuroimage
,
31
,
1116
1128
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.015
Zeidman
,
P.
,
Lutti
,
A.
, &
Maguire
,
E. A.
(
2015
).
Investigating the functions of subregions within anterior hippocampus
.
Cortex
,
73
,
240
256
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2015.09.002
Zeidman
,
P.
, &
Maguire
,
E. A.
(
2016
).
Anterior hippocampus: The anatomy of perception, imagination and episodic memory
.
Nat Rev Neurosci
,
17
,
173
182
. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2015.24
Zeman
,
A.
,
Dewar
,
M.
, &
Della Sala
,
S.
(
2015
).
Lives without imagery—Congenital aphantasia
.
Cortex
,
73
,
378
380
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2015.05.019
Zeman
,
A.
,
Milton
,
F.
,
Della Sala
,
S.
,
Dewar
,
M.
,
Frayling
,
T.
,
Gaddum
,
J.
,
Hattersley
,
A.
,
Heuerman-Williamson
,
B.
,
Jones
,
K.
,
MacKisack
,
M.
, &
Winlove
,
C.
(
2020
).
Phantasia—The psychological significance of lifelong visual imagery vividness extremes
.
Cortex
,
130
,
426
440
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2020.04.003
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International (CC BY 4.0) license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. For a full description of the license, please visit https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/legalcode.

Supplementary data