We aimed to replicate recent findings on the association between the extent of cerebral small vessel disease (cSVD), functional brain network dedifferentiation, and cognitive impairment. We analyzed demographic, imaging, and behavioral data from the prospective population-based Hamburg City Health Study. Using a fully prespecified analysis pipeline, we estimated discrete brain states from structural and resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). In a multiverse analysis, we varied brain parcellations and functional MRI confound regression strategies. The severity of cSVD was operationalized as the volume of white matter hyperintensities of presumed vascular origin. Processing speed and executive dysfunction were quantified using the Trail Making Test (TMT). We hypothesized a) that a greater volume of supratentorial white matter hyperintensities would be associated with less time spent in functional MRI-derived brain states of high fractional occupancy; and b) that less time spent in these high-occupancy brain states associated with a longer time to completion in part B of the TMT. High-occupancy brain states were characterized by activation or suppression of the default mode network. Every 5.1-fold increase in WMH volume was associated with a 0.94-fold reduction in the odds of occupying DMN-related brain states (P = 5.01×108). Every 5% increase in time spent in high-occupancy brain states was associated with a 0.98-fold reduction in the TMT-B completion time (P = 0.0116). Findings were robust across most brain parcellations and confound regression strategies. In conclusion, we successfully replicated previous findings on the association between cSVD, functional brain occupancy, and cognition in an independent sample. The data provide further evidence for a functional network dedifferentiation hypothesis of cSVD-related cognitive impairment. Further research is required to elucidate the mechanisms underlying these associations.

Cerebral small vessel disease (cSVD) is an arteriolopathy of the brain associated with age and common cardiovascular risk factors (Wardlaw, C. Smith, & Dichgans, 2013). cSVD predisposes patients to ischemic stroke (in particular lacunar stroke) and may lead to cognitive impairment and dementia (Cannistraro et al., 2019). Neuroimaging findings in cSVD reflect its underlying pathology (Wardlaw et al., 2015) and include white matter hyperintensities (WMH), lacunes of presumed vascular origin, small subcortical infarcts and microbleeds, enlarged perivascular spaces, as well as brain atrophy (Wardlaw, E. E. Smith, Biessels, et al., 2013). However, the extent of visible cSVD features on magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) is an imperfect predictor of the severity of clinical sequelae (Das et al., 2019) and our understanding of the causal mechanisms linking cSVD-associated brain damage to clinical deficits remains limited (Bos et al., 2018).

Recent efforts have focused on exploiting network aspects of the structural (Tuladhar, et al., 2016, 2020; Lawrence, Zeestraten, et al., 2018) and functional (Dey et al., 2016; Schulz et al., 2021) organization of the brain to understand the relationship between cSVD and clinical deficits in cognition and other domains that rely on distributed processing. Reduced structural network efficiency has repeatedly been described as a causal factor in the development of cognitive impairment, particularly executive dysfunction and reduced processing speed in cSVD (Lawrence, Chung, et al., 2014; Shen et al., 2020; Reijmer et al., 2016; Prins et al., 2005). Findings with respect to functional connectivity (FC), however, are more heterogeneous than their SC counterparts, perhaps because FC measurements are prone to be affected by hemodynamic factors and noise, resulting in relatively low reliability, especially with resting-state scans of short duration (Laumann et al., 2015). This problem is exacerbated in the presence of cSVD and worsened by arbitrary processing choices (Gesierich et al., 2020; Lawrence, Tozer, et al., 2018).

As a promising new avenue, time-varying, or dynamic, functional connectivity approaches have recently been explored in patients with subcortical ischemic vascular disease (Xu et al., 2021; Yin et al., 2022). Although the study of dynamic FC measures may not solve the problem of limited reliability, especially in small populations or participants with extensive structural brain changes, it adds another—temporal—dimension to the study of functional brain organization, which is otherwise overlooked. Importantly, FC dynamics not only reflect moment-to-moment fluctuations in cognitive processes, but are also related to brain plasticity and homeostasis (Laumann & Snyder, 2021; Laumann, et al., 2017), which may be impaired in cSVD.

In the present paper, we aimed to replicate and extend the main results of Schlemm et al. (2022). In this recent study, the authors analyzed MR imaging and clinical data from the prospective Hamburg City Health Study (HCHS, Jagodzinski et al., 2020) using a coactivation pattern approach to define discrete brain states, and found associations between the WMH load, time spent in high-occupancy brain states characterized by activation or suppression of the default mode network (DMN), and cognitive impairment. Specifically, every 4.7-fold increase in WMH volume was associated with a 0.95-fold reduction in the odds of occupying a DMN-related brain state; every 2.5 seconds (i.e., one repetition time) not spent in one of those states was associated with a 1.06-fold increase in TMT-B completion times.

The fractional occupancy of a functional MRI-derived discrete brain state is a participant-specific measure of brain dynamics and is defined as the proportion of BOLD volumes assigned to that state relative to all BOLD volumes acquired during a resting-state scan.

Our primary hypothesis for the present work was that the volume of supratentorial white matter hyperintensities is associated with fractional occupancy of DMN-related brain states in a middle-aged to elderly population mildly affected by cSVD. Our secondary hypothesis was that fractional occupancy is associated with executive dysfunction and reduced processing speed, measured as the time to complete part B of the Trail Making Test (TMT).

Both hypotheses were tested in an independent subsample of the HCHS study population using the same imaging protocols, examination procedures, and analysis pipelines as those in Schlemm et al. (2022). The robustness of the associations was explored using a multiverse approach by varying key steps in the analysis pipeline.

2.1 Study population

This study analyzed data from the Hamburg City Health Study (HCHS), an ongoing prospective, population-based cohort study aiming to recruit a cross-sectional sample of 45000 adult participants from the city of Hamburg, Germany (Jagodzinski et al., 2020). From the first 10000 participants of the HCHS, we planned to include those who were documented to have received brain imaging (n = 2648) and exclude those who were analyzed in our previous report (Schlemm et al., 2022) (n = 970). The ethical review board of the Landesärztekammer Hamburg (State of Hamburg Chamber of Medical Practitioners) approved the HCHS (PV5131), and all participants provided written informed consent.

2.2 Demographic and clinical characterization

From the study database, we extracted the participants’ age at the time of inclusion in years, their sex, and the number of years spent in education. During the visit to the study center, participants underwent cognitive assessment using standardized tests. From the database, we extracted their performance scores on the Trail Making Test part B, measured in seconds, as an operationalization of executive function and psychomotor processing speed (Arbuthnott & Frank, 2000; Tombaugh, 2004). For descriptive purposes, we also extracted data on past medical history and reported the proportion of participants with a previous diagnosis of dementia.

2.3 MRI acquisition and preprocessing

The magnetic resonance imaging protocol for the HCHS includes structural and resting-state functional sequences. The acquisition parameters for a 3 T Siemens Skyra MRI scanner (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) have been previously reported (Frey et al., 2021; Petersen et al., 2020) and are given as follows:

For T1-weighted anatomical images, a 3D rapid acquisition gradient-echo sequence (MPRAGE) was used with the following sequence parameters: repetition time TR=2500ms, echo time TE=2.12ms, 256 axial slices, slice thickness ST=0.94mm, and in-plane resolution IPR=(0.83×0.83)mm2.

T2-weighted fluid attenuated inversion recovery (FLAIR) images were acquired with the following sequence parameters: TR=4700ms, TE=392ms, 192 axial slices, ST=0.9mm, IPR=(0.75×0.75)mm2.

125 resting state functional MRI volumes were acquired (TR=2500ms; TE=25ms; flip angle=90°; slices = 49; ST=3mm; slice gap=0mm; IPR=(2.66×2.66) mm2). The participants were asked to keep their eyes open and to think of nothing.

We verified the presence and voxel dimensions of expected MRI data for each participant and excluded those for whom at least one of T1-weighted, FLAIR, and resting-state MRI was missing. We also excluded participants with neuroradiologically confirmed space-occupying intra-axial lesion. To ensure reproducibility, no visual quality assessment of raw images was performed.

For the remaining participants, structural and resting-state functional MRI data were preprocessed using FreeSurfer v6.0 (https://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/), and fMRIPrep v20.2.6 (Esteban et al., 2019), using default parameters. Participants were excluded if automated processing using at least one of these packages failed.

2.4 Quantification of WMH load

For our primary analysis, the extent of ischemic white matter disease was operationalized as the total volume of supratentorial WMH obtained from automated segmentation using a combination of anatomical priors, BIANCA (Griffanti et al., 2016), and LOCATE (Sundaresan et al., 2019), post-processed with a minimum cluster size of 30 voxels, as described in Schlemm et al. (2022). In an exploratory analysis, we partitioned voxels identified as WMH into deep and periventricular components according to their distance to the ventricular system (cut-off 10mm, Griffanti et al., 2018).

2.5 Brain state estimation

The output from fMRIprep was post-processed using xcpEngine v1.2.3 to obtain de-confounded spatially averaged BOLD time series (Ciric et al., 2017). For the primary analysis, we used the 36p regression strategy and the Schaefer-400 parcellation (Schaefer et al., 2018), as in Schlemm et al. (2022).

Different atlases and confound regression strategies, as implemented in xcpEngine, were included in an exploratory multiverse analysis.

Co-activation pattern (CAP) analysis was performed by first aggregating parcellated, de-confounded BOLD signals into a (nparcels×intime points,i) feature matrix, where ntime points,i denotes the number of retained volumes for participant i after confound regression. Clustering was performed using the k-means algorithm (k=5) with a distance measure given by 1 minus the sample Pearson correlation between points, as implemented in Matlab R2021a. We estimated the participant- and state-specific fractional occupancies, which are defined as the proportion of BOLD volumes assigned to each brain state (Vidaurre et al., 2018). The two states with the highest average occupancies were identified as the basis for further analysis.

2.6 Statistical analysis

For demographic (age, sex, and years of education) and clinical (TMT-B) variables, the number of missing items is reported. For non-missing values, we provide descriptive summary statistics using median and interquartile range. The proportions of men and women in the sample are reported. Since we expected based on our pilot data (Schlemm et al., 2022) that the proportion of missing data would be small, primary regression modelling was carried out as a complete-case analysis.

As an outcome-neutral quality check of the implementation of the MRI processing pipeline, brain state estimation, and co-activation pattern analysis, we compared fractional occupancies between brain states. We expected that the average fractional occupancy in the two high-occupancy states would be higher than the average fractional occupancy in the other three states. Point estimates and 95% confidence intervals are presented for the difference in average fractional occupancy to verify this assertion.

For further analyses, non-zero WMH volumes were subjected to logarithmic transformation. Zero values retained their value of zero; to compensate, all models included a binary indicator for zero WMH volume if at least one non-zero WMH value was present.

To assess the primary hypothesis of a negative association between the extent of ischemic white matter disease and time spent in high-occupancy brain states, we performed a fixed-dispersion Beta regression to model the logit of the conditional expectation of the average fractional occupancy of two high-occupancy states as an affine function of the logarithmized WMH load. Age and sex were included as covariates. The strength of the association was quantified as the odds ratio per interquartile ratio of the WMH burden distribution, and is accompanied by a 95% confidence interval. Significance testing of the null hypothesis of no association was conducted at the conventional significance level of 0.05. Estimation and testing were carried out using the “betareg” package v3.1.4 in R v4.2.1.

To assess the secondary hypothesis of an association between time spent in high-occupancy brain states and executive dysfunction, we performed a generalized linear regression with a Gamma response distribution to model the logarithm of the conditional expected completion time in part B of the TMT as an affine function of the average fractional occupancy of two high-occupancy states. Age, sex, years of education, and logarithmized WMH load were included as covariates. The strength of the association was quantified as a multiplicative factor per percentage point and accompanied by a 95% confidence interval. Significance testing of the null hypothesis of no association was conducted at the conventional significance level of 0.05. Estimation and testing were performed using the glm function included in the “stats” package from R v4.2.1.

2.7 Pre-registered analyses

The analysis plan was pre-registered on June 27, 2023 at https://osf.io/fcqmb. The sample size calculation was based on an effect size on the odds ratio scale of 0.95, corresponding to an absolute difference in the probability of occupying a DMN-related brain state between the first and third WMH-load quartile of 1.3 percentage points, and between the 5% and 95% percentile of 3.1 percentage points. Approximating half the difference in fractional occupancy of DMN-related states between different task demands (rest vs n-back) in healthy participants, which was estimated to lie between 6 and 7 percentage points (Cornblath et al., 2020), this value represented a plausible choice for the smallest effect size of theoretical and practical interest. It also equals the estimated effect size based on the data presented in Schlemm et al. (2022).

Simple bootstrapping was used to create 10000 hypothetical datasets of size 200, 400, 600, 800, 900, 910,… , 1090, 1100, 1200, 1400, 1500, and 1600. Each dataset was then subjected to the estimation procedure described above. For each sample size, the proportion of datasets in which the primary null hypothesis of no association between fractional occupancy and WMH load could be rejected at α=0.05 was computed and recorded as a power curve in Figure 1.

Fig. 1.

Sample size and power estimation. A-priori estimated power for different sample sizes was obtained as the proportion of synthetic data sets in which the null hypothesis of no association between WMH volume and time spent in high-occupancy brain states could be rejected at the α=0.05 significance level. Proportions are based on a total of 10000 synthetic data sets obtained by bootstrapping the data presented in Schlemm et al. (2022). Highlighted in orange are the smallest sample size ensuring a power of at least 80% (n=960), the sample size of the pilot data (n=988, post-hoc power 81.3%), the expected sample sample size for this replication study (n=1500, a-priori power 93.8%), and the achieved sample size (n=1651, a-priori power 95.4%).

Fig. 1.

Sample size and power estimation. A-priori estimated power for different sample sizes was obtained as the proportion of synthetic data sets in which the null hypothesis of no association between WMH volume and time spent in high-occupancy brain states could be rejected at the α=0.05 significance level. Proportions are based on a total of 10000 synthetic data sets obtained by bootstrapping the data presented in Schlemm et al. (2022). Highlighted in orange are the smallest sample size ensuring a power of at least 80% (n=960), the sample size of the pilot data (n=988, post-hoc power 81.3%), the expected sample sample size for this replication study (n=1500, a-priori power 93.8%), and the achieved sample size (n=1651, a-priori power 95.4%).

Close modal

A sample size of 960 would have allowed the replication of the reported effect with a power of 80.2%. We had anticipated a sample size of 1500, which would have yielded a power of 93.8%.

2.8 Multiverse analysis

In both (Schlemm et al., 2022) and our primary replication analysis, we made certain analytical choices in the operationalization of brain states and ischemic white matter disease, namely the use of the 36p confound regression strategy, the Schaefer-400 parcellation, and a BIANCA/LOCATE-based WMH segmentation algorithm. The robustness of the association between WMH burden and time spent in high-occupancy states with regard to other choices was explored in a multiverse analysis (Steegen et al., 2016). Specifically, in an exploratory analysis, we estimated brain states from BOLD time series processed according to a variety of established confound regression strategies and aggregated over different cortical brain parcellations (Table 2, Ciric et al., 2017, 2018). The extent of cSVD was additionally quantified by the volume of deep and periventricular white matter hyperintensities.

For each combination of analytical choice of confound regression strategy, parcellation, and subdivision of white matter lesion load (9×9×3=243 scenarios in total), we quantified the association between WMH load and average time spent in high-occupancy brain states using odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals as described above.

No hypothesis testing was performed for these multiverse analyses. Rather, they serve to inform about the robustness of the outcome of the test of the primary hypothesis. Any substantial conclusions about the association between the severity of cerebral small vessel pathology and the time spent in high-occupancy brain states were drawn from the primary analysis using pre-specified methodological choices, as stated in the Scientific Question in Table 1.

Table 1.

Study Design.

QuestionHypothesisSampling planAnalysis planRationale for deciding the sensitivity of the testInterpretation given different outcomesTheory that could be shown wrong by the outcome
Is severity of cerebral small disease, quantified by the volume of supratentorial white matter hyperintensities of presumed vascular origin (WMH), associated with time spent in high-occupancy brain states, defined by resting-state functional MRI? (Primary) Higher WMH volume is associated with lower average occupancy of the two highest-occupancy brain states. Available participants with clinical and imaging data from the the HCHS (Jagodzinski et al., 2020Standardized preprocessing of structural and functional MRI data • automatic quantification of WMH • co-activation pattern analysis • multivariable generalized regression analyses Tradition P<0.05—> rejection of the null hypothesis of no association between cSVD and fractional occupancy; P>0.05—> insufficient evidence to reject the null hypothesis Functional brain dynamics are not related to subcortical ischemic vascular disease. 
Is time spent in high-occupancy brain states associated with cognitive impairment, measured as the time to complete part B of the trail making test (TMT)? (Secondary) Lower average occupancy of the two highest-occupancy brain states is associated with longer TMT-B time. as above as above as above P<0.05—> rejection of the null hypothesis of no association between fractional occupancy and cognitive impairment; P>0.05—> insufficient evidence to reject the null hypothesis Cognitive function is not related to MRI-derived functional brain dynamics. 
QuestionHypothesisSampling planAnalysis planRationale for deciding the sensitivity of the testInterpretation given different outcomesTheory that could be shown wrong by the outcome
Is severity of cerebral small disease, quantified by the volume of supratentorial white matter hyperintensities of presumed vascular origin (WMH), associated with time spent in high-occupancy brain states, defined by resting-state functional MRI? (Primary) Higher WMH volume is associated with lower average occupancy of the two highest-occupancy brain states. Available participants with clinical and imaging data from the the HCHS (Jagodzinski et al., 2020Standardized preprocessing of structural and functional MRI data • automatic quantification of WMH • co-activation pattern analysis • multivariable generalized regression analyses Tradition P<0.05—> rejection of the null hypothesis of no association between cSVD and fractional occupancy; P>0.05—> insufficient evidence to reject the null hypothesis Functional brain dynamics are not related to subcortical ischemic vascular disease. 
Is time spent in high-occupancy brain states associated with cognitive impairment, measured as the time to complete part B of the trail making test (TMT)? (Secondary) Lower average occupancy of the two highest-occupancy brain states is associated with longer TMT-B time. as above as above as above P<0.05—> rejection of the null hypothesis of no association between fractional occupancy and cognitive impairment; P>0.05—> insufficient evidence to reject the null hypothesis Cognitive function is not related to MRI-derived functional brain dynamics. 

Overview of the Scientific Questions addressed in the present study (first column), the two main hypotheses being investigated (second column), and details of the underlying study.

Table 2.

Multiverse analysis.

Name of the atlas#parcelsReference
Desikan–Killiany 86 Desikan et al., 2006  
AAL 116 Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002  
Harvard–Oxford 112 Makris et al., 2006  
glasser360 360 Glasser et al., 2016  
gordon333 333 Gordon et al., 2016  
power264 264 Power et al., 2011  
schaefer{N} 100 Schaefer et al., 2018  
 200  
 400  
(a) Parcellations 
Design Reference 
24p Friston et al., 1996  
24p + GSR Macey et al., 2004  
36p Satterthwaite et al., 2013  
36p + spike regression Cox, 1996  
36p + despiking Satterthwaite et al., 2013  
36p + scrubbing Power et al., 2014  
ACompCor Muschelli et al., 2014  
TCompCor Behzadi et al., 2007  
AROMA Pruim et al., 2015  
(b) Confound regression strategies, adapted from (Ciric et al., 2017
Name of the atlas#parcelsReference
Desikan–Killiany 86 Desikan et al., 2006  
AAL 116 Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002  
Harvard–Oxford 112 Makris et al., 2006  
glasser360 360 Glasser et al., 2016  
gordon333 333 Gordon et al., 2016  
power264 264 Power et al., 2011  
schaefer{N} 100 Schaefer et al., 2018  
 200  
 400  
(a) Parcellations 
Design Reference 
24p Friston et al., 1996  
24p + GSR Macey et al., 2004  
36p Satterthwaite et al., 2013  
36p + spike regression Cox, 1996  
36p + despiking Satterthwaite et al., 2013  
36p + scrubbing Power et al., 2014  
ACompCor Muschelli et al., 2014  
TCompCor Behzadi et al., 2007  
AROMA Pruim et al., 2015  
(b) Confound regression strategies, adapted from (Ciric et al., 2017

Overview over different brain parcellations and confound regression strategies implemented using xcpEngine (Ciric et al., 2018). A total of 9×9=81 analytical combinations were explored to assess the robustness of our results with respect to these processing choices.

AAL: Automatic Anatomical Labeling; AROMA: Automatic Removal of Motion Artifacts; GSR: Global signal regression.

2.9 Further exploratory analysis

In previous work, two high-occupancy brain states have been related to the default mode network (Cornblath et al., 2020). We further explored this relationship by computing, for each individual brain state, the cosine similarity of the positive and negative activations of the cluster’s centroid with a set of a priori defined functional ‘communities’ or networks (Schaefer et al., 2018; Yeo et al., 2011). The results were visualized as spider plots for the Schaefer atlases.

In further exploratory analyses, we describe the associations between brain state dynamics and other measures of cognitive ability such as memory and language.

2.10 Pilot data and analysis

Summary data from the first 1000 imaging data points of the HCHS have been published with Schlemm et al. (2022) and formed the basis for the hypotheses tested in this replication study. Before preregistration, we had implemented our prespecified analysis pipeline described above in R and Matlab, and applied it to this previous sample. Data, code, and results from this pilot analysis have been stored with the archived Stage 1 report on GitHub (https://github.com/csi-hamburg/HCHS-brain-states-RR, v1.5) and preserved on Zenodo.

2.11 Timeline and access to data

At the time of planning of this study, all demographic, clinical, and imaging data used in this analysis had been collected by the HCHS and were held in the central trial database. Quality checks for non-imaging variables had been performed centrally. WMH segmentation based on structural MRI data of the first 10000 participants of the HCHS had been performed previously using the BIANCA/LOCATE approach (Rimmele et al., 2022). Functional MRI data and clinical measures of executive dysfunction (TMT-B scores) had not previously been analyzed by the pre-registering author (ES).

2.12 Deviations from preregistration

For deconfounding and aggregating BOLD data at brain parcellation level, the software xcpEngine was used in version 1.2.3, not 1.2.1, to ensure that that the correct MNI reference template (MNI152NLin2009cAsym) is used for registration of brain atlases. This decision was made before analyzing the data.

For this replication study, a total of 2648 datasets were available, of which 970 were already included in our previous analysis and thus discarded. In 13 of the resulting 1678 datasets, one or more MRI sequences were missing. Of the complete datasets (n = 1665), we excluded 5 participants due to intra-axial space-occupying lesions. An additional 9 participants were excluded because of unsuccessful preprocessing, WMH segmentation, or xcpEngine failure, resulting in 1651 datasets for analysis. A study-flowchart is provided in Figure 2.

Fig. 2.

Study flowchart. Composition of the study population after application of inclusion and exclusion criteria, and image processing.

Fig. 2.

Study flowchart. Composition of the study population after application of inclusion and exclusion criteria, and image processing.

Close modal

Baseline demographic and cognitive values, including the number of missing items, are reported in Table 3.

Table 3.

Descriptive statistics of the study population.

N = 1651
Demographics (no Missing, n (%)) 
Age, yr 
 Median (IQR) 66 (59–72) 
Sex 
 Male 940/1651 (57%) 
 Female 711/1651 (43%) 
Cardiovascular risk factors 
Hypertension 
 Present 1177/1611 (73.1%) 
 Missing n (%) 40 (2.4%) 
Diabetes 
 Present 157/1566 (10%) 
 Missing n (%) 85 (5.1%) 
Smoking 
 Present 200/1360 (14.7%) 
 Missing n (%) 201 (12.9%%) 
Hyperlipidaemia 
 Present 426/1578 (27%) 
 Missing n (%) 73 (4.4%) 
Cognitive test results 
MMSE, # (max. 30) 
 Median (IQR) 28 (27–29) 
 Missing n (%) 129 (7.8%) 
Vocabulary (MWT-B), # (max. 37) 
 Median (IQR) 32 (30–34) 
 Missing n (%) 295 (18%) 
Word recall, # (max. 10) 
 Median (IQR) 8 (6–9) 
 Missing n (%) 180 (11%) 
Animal Naming 
 Median (IQR) 24 (20–29) 
 Missing n (%) 116 (7.0%) 
TMT-A, seconds 
 Median (IQR) 38 (31–48) 
 Missing n (%) 144 (8.7%) 
TMT-B, seconds 
 Median (IQR) 83 (65–110) 
 Missing n (%) 162 (9.8%) 
History 
Diagnosed dementia 
 Present 6/1645 (0.4%) 
 Missing n (%) 6 (0.4%) 
Years of education 
 Median (IQR) 13 (12–16) 
 Missing n (%) 34 (2%) 
N = 1651
Demographics (no Missing, n (%)) 
Age, yr 
 Median (IQR) 66 (59–72) 
Sex 
 Male 940/1651 (57%) 
 Female 711/1651 (43%) 
Cardiovascular risk factors 
Hypertension 
 Present 1177/1611 (73.1%) 
 Missing n (%) 40 (2.4%) 
Diabetes 
 Present 157/1566 (10%) 
 Missing n (%) 85 (5.1%) 
Smoking 
 Present 200/1360 (14.7%) 
 Missing n (%) 201 (12.9%%) 
Hyperlipidaemia 
 Present 426/1578 (27%) 
 Missing n (%) 73 (4.4%) 
Cognitive test results 
MMSE, # (max. 30) 
 Median (IQR) 28 (27–29) 
 Missing n (%) 129 (7.8%) 
Vocabulary (MWT-B), # (max. 37) 
 Median (IQR) 32 (30–34) 
 Missing n (%) 295 (18%) 
Word recall, # (max. 10) 
 Median (IQR) 8 (6–9) 
 Missing n (%) 180 (11%) 
Animal Naming 
 Median (IQR) 24 (20–29) 
 Missing n (%) 116 (7.0%) 
TMT-A, seconds 
 Median (IQR) 38 (31–48) 
 Missing n (%) 144 (8.7%) 
TMT-B, seconds 
 Median (IQR) 83 (65–110) 
 Missing n (%) 162 (9.8%) 
History 
Diagnosed dementia 
 Present 6/1645 (0.4%) 
 Missing n (%) 6 (0.4%) 
Years of education 
 Median (IQR) 13 (12–16) 
 Missing n (%) 34 (2%) 

Data are presented as median (interquartile range) or count (percentage) of non-missing items, as appropriate. Number of percentage of missing items is reported separately.

WMH volumes (median 1.05mL, IQR 0.47mL to 2.37mL), motion estimates, and fractional occupancies of brain states 1 through 5 are reported in Table 4.

Table 4.

Structural and functional imaging characteristics

N = 1651
WMH volume1, mL 
 Total 1.05 (0.47–2.37), 9 Z 
 Periventricular 0.94 (0.43–2.04), 9 Z 
 Deep 0.10 (0.03–0.37), 344 Z 
Motion during rs-fMRI 
 Framewise displacement, mm 0.21 (0.15–0.63) 
 RMSD, mm 0.086 (0.058–0.12) 
 DVARS 27.8 (24.3–31.8) 
Fractional occupancy, % 
 DMN+ 24.8 (20.8–28.0) 
 DMN- 24.0 (20.0–28.0) 
 S3 18.4 (15.2–22.4) 
 S4 16.8 (12.8–20.8) 
 S5 15.2 (12.0–19.2) 
N = 1651
WMH volume1, mL 
 Total 1.05 (0.47–2.37), 9 Z 
 Periventricular 0.94 (0.43–2.04), 9 Z 
 Deep 0.10 (0.03–0.37), 344 Z 
Motion during rs-fMRI 
 Framewise displacement, mm 0.21 (0.15–0.63) 
 RMSD, mm 0.086 (0.058–0.12) 
 DVARS 27.8 (24.3–31.8) 
Fractional occupancy, % 
 DMN+ 24.8 (20.8–28.0) 
 DMN- 24.0 (20.0–28.0) 
 S3 18.4 (15.2–22.4) 
 S4 16.8 (12.8–20.8) 
 S5 15.2 (12.0–19.2) 

Data are presented as median (interquartile range). Supratentorial WMH volumes were obtained by semiautomatic segmentation of FLAIR images using a BINACA/LOCATE-based k-nearest neighbors algorithm and stratified by their distance to the lateral ventricles (<10mm, periventricular; >10mm, deep). Motion parameters were estimated during fMRIprep processing of BOLD scans. Fractional occupancies were calculated by assigning individual BOLD volumes to one of five discrete brain states defined by k-means clustering-based co-activation pattern analysis. Two high-occupancy states are labeled DMN+ and DMN, in view of their network connectivity profiles as shown in Figure 6.

1

Number of zero values indicated by Z.

In an outcome-neutral quality check of the implementation of (i) the MRI processing pipeline, (ii) brain state estimation, and (iii) co-activation pattern analysis, the mean difference in fractional occupancy between high- and low-occupancy states was consistently maintained, with a point-estimate of the separation between two high-occupancy and three low-occupancy states of 6.7% (95% confidence interval, 6.2% to 7.1%) in the 36p paradigm. This indicates that the implementation of the pipeline was correct and that the brain state estimation and co-activation pattern analysis worked as intended.

3.1 Pre-registered hypotheses

3.1.1 Association between WMH load and fractional occupancy

The results of the test of our primary preregistered hypothesis of an association between supratentorial WMH volume and the time spent in high-occupancy brain states are shown in Figure 3 and Table 5.

Fig. 3.

Association between time spent in high-occupancy brain states and supratentorial WMH volume. Point estimates (black line) and 95%-confidence region (light blue ribbon) of the conditional mean fractional occupancy are obtained from unadjusted beta regression modeling. Each marker represents one of N = 1642 independent participants with a non-zero total WMH volume.

Fig. 3.

Association between time spent in high-occupancy brain states and supratentorial WMH volume. Point estimates (black line) and 95%-confidence region (light blue ribbon) of the conditional mean fractional occupancy are obtained from unadjusted beta regression modeling. Each marker represents one of N = 1642 independent participants with a non-zero total WMH volume.

Close modal
Table 5.

Association between time spent in high-occupancy DMN-related brain states and WMH volume adjusted for age and sex.

EstimateP95%-CI
Intercept 0.24 <0.0001 0.21–0.27 
WMH, per 5.1-fold increase1 0.94 <0.0001 0.92–0.96 
Age, per 10 years 1.04 0.001 1.01–1.06 
Female sex 1.12 <0.0001 1.09–1.16 
1{WMH=0} 0.93 0.477 0.75–1.14 
EstimateP95%-CI
Intercept 0.24 <0.0001 0.21–0.27 
WMH, per 5.1-fold increase1 0.94 <0.0001 0.92–0.96 
Age, per 10 years 1.04 0.001 1.01–1.06 
Female sex 1.12 <0.0001 1.09–1.16 
1{WMH=0} 0.93 0.477 0.75–1.14 

Beta regression table estimated from n=1651 independent participants using the model equation FOhighlogWMH++1{WMH=0}+age+sex.

1

Interquartile ratio 2.37/0.468=5.06.

Adjusted for age and sex, there was a 0.94-fold reduction in the odds of occupying a high-occupancy brain state for every 5.1-fold increase in WMH load (P = 5.01×108).

3.1.2 Association between executive function and fractional occupancy in DMN-related states

The results of the test of our secondary preregistered hypothesis of an association between time spent in high-occupancy brain states and executive function as measured by the complete part B of the TMT are shown in Figure 4 and Table 6.

Fig. 4.

Association between time spent in high-occupancy DMN-related brain states and TMT-B completion time. Point estimates (black line) and 95%-confidence region (light blue ribbon) of the conditional mean TMT-B completion time are obtained from unadjusted Gamma regression modeling. Each marker represent one of N = 1482 independent participants with non-zero total WMH volume and available TMT-B data.

Fig. 4.

Association between time spent in high-occupancy DMN-related brain states and TMT-B completion time. Point estimates (black line) and 95%-confidence region (light blue ribbon) of the conditional mean TMT-B completion time are obtained from unadjusted Gamma regression modeling. Each marker represent one of N = 1482 independent participants with non-zero total WMH volume and available TMT-B data.

Close modal
Table 6.

Association between TMT-B and time spent in high-occupancy DMN-related brain states adjusted for age, sex, WMH volume, and years of education.

EstimateP95% CI
Intercept 53.41 <0.0001 42.7–66.8 
FOhigh, per 5% 0.98 0.0116 0.96–0.99 
WMH, per 5.1-fold increase1 1.01 0.367 0.98–1.05 
Age, per 10 years 1.18 <0.0001 1.15–1.21 
Female sex 0.99 0.666 0.95–1.03 
Education, per year 0.97 <0.0001 0.97–0.98 
1{WMH=0} 0.97 0.398 0.92–1.03 
EstimateP95% CI
Intercept 53.41 <0.0001 42.7–66.8 
FOhigh, per 5% 0.98 0.0116 0.96–0.99 
WMH, per 5.1-fold increase1 1.01 0.367 0.98–1.05 
Age, per 10 years 1.18 <0.0001 1.15–1.21 
Female sex 0.99 0.666 0.95–1.03 
Education, per year 0.97 <0.0001 0.97–0.98 
1{WMH=0} 0.97 0.398 0.92–1.03 

Gamma regression table estimated from n=1483 independent participants using the model equation TMTBFOhigh+logWMH++1{WMH=0}+age+sex+educationyears.

1

Interquartile ratio 2.37/0.468=5.06

Adjusted for age, sex, WMH volume, and years of education, there was a 0.98-fold reduction in the time to complete the TMT-B for every 5% increase in the time spent in high-occupancy brain states (P = 0.0116).

3.2 Multiverse analysis

In a multiverse analysis, the main findings of associations between WMH load and FO and, to a lesser extent, between FO and TMT-B were robust with respect to the processing choices of brain parcellation and confound regression strategy.

A nominally statistically significant negative association between the total WMH load and time spent in high-occupancy states was observed in 48 out of 81 scenarios, with 8 out of 81 significant positive associations occurring with the Desikan–Killiany parcellation only (Fig. 5A). For periventricular (deep) WMH volume, the results were similarly robust with 49 out of 81 (39/81) negative and 8 out of 81 (0/81) positive associations of nominal statistical significance, respectively (Appendix Figures 1 and 2).

Fig. 5.

Multiverse analysis. Adjusted effect size estimates of the associations between cSVD severity (WMH volume) and network dedifferentiation (less time spent in high-occupancy DMN-related brain states) (A), and between network dedifferentiation and executive function (TMT-B completion time) (B). Effect sizes are given per 5.1-fold increase in WMH volume and a 5%-increase in fractional occupancy, respectively. Markers and line segments indicate point estimates and 95%-confidence intervals for adjusted odds ratios for different combinations of confound regression strategy and brain parcellation. The primary analytical choices are indicated by dark blue circles (36p) and light blue shading (Schaefer-400). Model equations for beta and gamma regressions, respectively, are given at the top. Vertical lines indicate no effect (black) and the effect size observed in the discovery cohort (Schlemm et al., 2022) (light blue), respectively, for reference. Effect sizes not reaching nominal statistical significance (α=0.05) are desaturated. Corresponding data based on periventricular and deep WMH volumes are presented in the Appendix.

Fig. 5.

Multiverse analysis. Adjusted effect size estimates of the associations between cSVD severity (WMH volume) and network dedifferentiation (less time spent in high-occupancy DMN-related brain states) (A), and between network dedifferentiation and executive function (TMT-B completion time) (B). Effect sizes are given per 5.1-fold increase in WMH volume and a 5%-increase in fractional occupancy, respectively. Markers and line segments indicate point estimates and 95%-confidence intervals for adjusted odds ratios for different combinations of confound regression strategy and brain parcellation. The primary analytical choices are indicated by dark blue circles (36p) and light blue shading (Schaefer-400). Model equations for beta and gamma regressions, respectively, are given at the top. Vertical lines indicate no effect (black) and the effect size observed in the discovery cohort (Schlemm et al., 2022) (light blue), respectively, for reference. Effect sizes not reaching nominal statistical significance (α=0.05) are desaturated. Corresponding data based on periventricular and deep WMH volumes are presented in the Appendix.

Close modal

The secondary finding of an association between greater TMT-B times and lower fractional occupancy was less robust with only 16 out of 81 nominally statistically significant negative and no significant positive associations, irrespective of operationalization of cSVD (total vs. periventricular vs. deep WMH volume) (Fig. 5B, Appendix Figures 1 and 2).

3.3 Additional analyses

3.3.1 Connectivity profiles of brain states—relation to default mode network

Based on the cosine similarity between positive and negative activations of cluster centroids and indicator vectors of pre-defined large-scale brain networks, network activation profiles were computed for brain states estimated from Schaefer parcellations of varying spatial resolutions.

Figure 6 shows the corresponding spider plots, identifying states characterized by activation (DMN+) or suppression (DMN-) of the default mode network as states with the highest fractional occupancy.

Fig. 6.

Connectivity profiles of brain states. [Top] Centroids of each identified brain state visualized in brain space. Note the individual color scales. [Bottom] Cosine similarity between centroids of brain states and signed indicator vectors corresponding to activation (green) and suppression (red) of each of seven predefined large-scale functional brain networks (Yeo et al., 2011). States are ordered by mean fractional occupancy across N = 1651 independent participants, indicated by parenthetical percentages. Two high-occupancy states are characterized by activation or suppression of the DMN; the remaining three low-occupancy states (S3–5) were not used in the present study. Note that mean FO values are similar, but not identical, to median FO values reported in Table 4.

Fig. 6.

Connectivity profiles of brain states. [Top] Centroids of each identified brain state visualized in brain space. Note the individual color scales. [Bottom] Cosine similarity between centroids of brain states and signed indicator vectors corresponding to activation (green) and suppression (red) of each of seven predefined large-scale functional brain networks (Yeo et al., 2011). States are ordered by mean fractional occupancy across N = 1651 independent participants, indicated by parenthetical percentages. Two high-occupancy states are characterized by activation or suppression of the DMN; the remaining three low-occupancy states (S3–5) were not used in the present study. Note that mean FO values are similar, but not identical, to median FO values reported in Table 4.

Close modal

3.3.2 Association with other cognitive domains

Associations between the time spent in high-occupancy DMN-related brain states and cognitive measures beyond TMT-B are shown in Figure 7.

Fig. 7.

Association between time spent in high-occupancy DMN-related brain states and cognitive measures. Point estimates (black line) and 95%-confidence region (light blue ribbon) of the conditional mean cognitive measures are obtained from unadjusted binomial (top row: Mini-Mental State Examination, Vocabulary, Word List Recall, logit link) and Gamma regression (bottom row: Animal Naming, Trail Making Test [TMT] A/B: log link) modeling. Each marker represents one of N independent participants, as indicated. Insets report effect sizes with (adjusted [a-]) and without adjustment for the nuisance variables age, sex, WMH volume (coded as in Fig. 5), and years of education. Effect sizes were quantified as odds ratios (ORs) (top) or response scale multipliers [exp(b)] (bottom) complemented by 95%-confidence intervals, and correspond to a 20%-increase in fractional occupancy. Note the different reference change in FO compared to Table 6 chosen to adequately represent some of the smaller effect sizes. The bottom right panel highlighted in dark blue reproduces Figure 4.

Fig. 7.

Association between time spent in high-occupancy DMN-related brain states and cognitive measures. Point estimates (black line) and 95%-confidence region (light blue ribbon) of the conditional mean cognitive measures are obtained from unadjusted binomial (top row: Mini-Mental State Examination, Vocabulary, Word List Recall, logit link) and Gamma regression (bottom row: Animal Naming, Trail Making Test [TMT] A/B: log link) modeling. Each marker represents one of N independent participants, as indicated. Insets report effect sizes with (adjusted [a-]) and without adjustment for the nuisance variables age, sex, WMH volume (coded as in Fig. 5), and years of education. Effect sizes were quantified as odds ratios (ORs) (top) or response scale multipliers [exp(b)] (bottom) complemented by 95%-confidence intervals, and correspond to a 20%-increase in fractional occupancy. Note the different reference change in FO compared to Table 6 chosen to adequately represent some of the smaller effect sizes. The bottom right panel highlighted in dark blue reproduces Figure 4.

Close modal

Adjusted for age, sex, WMH load, and years of education, FO in DMN-related states appeared to be associated with better word recall (adjusted OR 1.19, nominal P 0.013), but not with global cognitive functioning (MMSE, adjusted OR 1.09) or vocabulary (aOR 1.09), nor with verbal fluency (animal naming, adjusted exp(β) 1.04), or pure processing speed (TMT-A, adjusted exp(β) 0.97).

In this pre-registered cross-sectional study, we replicated the key findings of Schlemm et al. (2022) in an independent population-based sample of 1651 middle-aged to elderly participants of the Hamburg City Health Study.

First, we confirmed that the severity of cerebral small vessel disease is associated with the time spent in high-occupancy brain states, defined by functional MRI. More precisely, we showed that every 5.1-fold increase in the volume of supratentorial white matter hyperintensities of presumed vascular origin (WMH) was associated with a 0.95-fold reduction in the odds of occupying a brain state characterized by activation or suppression of the default-mode network, at any given time during the resting-state scan.

Second, we confirmed that the time spent in high-occupancy brain states at rest is associated with cognitive performance. More precisely, a 5%-reduction in the fractional occupancy of DMN-related brain states was associated with a 1.02-fold increase in the time to complete part B of the trail making test (TMT).

In a pre-planned multiverse analysis, findings relating to our primary and, to a lesser extent, secondary hypotheses were robust with respect to variations in brain parcellations and confound regression strategies. Inconsistent results were found with the Desikan–Killiany parcellation, likely reflecting the notion that the spatial resolution and functional specificity of this coarse, structurally defined atlas are inadequate for analyzing functionally defined brain states. Across brain parcellations, effect sizes were smaller with the ICA-AROMA confound regression strategy and failed to reach nominal statistical significance. This might be due to a relatively large residual motion component in measures of dynamical functional connectivity after de-noising with ICA-AROMA, as described previously (Lydon-Staley et al., 2019).

We also confirmed across several brain parcellation resolutions that high-occupancy states at rest are characterized by either activation or suppression of the default mode network, reflecting its role as the predominant task-negative brain network.

In unplanned, exploratory analyses, we described the association between brain state dynamics and cognitive measures other than executive function and processing speed and reported a strong, preliminary association between time spent in high-occupancy states and delayed word recall.

We further explored, and report in Appendix Tables 1 and 2, the effect of motion; results relating to our primary and, to a lesser extent, secondary, hypotheses were robust to additional, unplanned adjustments for DVARS, RMSD, and mean framewise displacement.

The presented results provide robust evidence for a behaviorally relevant association between cerebral small vessel disease and functional brain network dedifferentiation.

Further research is required to replicate our findings in different populations, such as those affected more severely by cSVD or cognitive impairment, or being studied using different imaging protocols, to determine the generalizability of our findings with respect to varying operationalizations of the notions of cSVD, brain state, and cognition, and to understand the mechanisms underlying the reported associations.

Conceptualization: T.I., B.C., G.T., and E.S.; Data curation: C.M., M.P., B.M.F., and E.S.; Formal analysis: T.I., M.P., B.M.F., and E.S.; Funding acquisition: B.C., G.T., and E.S.; Investigation: T.I., C.M., U.H., C.G., B.C., G.T., and E.S.; Methodology: T.I., B.C., G.T., and E.S.; Project administration: J.F., U.H., S.K., J.G., R.T., C.G., B.C., and G.T.; Resources: J.F., J.G., R.T., C.G., B.C., and G.T.; Software: T.I., M.P., B.M.F., and E.S.; Supervision: B.C., G.T., and E.S.; Validation: T.I., M.P., and E.S.; Visualization: T.I., M.P., and E.S.; Writing—original draft: T.I. and E.S.; Writing—review & editing: T.I., C.M., M.P., B.M.F., J.F., U.H., S.K., J.G., R.T., C.G., B.C., G.T., and E.S.

The authors of this article declare that they have no financial conflict of interest with the content of this article.

This preprint was created using the LaPreprint template (https://github.com/roaldarbol/lapreprint) by Mikkel Roald-Arbøl.

Arbuthnott
,
K.
, &
Frank
,
J.
(
2000
).
Trail making test, part B as a measure of executive control: Validation using a set-switching paradigm
.
Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology
,
22
(
4
),
518
528
. https://doi.org/10.1076/1380-3395(200008)22:4;1-0;ft518
Behzadi
,
Y.
,
Restom
,
K.
,
Liau
,
J.
, &
Liu
,
T. T.
(
2007
).
A component based noise correction method (CompCor) for BOLD and perfusion based fMRI
.
Neuroimage
,
37
(
1
),
90
101
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.04.042
Bos
,
D.
,
Wolters
,
F. J.
,
Darweesh
,
S. K. L.
,
Vernooij
,
M. W.
,
de Wolf
,
F.
,
Ikram
,
M. A.
, &
Hofman
,
A.
(
2018
).
Cerebral small vessel disease and the risk of dementia: A systematic review and meta-analysis of population-based evidence
.
Alzheimer’s & Dementia
,
14
(
11
),
1482
1492
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2018.04.007
Cannistraro
,
R. J.
,
Badi
,
M.
,
Eidelman
,
B. H.
,
Dickson
,
D. W.
,
Middlebrooks
,
E. H.
, &
Meschia
,
J. F.
(
2019
).
CNS small vessel disease: A clinical review
.
Neurology
,
92
(
24
),
1146
1156
. https://doi.org/10.1212/wnl.0000000000007654
Ciric
,
R.
,
Rosen
,
A. F. G.
,
Erus
,
G.
,
Cieslak
,
M.
,
Adebimpe
,
A.
,
Cook
,
P. A.
,
Bassett
,
D. S.
,
Davatzikos
,
C.
,
Wolf
,
D. H.
, &
Satterthwaite
,
T. D.
(
2018
).
Mitigating head motion artifact in functional connectivity MRI
.
Nature Protocols
,
13
(
12
),
2801
2826
. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41596-018-0065-y
Ciric
,
R.
,
Wolf
,
D. H.
,
Power
,
J. D.
,
Roalf
,
D. R.
,
Baum
,
G. L.
,
Ruparel
,
K.
,
Shinohara
,
R. T.
,
Elliott
,
M. A.
,
Eickhoff
,
S. B.
,
Davatzikos
,
C.
,
Gur
,
R. C.
,
Gur
,
R. E.
,
Bassett
,
D. S.
, &
Satterthwaite
,
T. D.
(
2017
).
Benchmarking of participant-level confound regression strategies for the control of motion artifact in studies of functional connectivity
.
Neuroimage
,
154
,
174
187
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.03.020
Cornblath
,
E. J.
,
Ashourvan
,
A.
,
Kim
,
J. Z.
,
Betzel
,
R. F.
,
Ciric
,
R.
,
Adebimpe
,
A.
,
Baum
,
G. L.
,
He
,
X.
,
Ruparel
,
K.
,
Moore
,
T. M.
,
Gur
,
R. C.
,
Gur
,
R. E.
,
Shinohara
,
R. T.
,
Roalf
,
D. R.
,
Satterthwaite
,
T. D.
, &
Bassett
,
D. S.
(
2020
).
Temporal sequences of brain activity at rest are constrained by white matter structure and modulated by cognitive demands
.
Communications Biology
,
3
(
1
),
261
. https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-020-0961-x
Cox
,
R. W.
(
1996
).
AFNI: Software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages
.
Computers and Biomedical Research
,
29
(
3
),
162
173
. https://doi.org/10.1006/cbmr.1996.0014
Das
,
A. S.
,
Regenhardt
,
R. W.
,
Vernooij
,
M. W.
,
Blacker
,
D.
,
Charidimou
,
A.
, &
Viswanathan
,
A.
(
2019
).
Asymptomatic cerebral small vessel disease: Insights from population-based studies
.
Journal of Stroke
,
21
(
2
),
121
138
. https://doi.org/10.5853/jos.2018.03608
Desikan
,
R. S.
,
Ségonne
,
F.
,
Fischl
,
B.
,
Quinn
,
B. T.
,
Dickerson
,
B. C.
,
Blacker
,
D.
,
Buckner
,
R. L.
,
Dale
,
A. M.
,
Maguire
,
R. P.
,
Hyman
,
B. T.
,
Albert
,
M. S.
, &
Killiany
,
R. J.
(
2006
).
An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest
.
Neuroimage
,
31
(
3
),
968
980
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021
Dey
,
A. K.
,
Stamenova
,
V.
,
Turner
,
G.
,
Black
,
S. E.
, &
Levine
,
B.
(
2016
).
Pathoconnectomics of cognitive impairment in small vessel disease: A systematic review
.
Alzheimer’s Dementia
,
12
(
7
),
831
845
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2016.01.007
Esteban
,
O.
,
Markiewicz
,
C. J.
,
Blair
,
R. W.
,
Moodie
,
C. A.
,
Isik
,
A. I.
,
Erramuzpe
,
A.
,
Kent
,
J. D.
,
Goncalves
,
M.
,
DuPre
,
E.
,
Snyder
,
M.
,
Oya
,
H.
,
Ghosh
,
S. S.
,
Wright
,
J.
,
Durnez
,
J.
,
Poldrack
,
R. A.
, &
Gorgolewski
,
K. J.
(
2019
).
fMRIPrep: A robust preprocessing pipeline for functional MRI
.
Nature Methods
,
16
(
1
),
111
116
. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41592-018-0235-4
Frey
,
B. M.
,
Petersen
,
M.
,
Schlemm
,
E.
,
Mayer
,
C.
,
Hanning
,
U.
,
Engelke
,
K.
,
Fiehler
,
J.
,
Borof
,
K.
,
Jagodzinski
,
A.
,
Gerloff
,
C.
,
Thomalla
,
G.
, &
Cheng
,
B.
(
2021
).
White matter integrity and structural brain network topology in cerebral small vessel disease: The Hamburg city health study
.
Human Brain Mapping
,
42
(
5
),
1406
1415
. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.25301
Friston
,
K. J.
,
Williams
S
,
Howard
,
R.
,
Frackowiak
,
R. S.
, &
Turner
,
R.
(
1996
).
Movement-related effects in fMRI time-series
.
Magnetic Resonance in Medicine
,
35
(
3
),
346
355
. https://doi.org/10.1002/mrm.1910350312
Gesierich
,
B.
,
Tuladhar
,
A. M.
,
Telgte
,
A. T.
,
Wiegertjes
,
K.
,
Konieczny
,
M. J.
,
Finsterwalder
,
S.
,
Hübner
,
M.
,
Pirpamer
,
L.
,
Koini
,
M.
,
Abdulkadir
,
A.
,
Franzmeier
,
N.
,
Norris
,
D. G.
,
Marques
,
J. P.
,
Eulenburg
,
P. Z.
,
Ewers
,
M.
,
Schmidt
,
R.
,
de Leeuw
,
F.-E.
, &
Duering
,
M.
(
2020
).
Alterations and test-retest reliability of functional connectivity network measures in cerebral small vessel disease
.
Human Brain Mapping
,
41
(
10
),
2629
2641
. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24967
Glasser
,
M. F.
,
Coalson
,
T. S.
,
Robinson
,
E. C.
,
Hacker
,
C. D.
,
Harwell
,
J.
,
Yacoub
,
E.
,
Ugurbil
,
K.
,
Andersson
,
J.
,
Beckmann
,
C. F.
,
Jenkinson
,
M.
,
Smith
,
S. M.
, &
Van Essen
,
D. C.
(
2016
).
A multi-modal parcellation of human cerebral cortex
.
Nature
,
536
(
7615
),
171
178
. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature18933
Gordon
,
E. M.
,
Laumann
,
T. O.
,
Adeyemo
,
B.
,
Huckins
,
J. F.
,
Kelley
,
W. M.
, &
Petersen
,
S. E.
(
2016
).
Generation and evaluation of a cortical area parcellation from resting-state correlations
.
Cerebral Cortex
,
26
(
1
),
288
303
. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhu239
Griffanti
,
L.
,
Jenkinson
,
M.
,
Suri
,
S.
,
Zsoldos
,
E.
,
Mahmood
,
A.
,
Filippini
,
N.
,
Sexton
,
C. E.
,
Topiwala
,
A.
,
Allan
,
C.
,
Kivimäki
,
M.
,
Singh-Manoux
,
A.
,
Ebmeier
,
K. P.
,
Mackay
,
C. E.
, &
Zamboni
,
G.
(
2018
).
Classification and characterization of periventricular and deep white matter hyperintensities on MRI: A study in older adults
.
Neuroimage
,
170
,
174
181
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.03.024
Griffanti
,
L.
,
Zamboni
,
G.
,
Khan
,
A.
,
Li
,
L.
,
Bonifacio
,
G.
,
Sundaresan
,
V.
,
Schulz
,
U. G.
,
Kuker
,
W.
,
Battaglini
,
M.
,
Rothwell
,
P. M.
, &
Jenkinson
,
M.
(
2016
).
BIANCA (Brain Intensity AbNormality Classification Algorithm): A new tool for automated segmentation of white matter hyperintensities
.
Neuroimage
,
141
,
191
205
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.07.018
Jagodzinski
,
A.
,
Johansen
,
C.
,
Koch-Gromus
,
U.
,
Aarabi
,
G.
,
Adam
,
G.
,
Anders
,
S.
,
Augustin
,
M.
,
der Kellen
,
R. B.
,
Beikler
,
T.
,
Behrendt
,
C.-A.
,
Betz
,
C. S.
,
Bokemeyer
,
C.
,
Borof
,
K.
,
Briken
,
P.
,
Busch
,
C.-J.
,
Büchel
,
C.
,
Brassen
,
S.
,
Debus
,
E. S.
,
Eggers
,
L.
,…
Blankenberg
,
S.
(
2020
).
Rationale and design of the hamburg city health study
.
European Journal of Epidemiology
,
35
(
2
),
169
181
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10654-019-00577-4
Laumann
,
T. O.
,
Gordon
,
E. M.
,
Adeyemo
,
B.
,
Snyder
,
A. Z.
,
Joo
,
S. J.
,
Chen
,
M. Y.
,
Gilmore
,
A. W.
,
McDermott
,
K. B.
,
Nelson
,
S. M.
,
Dosenbach
,
N. U.
,
Schlaggar
,
B. L.
,
Mumford
,
J. A.
,
Poldrack
,
R. A.
, &
Petersen
,
S. E.
(
2015
).
Functional system and areal organization of a highly sampled individual human brain
.
Neuron
,
87
(
3
),
657
670
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2015.06.037
Laumann
,
T. O.
, &
Snyder
,
A. Z.
(
2021
).
Brain activity is not only for thinking
.
Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences
,
40
,
130
136
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2021.04.002
Laumann
,
T. O.
,
Snyder
,
A. Z.
,
Mitra
,
A.
,
Gordon
,
E. M.
,
Gratton
,
C.
,
Adeyemo
,
B.
,
Gilmore
,
A. W.
,
Nelson
,
S. M.
,
Berg
,
J. J.
,
Greene
,
D. J.
,
McCarthy
,
J. E.
,
Tagliazucchi
,
E.
,
Laufs
,
H.
,
Schlaggar
,
B. L.
,
Dosenbach
,
N. U. F.
, &
Petersen
,
S. E.
(
2017
).
On the stability of BOLD fMRI correlations
.
Cerebral Cortex
,
27
(
10
),
4719
4732
. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhw265
Lawrence
,
A. J.
,
Chung
,
A. W.
,
Morris
,
R. G.
,
Markus
,
H. S.
, &
Barrick
,
T. R.
(
2014
).
Structural network efficiency is associated with cognitive impairment in small-vessel disease
.
Neurology
,
83
(
4
),
304
311
. https://doi.org/10.1212/wnl.0000000000000612
Lawrence
,
A. J.
,
Tozer
,
D. J.
,
Stamatakis
,
E. A.
, &
Markus
,
H. S.
(
2018
).
A comparison of functional and tractography based networks in cerebral small vessel disease
.
Neuroimage Clinical
,
18
,
425
432
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2018.02.013
Lawrence
,
A. J.
,
Zeestraten
,
E. A.
,
Benjamin
,
P.
,
Lambert
,
C. P.
,
Morris
,
R. G.
,
Barrick
,
T. R.
, &
Markus
,
H. S.
(
2018
).
Longitudinal decline in structural networks predicts dementia in cerebral small vessel disease
.
Neurology
,
90
(
21
),
e1898
e1910
. https://doi.org/10.1212/wnl.0000000000005551
Lydon-Staley
,
D. M.
,
Ciric
,
R.
,
Satterthwaite
,
T. D.
, &
Bassett
,
D. S.
(
2019
).
Evaluation of confound regression strategies for the mitigation of micromovement artifact in studies of dynamic resting-state functional connectivity and multilayer network modularity
.
Network Neuroscience
,
3
(
2
),
427
454
. https://doi.org/10.1162/netn_a_00071
Macey
,
P. M.
,
Macey
,
K. E.
,
Kumar
,
R.
, &
Harper
,
R. M.
(
2004
).
A method for removal of global effects from fMRI time series
.
Neuroimage
,
22
(
1
),
360
366
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.12.042
Makris
,
N.
,
Goldstein
,
J. M.
,
Kennedy
,
D.
,
Hodge
,
S. M.
,
Caviness
,
V. S.
,
Faraone
,
S. V.
,
Tsuang
,
M. T.
, &
Seidman
,
L. J.
(
2006
).
Decreased volume of left and total anterior insular lobule in schizophrenia
.
Schizophrenia Research
,
83
(
2-3
),
155
171
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2005.11.020
Muschelli
,
J.
,
Nebel
,
M. B.
,
Caffo
,
B. S.
,
Barber
,
A. D.
,
Pekar
,
J. J.
, &
Mostofsky
,
S. H.
(
2014
).
Reduction of motion-related artifacts in resting state fMRI using aCompCor
.
Neuroimage
,
96
,
22
35
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.03.028
Petersen
,
M.
,
Frey
,
B. M.
,
Schlemm
,
E.
,
Mayer
,
C.
,
Hanning
,
U.
,
Engelke
,
K.
,
Fiehler
,
J.
,
Borof
,
K.
,
Jagodzinski
,
A.
,
Gerloff
,
C.
,
Thomalla
,
G.
, &
Cheng
,
B.
(
2020
).
Network localisation of white matter damage in cerebral small vessel disease
.
Scientific Reports
,
10
(
1
),
9210
. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-66013-w
Power
,
J. D.
,
Cohen
,
A. L.
,
Nelson
,
S. M.
,
Wig
,
G. S.
,
Barnes
,
K. A.
,
Church
,
J. A.
,
Vogel
,
A. C.
,
Laumann
,
T. O.
,
Miezin
,
F. M.
,
Schlaggar
,
B. L.
, &
Petersen
,
S. E.
(
2011
).
Functional network organization of the human brain
.
Neuron
,
72
(
4
),
665
678
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.006
Power
,
J. D.
,
Mitra
,
A.
,
Laumann
,
T. O.
,
Snyder
,
A. Z.
,
Schlaggar
,
B. L.
, &
Petersen
,
S. E.
(
2014
).
Methods to detect, characterize, and remove motion artifact in resting state fMRI
.
Neuroimage
,
84
,
320
341
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.048
Prins
,
N. D.
,
van Dijk
,
E. J.
,
den Heijer
,
T.
,
Vermeer
,
S. E.
,
Jolles
,
J.
,
Koudstaal
,
P. J.
,
Hofman
,
A.
, &
Breteler
,
M. M.
(
2005
).
Cerebral small-vessel disease and decline in information processing speed, executive function and memory
.
Brain
,
128
(
Pt 9
),
2034
2041
. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awh553
Pruim
,
R. H. R.
,
Mennes
,
M.
,
van Rooij
,
D.
,
Llera
,
A.
,
Buitelaar
,
J. K.
, &
Beckmann
,
C. F.
(
2015
).
ICA-AROMA: A robust ICA-based strategy for removing motion artifacts from fMRI data
.
Neuroimage
,
112
,
267
277
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.02.064
Reijmer
,
Y. D.
,
Fotiadis
,
P.
,
Piantoni
,
G.
,
Boulouis
,
G.
,
Kelly
,
K. E.
,
Gurol
,
M. E.
,
Leemans
,
A.
,
O'Sullivan
,
M. J.
,
Greenberg
,
S. M.
, &
Viswanathan
,
A.
(
2016
).
Small vessel disease and cognitive impairment: The relevance of central network connections
.
Human Brain Mapping
,
37
(
7
),
2446
2454
. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23186
Rimmele
,
D. L.
,
Petersen
,
E. L.
,
Schlemm
,
E.
,
Kessner
,
S. S.
,
Petersen
,
M.
,
Mayer
,
C.
,
Cheng
,
B.
,
Zeller
,
T.
,
Waldeyer
,
C.
,
Behrendt
,
C.-A.
,
Gerloff
,
C.
, &
Thomalla
,
G.
(
2022
).
Association of carotid plaque and flow velocity with white matter integrity in a middle-aged to elderly population
.
Neurology
,
99
(
24
),
e2699
e2707
. https://doi.org/10.1212/wnl.0000000000201297
Satterthwaite
,
T. D.
,
Elliott
,
M. A.
,
Gerraty
,
R. T.
,
Ruparel
,
K.
,
Loughead
,
J.
,
Calkins
,
M. E.
,
Eickhoff
,
S. B.
,
Hakonarson
,
H.
,
Gur
,
R. C.
,
Gur
,
R. E.
, &
Wolf
,
D. H.
(
2013
).
An improved framework for confound regression and filtering for control of motion artifact in the preprocessing of resting-state functional connectivity data
.
Neuroimage
,
64
,
240
256
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.08.052
Schaefer
,
A.
,
Kong
,
R.
,
Gordon
,
E. M.
,
Laumann
,
T. O.
,
Zuo
,
X.-N.
,
Holmes
,
A. J.
,
Eickhoff
,
S. B.
, &
Yeo
,
B. T. T.
(
2018
).
Local-global parcellation of the human cerebral cortex from intrinsic functional connectivity MRI
.
Cerebral Cortex
,
28
(
9
),
3095
3114
. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhx179
Schlemm
,
E.
,
Frey
,
B. M.
,
Mayer
,
C.
,
Petersen
,
M.
,
Fiehler
,
J.
,
Hanning
,
U.
,
Kühn
,
S.
,
Twerenbold
,
R.
,
Gallinat
,
J.
,
Gerloff
,
C.
,
Thomalla
,
G.
, &
Cheng
,
B.
(
2022
).
Equalization of brain state occupancy accompanies cognitive impairment in cerebral small vessel disease
.
Biological Psychiatry
,
92
(
7
),
592
602
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2022.03.019
Schulz
,
M.
,
Malherbe
,
C.
,
Cheng
,
B.
,
Thomalla
,
G.
, &
Schlemm
,
E.
(
2021
).
Functional connectivity changes in cerebral small vessel disease—A systematic review of the resting-state MRI literature
.
BMC Medicine
,
19
(
1
),
103
. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12916-021-01962-1
Shen
,
J.
,
Tozer
,
D. J.
,
Markus
,
H. S.
, &
Tay
,
J.
(
2020
).
Network efficiency mediates the relationship between vascular burden and cognitive impairment: A diffusion tensor imaging study in UK Biobank
.
Stroke
,
51
(
6
),
1682
1689
. https://doi.org/10.1161/strokeaha.119.028587
Steegen
,
S.
,
Tuerlinckx
,
F.
,
Gelman
,
A.
, &
Vanpaemel
,
W.
(
2016
).
Increasing transparency through a multiverse analysis
.
Perspectives on Psychological Science
,
11
(
5
),
702
712
. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691616658637
Sundaresan
,
V.
,
Zamboni
,
G.
,
Le Heron
,
C.
,
Rothwell
,
P. M.
,
Husain
,
M.
,
Battaglini
,
M.
,
De Stefano
,
N.
,
Jenkinson
,
M.
, &
Griffanti
,
L.
(
2019
).
Automated lesion segmentation with BIANCA: Impact of population-level features, classification algorithm and locally adaptive thresholding
.
Neuroimage
,
202
,
116056
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116056
Tombaugh
,
T. N.
(
2004
).
Trail making test A and B: Normative data stratified by age and education
.
Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology
,
19
(
2
),
203
214
. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0887-6177(03)00039-8
Tuladhar
,
A. M.
,
Tay
,
J.
,
van Leijsen
,
E.
,
Lawrence
,
A. J.
,
van Uden
,
I. W. M.
,
Bergkamp
,
M.
,
van der Holst
,
E.
,
Kessels
,
R. P. C.
,
Norris
,
D.
,
Markus
,
H. S.
, &
De Leeuw
,
F. E.
(
2020
).
Structural network changes in cerebral small vessel disease
.
Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry
,
91
(
2
),
196
203
. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp-2019-321767
Tuladhar
,
A. M.
,
van Dijk
,
E.
,
Zwiers
,
M. P.
,
van Norden
,
A. G.
,
de Laat
,
K. F.
,
Shumskaya
,
E.
,
Norris
,
D. G.
, &
de Leeuw
,
F. E.
(
2016
).
Structural network connectivity and cognition in cerebral small vessel disease
.
Human Brain Mapping
,
37
(
1
),
300
310
. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23032
Tzourio-Mazoyer
,
N.
,
Landeau
,
B.
,
Papathanassiou
,
D.
,
Crivello
,
F.
,
Etard
,
O.
,
Delcroix
,
N.
,
Mazoyer
,
B.
, &
Joliot
,
M.
(
2002
).
Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain
.
Neuroimage
,
15
(
1
),
273
289
. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2001.0978
Vidaurre
,
D.
,
Abeysuriya
,
R.
,
Becker
,
R.
,
Quinn
,
A. J.
,
Alfaro-Almagro
,
F.
,
Smith
,
S. M.
, &
Woolrich
,
M. W.
(
2018
).
Discovering dynamic brain networks from big data in rest and task
.
Neuroimage
,
180
(
Pt B
),
646
656
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.06.077
Wardlaw
,
J. M.
,
Smith
,
C.
, &
Dichgans
,
M.
(
2013
).
Mechanisms of sporadic cerebral small vessel disease: Insights from neuroimaging
.
Lancet Neurology
,
12
(
5
),
483
497
. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1474-4422(13)70060-7
Wardlaw
,
J. M.
,
Smith
,
E. E.
,
Biessels
,
G. J.
,
Cordonnier
,
C.
,
Fazekas
,
F.
,
Frayne
,
R.
,
Lindley
,
R. I.
,
O'Brien
,
J. T.
,
Barkhof
,
F.
,
Benavente
,
O. R.
,
Black
,
S. E.
,
Brayne
,
C.
,
Breteler
,
M.
,
Chabriat
,
H.
,
Decarli
,
C.
,
de Leeuw
,
F. E.
,
Doubal
,
F.
,
Duering
,
M.
,
Fox
,
N. C.
,… STandards for ReportIng Vascular changes on nEuroimaging (STRIVE v1)
(
2013
).
Neuroimaging standards for research into small vessel disease and its contribution to ageing and neurodegeneration
.
Lancet Neurology
,
12
(
8
),
822
838
. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1474-4422(13)70124-8
Wardlaw
,
J. M.
,
Hernández
,
M. C. V.
, &
Muñoz-Maniega
,
S.
(
2015
).
What are white matter hyperintensities made of? Relevance to vascular cognitive impairment
.
Journal of the American Heart Association
,
4
(
6
),
001140
. https://doi.org/10.1161/jaha.114.001140
Xu
,
Y.
,
Shang
,
H.
,
Lu
,
H.
,
Zhang
,
J.
,
Yao
,
L.
, &
Long
,
Z.
(
2021
).
Altered dynamic functional connectivity in subcortical ischemic vascular disease with cognitive impairment
.
Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience
,
13
,
758137
. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2021.758137
Yeo
,
B. T.
,
Krienen
,
F. M.
,
Sepulcre
,
J.
,
Sabuncu
,
M. R.
,
Lashkari
,
D.
,
Hollinshead
,
M.
,
Roffman
,
J. L.
,
Smoller
,
J. W.
,
Zöllei
,
L.
,
Polimeni
,
J. R.
,
Fischl
,
B.
,
Liu
,
H.
, &
Buckner
,
R. L.
(
2011
).
The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity
.
Journal of Neurophysiology
,
106
(
3
),
1125
1165
. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00338.2011
Yin
,
W.
,
Zhou
,
X.
,
Li
,
C.
,
You
,
M.
,
Wan
,
K.
,
Zhang
,
W.
,
Zhu
,
W.
,
Li
,
M.
,
Zhu
,
X.
,
Qian
,
Y.
, &
Sun
,
Z.
(
2022
).
The clustering analysis of time properties in patients with cerebral small vessel disease: A dynamic connectivity study
.
Frontiers in Neurology
,
13
,
913241
. https://doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2022.913241

A1.1 Deep and periventricular WMH

Here we present, in analogy to Figure 5, the results of the multiverse analyses of the association between cSVD burden, FO of DMN-related states, and executive function, when cSVD is operationalized as the volume of deep or periventricular white matter hyperintensities, respectively.

Appendix Fig. 1.

Multiverse analysis. Adjusted effect size estimates of the associations between cSVD severity (deep WMH volume) and network dedifferentiation (less time spent in high-occupancy DMN-related brain states) (A), and between network dedifferentiation and executive function (TMT-B completion time) (B). For details, see caption to Fig. 5.

Appendix Fig. 1.

Multiverse analysis. Adjusted effect size estimates of the associations between cSVD severity (deep WMH volume) and network dedifferentiation (less time spent in high-occupancy DMN-related brain states) (A), and between network dedifferentiation and executive function (TMT-B completion time) (B). For details, see caption to Fig. 5.

Close modal
Appendix Fig. 2.

Multiverse analysis. Adjusted effect size estimates of the associations between cSVD severity (periventricular WMH volume) and network dedifferentiation (less time spent in high-occupancy DMN-related brain states) (A), and between network dedifferentiation and executive function (TMT-B completion time) (B). For details, see caption to Fig. 5.

Appendix Fig. 2.

Multiverse analysis. Adjusted effect size estimates of the associations between cSVD severity (periventricular WMH volume) and network dedifferentiation (less time spent in high-occupancy DMN-related brain states) (A), and between network dedifferentiation and executive function (TMT-B completion time) (B). For details, see caption to Fig. 5.

Close modal

A1.2 Motion parameters

We also present, in analogy to Tables 5 and 6, regression tables for the association between time spent in DMN-related brain states (FO) and WMH volume, and between TMT-B and FO, adjusted for DVARS, RSMD, and framewise displacement, in addition to age, sex, and, in the latter case, years of education.

Appendix Table 1.

Association between time spent in high-occupancy DMN-related brain states and WMH volume adjusted for age, sex, and motion parameters.

EstimateP95%-CI
Intercept 0.32 <0.0001 0.28–0.36 
WMH, per 5.1-fold increase1 0.96 0.0004 0.94–0.98 
Age, per 10 years 1.01 <0.0001 1.00–1.01 
Female sex 1.11 <0.0001 1.08–1.15 
1{WMH=0} 0.91 0.3552 0.74–1.11 
DVARS 0.98 <0.0001 0.98–0.99 
RMSD 28.29 0.0055 2.67–299.8 
Framewise displacement 0.16 0.0112 0.04–0.66 
EstimateP95%-CI
Intercept 0.32 <0.0001 0.28–0.36 
WMH, per 5.1-fold increase1 0.96 0.0004 0.94–0.98 
Age, per 10 years 1.01 <0.0001 1.00–1.01 
Female sex 1.11 <0.0001 1.08–1.15 
1{WMH=0} 0.91 0.3552 0.74–1.11 
DVARS 0.98 <0.0001 0.98–0.99 
RMSD 28.29 0.0055 2.67–299.8 
Framewise displacement 0.16 0.0112 0.04–0.66 
1

Interquartile ratio 2.37/0.468=5.06.

Appendix Table 2.

Association between TMT-B and time spent in high-occupancy DMN-related brain states adjusted for age, sex, WMH volume and years of education, and motion parameters.

EstimateP95%-CI
Intercept 46.83 <0.0001 36.74–59.72 
FOhigh, per 5% 0.71 0.0718 0.49–1.03 
WMH, per 5.1-fold increase1 1.01 0.3414 0.98–1.04 
Age, per 10 years 1.02 <0.0001 1.01–1.02 
Female sex 1.00 0.8171 0.96–1.04 
Education, per year 0.97 <0.0001 0.97–0.98 
1{WMH=0} 0.96 0.7581 0.73–1.29 
DVARS 1.01 0.0001 1.00–1.01 
RMSD 0.31 0.4695 0.01–7.45 
Framewise displacement 1.08 0.9322 0.16–7.13 
EstimateP95%-CI
Intercept 46.83 <0.0001 36.74–59.72 
FOhigh, per 5% 0.71 0.0718 0.49–1.03 
WMH, per 5.1-fold increase1 1.01 0.3414 0.98–1.04 
Age, per 10 years 1.02 <0.0001 1.01–1.02 
Female sex 1.00 0.8171 0.96–1.04 
Education, per year 0.97 <0.0001 0.97–0.98 
1{WMH=0} 0.96 0.7581 0.73–1.29 
DVARS 1.01 0.0001 1.00–1.01 
RMSD 0.31 0.4695 0.01–7.45 
Framewise displacement 1.08 0.9322 0.16–7.13 
1

Interquartile ratio 2.37/0.468=5.06.

Author notes

Reviewed and recommended by Peer Review Community in Registered Reports (PCI RR)

Stage 1 received by PCI RR: November 22 2022; Stage 1 recommendation by PCI RR: June 27 2023; Stage 2 received by PCI RR: October 17 2023; Stage 2 recommendation by PCI RR: February 05 2024

Stage 1 recommendation: https://rr.peercommunityin.org/articles/rec?id=341

Stage 2 recommendation: https://rr.peercommunityin.org/articles/rec?id=576

OSF registration: https://osf.io/9yhzc

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International (CC BY 4.0) license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. For a full description of the license, please visit https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/legalcode.