The default mode (DMN), frontoparietal (FPN), and salience (SN) networks interact to support a range of behaviors, are vulnerable to environmental insults, and are disrupted in neurodevelopmental disorders. However, their development across the third trimester and perinatal transition remains unknown. Employing resting-state functional MRI at 30 to 32, 34 to 36, and 40 to 44 weeks postmenstrual age (PMA), we examined developmental trajectories of the intra- and internetwork connectivity of the 3 networks measured in 84 fetuses and neonates. A secondary analysis addressed the impact of maternal mental health on these networks. The DMN, FPN, and SN intranetwork connectivity evidenced significant increases between 36 and 44 weeks PMA, with connectivity measures reaching values significantly greater than 0 at 40 weeks PMA for all 3 networks. Connectivity between SN and DMN and between SN and FPN decreased significantly with the connectivity values significantly below 0 at 36–44 weeks. However, DMN-FPN connectivity increased between 30 and 44 weeks with the connectivity greater than 0 already at 36 months. Finally, higher maternal stress levels negatively affected the SN across 30-44 weeks PMA. These data provide a normative framework to compare fetuses and neonates at risk for neurobehavioral disorders and assess the impact of the environment on the developing brain.

The human brain is divided into functional networks, or collections of regions showing strong synchronization of neural activity. How these networks mature during the pre- to postnatal transition remains largely unknown (Kim et al., 2022, 2023; Scheinost et al., 2021; Thomason, 2020). Mapping the developmental trajectories of the networks is, therefore, critical for advancing understanding of both typical and atypical brain development and informing how prenatal exposures impact the developing brain (Karolis et al., 2023; Kim et al., 2022; Sylvester et al., 2023). In the present study, we demonstrate developmental trajectories of three large-scale cortical networks that support a wide range of behaviors including social cognition (Supekar et al., 2010; Uddin, 2015; Uddin et al., 2011), the default mode (DMN), frontoparietal (FPN), and salience networks (SN), from 30 to 44 weeks of gestation in typically developing participants using a combined prospective longitudinal and cross-sectional design.

Composed of the medial prefrontal cortex, posterior cingulate cortex, and bilateral angular gyri, the DMN attenuates activity during some cognitive tasks but shows positive activation during those involving social processes including monitoring of one’s own mental state and predicting behaviors of others (Andrews-Hanna et al., 2014; Buckner & DiNicola, 2019; Padmanabhan et al., 2017; Smallwood et al., 2021; Yeshurun et al., 2021). The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and intraparietal sulcus comprise the FPN (Menon, 2011; Menon & Uddin, 2010; Uddin et al., 2011), the network involved in the conscious decision-making required in goal-directed behaviors, inhibition, and self-reasoning. Finally, the salience network (SN) comprises the anterior cingulate cortex and the bilateral anterior insulae. The SN links other cortical networks with the limbic and autonomic systems, and is involved in detecting and orienting to salient external and internal stimuli, including those that are social in nature (Seeley et al., 2007; Uddin, 2015). Notably, the anterior insula has recently been recognized as a “gatekeeper of executive control” (Molnar-Szakacs & Uddin, 2022), initiating dynamic switching between large-scale neurocognitive networks, including the DMN and FPN (Menon & Uddin, 2010; Shaw et al., 2021; Snyder et al., 2021; Sridharan et al., 2008).

Studies addressing both intra- and internetwork connectivity in the developing brain before term equivalent age have relied upon both fetal studies and those of preterm neonates (De Asis-Cruz et al., 2021; De Asis-Cruz, Kapse, et al., 2020; Doria et al., 2010; Hu et al., 2022; Omidvarniaet al., 2014; Thomason et al., 2013, 2014, 2015; van den Heuvel & Thomason, 2016). Cross-sectional studies of fetuses throughout the second and third trimesters of gestation demonstrate that large scale networks develop in the prenatal period and inter- and intrahemispheric connectivity increase with advancing postmenstrual age (Thomason, 2020). Fetal studies suggest that the fetal brain is organized with adult network properties (Thomason et al., 2014; van den Heuvel & Thomason, 2016), with a reported 61.66% overlap in the overall brain connectome structure in fetuses and adults during the second and third trimesters of gestation (Turk et al., 2019).

Given their role for supporting many social, cognitive, and affective behaviors (De Asis-Cruz & Limperopoulos, 2023; Marshall et al., 2020; Sigar et al., 2023; Thomason, 2020; Uddin et al., 2010), there is great interest in mapping the emergence of the DMN, FPN, and SN networks in typically developing fetuses and neonates (Canini et al., 2020; De Asis-Cruz et al., 2021; Thomason, 2020; Turk et al., 2019; van den Heuvel & Thomason, 2016). Functional connections between the nodes in the DMN, FPN, and SN begin to emerge during the late second and third trimesters, with relatively lower connectivity between the medial posterior and anterior nodes of the DMN (Karolis et al., 2023). At birth, the DMN, FPN, and SN are considered to be topologically incomplete (Eyre et al., 2021; Gao et al., 2009, 2011, 2013, 2015; Smyser et al., 2011), with significant intranetwork growth occurring across the first postnatal year. Nevertheless, functional connectivity patterns in the DMN, FPN, and SN in neonates are moderately to highly correlated with patterns observed in older children and adults, although magnitudes of connectivity values are generally lower in infants compared with adults (Kim et al., 2022, 2023; Sylvester et al., 2023). In addition, long-range connectivity from clusters in the posterior DMN and FPN to anterior clusters from the same networks is also lower in neonates than in typically developing children and adults (Sylvester et al., 2023). While evidence regarding the three networks in fetuses and neonates is emerging, studies mapping intra- and internetwork connectivity of the DMN, FPN, and SN spanning the important fetal to neonatal transition in normative development are not yet readily available (Thomason, 2020).

Finally, while development of functional networks depends on numerous factors, emerging data suggest that prenatal exposure to maternal mental health challenges may also contribute variations in functional connectivity (Dufford et al., 2021). Recent studies are beginning to investigate the impact of prenatal exposure to maternal mental health challenges on the DMN, FPN, and SN. For example, higher levels of maternal stress are associated with increased fetal FPN connectivity (Thomason et al., 2021). In contrast, maternal/fetal residence in high crime neighborhoods decreases connectivity within the neonatal DMN (Brady et al., 2022). None, however, has examined connectivity across the perinatal transition.

The present study fills existing knowledge gaps by investigating development of intra- and internetwork functional connectivity in the DMN, FPN, and SN across the third trimester of gestation and the first 4 postnatal weeks in a longitudinal and cross-sectional sample of 84 fetuses and neonates scanned between 30 weeks and 44 weeks PMA. We also assessed the impact of prenatal maternal mental health on connectivity in the three networks.

This work includes longitudinal and cross-sectional imaging data from two Yale School of Medicine studies and an open-source dataset obtained from the Developing Human Connectome Project (dHCP) (Eyre et al., 2021). The Yale University institutional review board approved all studies. Parent(s) provided written consent. The Developing Human Connectome (dHCP) project was reviewed and approved by the UK National Research Ethics Authority. DHCP study investigators obtained written informed parental consent. An overview of the composition of the study cohorts is presented in Table 1.

Table 1.

Sample characteristics.

Fetal-neonatal cohortNeonatal cohortdHCPComparison p-value
Number 29 31 24  
Number of females (%) 13 (44.8) 19 (61.2) 9 (37.5) .188 
Birth weight (g) 3646 +/- 640 3510 +/- 377 3438 +/- 423 .296 
PMA at birth (wks) 39.4 +/- 1.5 39.9 +/- 0.8 39.4 +/- 1.6 .232 
Number of SGA (%) 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) n/a 
Race    .403^ 
 Asian 1 (3.4) 3 (9.7) 0 (0.0)  
 Black–African American 4 (13.8) 1 (3.2) 0 (0.0)  
 Native American 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0)  
 White 19 (65.5) 21 (67.7) 0 (0.0)  
 More than 1 race 5 (17.2) 6 (19.4) 0 (0.0)  
 Unknown/not reported 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 38 (100.0)  
Ethnicity    .505^ 
 Hispanic–Latinx 5 (17.2) 4 (12.9) 0 (0.0)  
 Unknown/not reported 1 (3.4) 0 (0.0) 38 (100.0)  
PMA at scan (wks)     
 Scan 1 (F1) 31.2 +/- 0.7 N/A N/A  
 Scan 2 (F2) 35.3 +/- 0.8 N/A N/A  
 Scan 3 (NN) 43.4 +/- 1.3 44.4 +/- 1.3 40.2 +/- 2.1 <.001 
Maternal education    .845^ 
 <High school 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0)  
 High school graduate 0 (0.0) 1 (3.2) 0 (0.0)  
 Some college 2 (6.9) 1 (3.2) 0 (0.0)  
 College graduate 25 (86.2) 27 (87.1) 0 (0.0)  
 Unknown/not reported 2 (6.9) 2 (6.5) 38 (100.0)  
Fetal-neonatal cohortNeonatal cohortdHCPComparison p-value
Number 29 31 24  
Number of females (%) 13 (44.8) 19 (61.2) 9 (37.5) .188 
Birth weight (g) 3646 +/- 640 3510 +/- 377 3438 +/- 423 .296 
PMA at birth (wks) 39.4 +/- 1.5 39.9 +/- 0.8 39.4 +/- 1.6 .232 
Number of SGA (%) 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) n/a 
Race    .403^ 
 Asian 1 (3.4) 3 (9.7) 0 (0.0)  
 Black–African American 4 (13.8) 1 (3.2) 0 (0.0)  
 Native American 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0)  
 White 19 (65.5) 21 (67.7) 0 (0.0)  
 More than 1 race 5 (17.2) 6 (19.4) 0 (0.0)  
 Unknown/not reported 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 38 (100.0)  
Ethnicity    .505^ 
 Hispanic–Latinx 5 (17.2) 4 (12.9) 0 (0.0)  
 Unknown/not reported 1 (3.4) 0 (0.0) 38 (100.0)  
PMA at scan (wks)     
 Scan 1 (F1) 31.2 +/- 0.7 N/A N/A  
 Scan 2 (F2) 35.3 +/- 0.8 N/A N/A  
 Scan 3 (NN) 43.4 +/- 1.3 44.4 +/- 1.3 40.2 +/- 2.1 <.001 
Maternal education    .845^ 
 <High school 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0)  
 High school graduate 0 (0.0) 1 (3.2) 0 (0.0)  
 Some college 2 (6.9) 1 (3.2) 0 (0.0)  
 College graduate 25 (86.2) 27 (87.1) 0 (0.0)  
 Unknown/not reported 2 (6.9) 2 (6.5) 38 (100.0)  

Legend: PMA=postmenstrual age.

Values are +/- SD.

Comparison p-value represents significance level of comparison between cohorts: ANOVA for continuous variables (e.g., birth weight) and chi-squared tests for categorical variables (e.g., race). Bold text indicates p < .05. ^p-values do not include dHCP cohort (data not available).

2.1 Participants

2.1.1 Yale fetal to neonatal cohort

The Yale cohort consists of longitudinal data collected from the third trimester to the neonatal period as part of the Yale Autism Center of Excellence Program Project. Between April 09, 2018, and April 13, 2022, 33 pregnant women were recruited for scanning at 30–33 weeks and 34–36 weeks postmenstrual age (PMA). Three did not have usable data from either fetal scan (F1 or F2), and one fetus-to-neonate participant was small for gestational age. Table 1 shows the sample characteristics for the 29 fetuses with usable data and appropriate for gestational age status. Mothers who participated in the fetal scans were invited to have their infants participate in the neonatal functional connectivity protocol with repeat MRI within the first 6 weeks of life. Usable longitudinal data were available for all 29 infants, with at least two out of three (two fetal and one neonatal) scans per participant. Out of 29 participants, 48.3% were scanned before the COVID-19 pandemic, with the remaining 51.7% scanned during the pandemic. Inclusion criteria for fetuses included pregnant mothers with no family history of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) in the first degree relatives who met the following criteria: 1) 26–28 weeks gestational age; 2) estimated fetal weight, femur length, and biparietal diameter appropriate for gestational age (Hadlock et al., 1990, 1991); 3) no known chromosome or structural abnormalities; 4) no brain injury on clinical ultrasound; 5) no congenital infections; 6) no known ethanol exposure since confirmation of pregnancy; 7) no known tobacco/nicotine use since confirmation of pregnancy; 8) no known illicit drug use/abuse; 9) singleton pregnancy; 10) receiving regular prenatal care; 11) maternal age 21-35 years; 12) no morbid obesity; and 13) mother able to give permission. Exclusion criteria included 1) any contraindications for MRI scanning; 2) too large to fit in the MRI machine comfortably for 45 minutes; 3) a clinically ordered MRI scheduled in the future; 4) difficulty lying down and remaining still for 45–60 minutes; 5) current drug, alcohol, and tobacco/nicotine use; and 6) preterm birth (born before 37 weeks gestation; for neonates). Drug screening (Integrated E-Z Split Key Cup II, Alere San Diego, Inc, San Diego, CA 92121) and alcohol and tobacco screening tools were administered to all mothers at the time of each fetal MRI study using the Fagerstrom Test for Nicotine Dependence and the Alcohol Timeline Followback assessments (Sobell & Sobell, 1995; Weinberger et al., 2007).

2.1.2 Yale neonatal cohort

Thirty-five healthy term infants born between 37 and 41 weeks of PMA were scanned within the first 6 weeks of their participation in the Yale Autism Center of Excellence Program Project. The scans were completed between April 30, 2018, and December 13, 2021. Four infants awoke before adequate functional data could be collected; thus, usable data are available for 31 of these subjects (Table 1). Inclusion criteria included singleton pregnancy, term birth (born after 37 weeks gestation), and appropriate for gestational age. Exclusion criteria included 1) congenital infections, 2) nonfebrile seizure disorder, 3) hearing loss, 4) visual impairment, 5) the presence of any known chromosomal abnormality, 6) prenatal exposure to illicit drugs, 7) major psychotic disorder in first degree relatives, and 8) contraindications to MRI including nonremovable metal medical implants (i.e., patent ductus arteriosus clip). Like the fetal cohort, only infants with no family history of autism or other neurodevelopmental disorders are included in this analysis. Please see Table 1 for sample characteristics.

2.1.3 dHCP cohort

The data from 38 term-born infants prospectively recruited as part of the dHCP, an observational, cross-sectional Open Science program approved by the UK National Research Ethics Authority, were included in these analyses. We used the first data release of the dHCP, which was the current data release at the start of this project. Infants were recruited from the postnatal wards and scanned at 37–43.5 weeks PMA. dHCP exclusion criteria include a history of severe compromise at birth requiring prolonged resuscitation, a diagnosed chromosomal abnormality, or any contraindication to MRI scanning. Additional exclusion criteria imposed for this analysis included the need for the infant’s birth weight to be appropriate for gestational age (i.e., between 10th and 90th percentile). A review of birth weight data for the 35 infants using UK-WHO growth charts for ages 0-4 years revealed that 14 of 38 were small for gestational age (less than 10th percentile). Thus, data from the 24 dHCP neonates are included in this analysis. Please see Table 1 for sample characteristics. Notably, race, ethnicity, and maternal education data were not available for the dHCP sample.

2.2 Maternal mental health: impact on connectivity

Maternal mental health data were collected at the first fetal scan for subjects in the Yale longitudinal analysis. Depression symptoms were quantified using the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS; a 10-item measure of perinatal depression, yielding possible scores ranging from 0 to 30) (Cox et al., 1987). Anxiety was measured using the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI; a 40-item instrument measuring state and trait anxiety total scores, ranging from 20 to 80) (Spielberger et al., 1983). Forty-two stress symptoms were indexed by the Perceived Stress Scale (PSS-14; a 14-item measure of perceived stress with scores ranging from 0 to 56) (Cohen et al., 1983). For all three measures, higher scores indicate the presence of more symptoms. The composite maternal mental health index was obtained by scaling and centering the original four variables, conducting principal components analysis (PCA), and using the first principal component (PC1) in the subsequent analyses. PC1 explains 73.0% of the total variance in the original data using a single variable, with loadings (eigenvectors) of 0.50, 0.46, 0.51, and 0.52 for EPDS, PSS, STAI State, and STAI Trait, respectively.

2.3 Imaging acquisition

2.3.1 Fetal-neonatal cohort

All MRIs were performed in a natural, unmedicated state. Fetuses were studied using repeat MRI protocols completed in <60 minutes using a 3 Tesla Siemens (Erlangen, Germany) Prisma MR system and a flexible, lightweight (~1 lb) cardiac 32-channel body coil. Five functional runs were acquired (TR = 1950 ms, TE = 21 ms, FoV = 320 mm, flip angle 90°, matrix size 94 x 94, SAR<0.4, slice thickness 3 mm, Bandwidth = 2215 Hz/pixel, 32 slices). Each of the five functional runs comprised 150 volumes (5.85 minutes). On average, 520 frames (range: 300 - 750) were retained for analysis or more than 17 minutes of data per participant. Follow-up neonatal MRI in this cohort occurred as part of a natural-sleep “feed and wrap” protocol. Infants were fed, bundled with multiple levels of ear protection, and immobilized in an MRI-safe vacuum swaddler. Heart rate and O2 saturation were continuously monitored during all scans. The same 3 Tesla Siemens (Erlangen, Germany) Prisma MR system employed for fetal imaging was also used for the neonatal data. Functional images were collected using an echo-planar image gradient echo pulse sequence (TR = 2120 ms, TE = 22 ms, FoV = 260 mm, matrix size = 102 ×102, slice thickness = 3 mm, flip angle = 90°, Bandwidth = 2335 Hz/pixel, 32 slices). Functional runs consisted of 360 volumes (6.18 minutes).

2.3.2 Neonatal cohort

Neonatal imaging was performed on the same 3 Tesla Siemens (Erlangen, Germany) Prisma MR system using a 32-channel parallel receiver head coil and the same “feed and wrap” MRI protocol as above. We collected five functional runs, each comprising 360 volumes. On average, 682 frames (range 285–750) were retained for analysis, and each neonate had an average of 11.5 minutes (SD = 1.4) of usable, functional data.

2.3.3 dHCP cohort

Imaging was acquired at the Evelina Newborn Imaging Centre, Evelina London Children’s Hospital, using a 3 T Philips Achieva system (Philips Medical Systems). All infants were scanned without sedation in a scanner environment, including a dedicated transport system, positioning device, and a customized 32-channel receiver coil with a custom-made acoustic hood. MRI-compatible ear putty and earmuffs were used to provide additional acoustic noise attenuation, and infants were fed, swaddled, and positioned in a vacuum jack before scanning to provide natural sleep (Eyre et al., 2021). High temporal resolution multiband EPI (TE = 38 ms; TR = 392 ms; MB factor = 9x; 2.15 mm isotropic) specifically developed for neonates was acquired for 15 minutes.

2.4 Image processing

2.4.1 Fetal connectivity preprocessing

Functional data were processed using validated fetal fMRI pipelines (Rutherford et al., 2022; Scheinost et al., 2018). Functional data were corrected for motion using a two-pass registration approach optimized for fetuses to correct for large and small head movements (Scheinost et al., 2018). Outlying frames were censored for data quality based on the signal-to-noise ratio within the fetal brain, the final weighted correlation value from optimization, and the frame-to-frame motion between adjacent frames. These frames were defined as frames with SNR, registration quality, or motion greater/less than 1 standard deviation above/below the mean values over all runs.

As in prior work (Thomason et al., 2017), several covariates of no interest were regressed from the data, including linear and quadratic drifts, six motion parameters, the mean cerebral-spinal-fluid (CSF) signal, the mean white matter signal, and the mean gray matter signal. The data were temporally smoothed with a zero mean unit variance Gaussian filter (approximate cutoff frequency = 0.12 Hz). A gray matter mask defined in template space was applied to the data, so only gray matter voxels were used in further calculations.

Next, to warp the network seeds from MNI space to fMRI space, a series of nonlinear registrations were calculated independently and combined into a single transform. This single transformation allows the seeds to be transformed into a single participant’s space with only one transformation, reducing interpolation error. First, the mean functional image from the motion-corrected fMRI data was registered to an age-appropriate template (i.e., 31 weeks or 34 weeks gestation) (Gholipour et al., 2017) using a low-resolution nonlinear registration. These age-appropriate fetal templates were nonlinearly registered to MNI space.

2.4.2 Infant connectivity preprocessing

Functional data for infants were processed using a previously validated pipeline (Kwon et al., 2014, 2015). Functional images were motion corrected using SPM8. Next, images were iteratively smoothed until the smoothness of any image had a full-width half maximum of approximately 6 mm using AFNI’s 3dBlurToFWHM. This iterative smoothing reduces motion-related confounds (Scheinost et al., 2014). All further analyses were performed using BioImage Suite (Joshi et al., 2011) unless otherwise specified. Several covariates of no interest were regressed from the data, including linear and quadratic drifts, mean cerebral-spinal-fluid (CSF) signal, mean white matter signal, and mean gray matter signal. For additional control of possible motion-related confounds, a 24-parameter motion model (including 6 rigid-body motion parameters, 6 temporal derivatives, and these terms squared) was regressed from the data. The data were temporally smoothed with a Gaussian filter (approximate cutoff frequency = 0.12 Hz).

Next, to warp the network seeds from MNI space to fMRI space, a series of nonlinear registrations were calculated independently and combined into a single transformation. First, the mean functional image from the motion-corrected fMRI data was registered to a custom infant template (as in Scheinost, Kwon, Shen, et al. (2016)) using a previously validated algorithm (Scheinost et al., 2017). Similarly, the same algorithm registered the infant template to the MNI brain.

2.4.3 Seed intranetwork and internetwork connectivity

After the seeds for each of the three networks (i.e., DMN, FPN, and SN; see Fig. 1 for network seeds and Table S1 for network node coordinates) were warped into a single participant’s space, the time course for each seed region was then computed as the average time course across all voxels in the reference region. The time courses were correlated between every seed pair and transformed to z-values using Fisher’s transform. Intranetwork connectivity strength for each overall network was defined as the average of the individual connections within each network, resulting in three functional connections per individual. Internetwork connectivity strength for each overall network was defined as the average of the individual connections between each network, resulting in three functional connections per individual.

Fig. 1.

Seeds used for the DMN, FPN, and SN networks. The DMN was defined as the posterior cingulate cortex (green), medial prefrontal cortex (yellow), right angular gyrus (red), and left angular gyrus (orange). The FPN was defined as the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (green), left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (yellow), left intraparietal sulcus (red), and right intraparietal sulcus (orange). The SN was defined as the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (yellow), left anterior insula (red), and right anterior insula (orange). Table S1 lists the MNI coordinates of each seed.

Fig. 1.

Seeds used for the DMN, FPN, and SN networks. The DMN was defined as the posterior cingulate cortex (green), medial prefrontal cortex (yellow), right angular gyrus (red), and left angular gyrus (orange). The FPN was defined as the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (green), left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (yellow), left intraparietal sulcus (red), and right intraparietal sulcus (orange). The SN was defined as the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (yellow), left anterior insula (red), and right anterior insula (orange). Table S1 lists the MNI coordinates of each seed.

Close modal

2.4.4 Head motion

Since head motion can potentially confound functional connectivity, we included several steps to ensure adequate control of motion confounds. For fetuses, we strictly censored all data for motion and data quality. There were no group differences in motion between the two fetal time points. For infants, the mean frame-to-frame displacement was calculated for each run for every individual. Runs with a mean frame-to-frame displacement of more than 0.2 mm were removed from further analysis. Additionally, iterative smoothing and regression of 24 motion parameters (6 rigid-body parameters, 6 temporal derivatives of these parameters, and these 12 parameters squared) were used in the infant data.

2.5 Statistical analyses

The quantities of interest included within-network connectivity strengths, between-network connectivity strengths, and differences between two such connectivity strengths. To investigate longitudinal changes in such a quantity as a function of PMA, we used a Bayesian spline model (Perperoglou et al., 2019) allowing a flexible range of nonlinear behaviors. The mean was modeled using a B-spline basis having a knot at each week of PMA from 30 weeks through 44 weeks. The extent of nonlinearity of the fit was controlled using a hierarchical model incorporating a Gaussian prior distribution on the second-order differences in the spline coefficients and allowing the model to estimate the standard deviation of that Gaussian prior. Random intercepts and slopes were included to account for the repeated measurements of individuals at different gestational ages. To account for the differences in the cohorts, the models also allowed each cohort to have an additive offset and error variance. Gaussian priors were used for regression coefficients, and half-Cauchy distributions were used for standard deviations, with scales chosen so that the priors were nearly flat for plausible values of the parameters. Posterior probabilities (that is, probabilities conditional on the observed data) were estimated using Markov chain Monte Carlo (MCMC), implemented using JAGS (Plummer, 2003) and R (Ihaka & Gentleman, 1996). For each quantity analyzed, the estimated parameters included mean values at the four PMA’s 32, 36, 40, and 44 weeks, changes in the mean from one age to the next ((PMA = 36)-(PMA = 32), (PMA = 40)-(PMA = 36), and (PMA = 44)-(PMA = 40)), as well as the change in mean over the whole range from PMA = 32 to PMA = 44 weeks. For each such parameter, we report MCMC estimates for the posterior mean (denoted by “m” in the results section), 2.5 and 97.5 percentiles (endpoints of 95% probability credible intervals, denoted by “lower” and “upper”), and the probability of a sign error (denoted by “pr”). The probability of a sign error is the posterior probability that the actual parameter has the opposite sign (negative or positive) from its reported estimated mean (Bickel, 2021). As the probability of a sign error is one sided, we report statistical “significance” when the probability of a sign error is estimated to be less than 0.05/2 = 0.025. To investigate the relationship between maternal mental health exposure and functional connectivity, we used the Bayesian spline model modified to include an additional linear effect of the first principal component, PC1, derived from the four maternal prenatal mental health variables.

3.1 Demographic information

Demographics for the subjects are shown in Table 1. Forty-one (49%) were females, the mean birth weight was 3536 (497) g, and the mean PMA at birth was 39.6 (1.3) weeks. There were no significant differences in sex, birth weights, race, ethnicity, or years of maternal education for the Yale cohorts. There were also no differences between the Yale and the dHCP cohorts in sex, birth weight, and PMA. Infants in the Yale longitudinal cohort underwent their first scan (F1) at a mean of 31.2 (0.7) weeks PMA; the second scan (F2) occurred at a mean PMA of 35.3 (0.8) weeks. Although there was no significant difference in PMA at birth for the three cohorts, infants in the dHCP underwent their neonatal scan earlier than those in the Yale cohorts (p<.001).

3.2 Intranetwork analyses

Longitudinal contrasts for all networks (Tables 2, 3, and 4, Fig. 2) revealed significant increases in connectivity strength across the third trimester and perinatal transition (PMA44–PMA32) as indexed by the mean change in within-network strength between 32 and 44 PMA time points: DMN: m = 0.199, FPN: m = 0.300, and SN: m = 0.289 (pr < 0.001 for all three networks). At the network level, all three evidenced significant increases during the prenatal to neonatal transition from 36 to 40 weeks PMA (DMN: m = 0.073, pr = 0.002; FPN: m = 0.161, pr < 0.001; SN: m = 0.161, pr = 0.012) and continued increases in connectivity during the first postnatal month (40–44 weeks; DMN: m = 0.083, pr = 0.001; FPN: m = 0.131, pr = 0.005; SN: m = 0.121, pr = 0.024).

Table 2.

Default mode network: development of intranetwork resting state functional connectivity from 32 to 44 postmenstrual weeks.

Estimated quantityMeanLowerUpperpr (sign error)
PMA = 32 -0.059 -0.095 -0.021 0.002 
PMA = 36 -0.015 -0.051 0.017 0.192 
PMA = 40 0.057 0.014 0.095 0.008 
PMA = 44 0.140 0.113 0.170 0.000 
(PMA = 36)-(PMA = 32) 0.043 -0.005 0.079 0.036 
(PMA = 40)-(PMA = 36) 0.073 0.034 0.119 0.002 
(PMA = 44)-(PMA = 40) 0.083 0.042 0.138 0.001 
(PMA = 44)-(PMA = 32) 0.199 0.155 0.245 0.000 
Estimated quantityMeanLowerUpperpr (sign error)
PMA = 32 -0.059 -0.095 -0.021 0.002 
PMA = 36 -0.015 -0.051 0.017 0.192 
PMA = 40 0.057 0.014 0.095 0.008 
PMA = 44 0.140 0.113 0.170 0.000 
(PMA = 36)-(PMA = 32) 0.043 -0.005 0.079 0.036 
(PMA = 40)-(PMA = 36) 0.073 0.034 0.119 0.002 
(PMA = 44)-(PMA = 40) 0.083 0.042 0.138 0.001 
(PMA = 44)-(PMA = 32) 0.199 0.155 0.245 0.000 

Legend: The upper four rows give estimates of the mean, lower, and upper endpoints of 95% probability credible intervals, and the probability of a sign error for the within-network connectivity values at postmenstrual ages of 32, 36, 40, and 44 weeks. The lower four rows give the same estimates for the increases in intranetwork connectivity from 32 to 36 weeks, from 36 to 40 weeks, from 40 to 44 weeks, and from 32 to 44 weeks postmenstrual age.

Table 3.

Frontoparietal network: development of intranetwork resting state functional connectivity from 32 to 44 postmenstrual weeks.

Estimated quantityMeanLowerUpperpr (sign error)
PMA = 32 -0.037 -0.091 0.014 0.077 
PMA = 36 -0.028 -0.074 0.017 0.110 
PMA = 40 0.133 0.062 0.210 0.001 
PMA = 44 0.264 0.225 0.303 0.000 
(PMA = 36)-(PMA = 32) 0.009 -0.053 0.071 0.391 
(PMA = 40)-(PMA = 36) 0.161 0.082 0.252 0.000 
(PMA = 44)-(PMA = 40) 0.131 0.039 0.217 0.005 
(PMA = 44)-(PMA = 32) 0.300 0.240 0.365 0.000 
Estimated quantityMeanLowerUpperpr (sign error)
PMA = 32 -0.037 -0.091 0.014 0.077 
PMA = 36 -0.028 -0.074 0.017 0.110 
PMA = 40 0.133 0.062 0.210 0.001 
PMA = 44 0.264 0.225 0.303 0.000 
(PMA = 36)-(PMA = 32) 0.009 -0.053 0.071 0.391 
(PMA = 40)-(PMA = 36) 0.161 0.082 0.252 0.000 
(PMA = 44)-(PMA = 40) 0.131 0.039 0.217 0.005 
(PMA = 44)-(PMA = 32) 0.300 0.240 0.365 0.000 

Legend: The upper four rows give estimates of the mean, lower and upper endpoints of 95% probability credible intervals, and the probability of a sign error for the within-network connectivity values at postmenstrual ages of 32, 36, 40, and 44 weeks. The lower four rows give the same estimates for the increases in intranetwork connectivity from 32 to 36 weeks, from 36 to 40 weeks, from 40 to 44 weeks, and from 32 to 44 weeks postmenstrual age.

Table 4.

Salience network: development of intranetwork resting state functional connectivity from 32 to 44 postmenstrual weeks.

Estimated quantityMeanLowerUpperpr (sign error)
PMA = 32 -0.006 -0.072 0.070 0.418 
PMA = 36 0.001 -0.060 0.069 0.495 
PMA = 40 0.163 0.059 0.264 0.002 
PMA = 44 0.283 0.233 0.331 0.000 
(PMA = 36)-(PMA = 32) 0.007 -0.081 0.094 0.430 
(PMA = 40)-(PMA = 36) 0.161 0.027 0.284 0.012 
(PMA = 44)-(PMA = 40) 0.121 0.001 0.236 0.024 
(PMA = 44)-(PMA = 32) 0.289 0.196 0.370 0.000 
Estimated quantityMeanLowerUpperpr (sign error)
PMA = 32 -0.006 -0.072 0.070 0.418 
PMA = 36 0.001 -0.060 0.069 0.495 
PMA = 40 0.163 0.059 0.264 0.002 
PMA = 44 0.283 0.233 0.331 0.000 
(PMA = 36)-(PMA = 32) 0.007 -0.081 0.094 0.430 
(PMA = 40)-(PMA = 36) 0.161 0.027 0.284 0.012 
(PMA = 44)-(PMA = 40) 0.121 0.001 0.236 0.024 
(PMA = 44)-(PMA = 32) 0.289 0.196 0.370 0.000 

Legend: The upper four rows give estimates of the mean, lower, and upper endpoints of 95% probability credible intervals, and the probability of a sign error for the within-network connectivity values at postmenstrual ages of 32, 36, 40, and 44 weeks. The lower four rows give the same estimates for the increases in intranetwork connectivity from 32 to 36 weeks, from 36 to 40 weeks, from 40 to 44 weeks, and from 32 to 44 weeks postmenstrual age.

Fig. 2.

Maturation of intranetwork connectivity for the DMN, FPN, and SN over the third trimester and first postnatal month. Estimates of connectivity strength at the anchors (32-, 36-, 40-, and 44-weeks PMA) for the Bayesian B-spline growth curve are shown as the red bars. The shaded red area represents 95% confidence intervals. Lines indicate longitudinal data from the same participant scanned at multiple time points.

Fig. 2.

Maturation of intranetwork connectivity for the DMN, FPN, and SN over the third trimester and first postnatal month. Estimates of connectivity strength at the anchors (32-, 36-, 40-, and 44-weeks PMA) for the Bayesian B-spline growth curve are shown as the red bars. The shaded red area represents 95% confidence intervals. Lines indicate longitudinal data from the same participant scanned at multiple time points.

Close modal

A complementary analysis comparing network strength to zero showed that during the prenatal period, none of the overall network strengths for the DMN, FPN, and SN networks was significantly greater than 0 at PMA 32 weeks and 36 weeks, while all three networks reached levels significantly greater than 0 levels at 40 weeks PMA (DMN: m = 0.057, pr = 0.008; FPN: m = 0.133, pr = 0.001; SN: m = 0.163, pr = 0.002; Tables 2, 3, and 4).

A second complementary analysis compared connectivity changes across the PMA44-PMA32 time interval for the 3 networks. As shown in Tables S2–S4, connectivity change across 32 to 44 weeks PMA was significantly lower for the DMN compared with SN (DMN–SN, PMA44–PMA32: m =-0.103, pr = 0.015). The connectivity change across this time interval was also significantly lower for the DMN than for FPN (DMN–FPN, PMA44–PMA32: m =-0.098, pr = 0.002). In contrast, there was no significant difference in growth curves when the FPN and SN were compared (SN–FPN, PMA44–PMA32: m = 0.024, pr = 0.255).

3.3 Internetwork analyses

Longitudinal contrasts for internetwork connectivity (Tables 5, 6, and 7; Fig. 3) revealed increases in DMN–FPN connectivity strength across the third trimester and perinatal transition (PMA 36–PMA 32: m = 0.029, pr = 0.037; PMA 40–PMA 36: m = 0.042, pr = 0.027; PMA 44-40 m = 0.066; pr < 0.001; PMA44-PMA32: m = 0.136; pr < 0.001). In contrast, there were significant decreases in internetwork connectivity for the DMN–SN and FPN–SN across this time interval (pr = 0.004 and pr = 0.001, respectively).

Table 5.

Development of internetwork resting state functional connectivity between default mode and frontoparietal networks from 32 to 44 postmenstrual weeks.

Estimated quantityMeanLowerUpperpr (sign error)
PMA = 32 0.024 -0.001 0.050 0.031 
PMA = 36 0.053 0.029 0.076 0.000 
PMA = 40 0.095 0.053 0.124 0.000 
PMA = 44 0.161 0.141 0.183 0.000 
(PMA = 36)-(PMA = 32) 0.029 -0.003 0.056 0.037 
(PMA = 40)-(PMA = 36) 0.042 -0.001 0.073 0.027 
(PMA = 44)-(PMA = 40) 0.066 0.034 0.119 0.000 
(PMA = 44)-(PMA = 32) 0.136 0.106 0.169 0.000 
Estimated quantityMeanLowerUpperpr (sign error)
PMA = 32 0.024 -0.001 0.050 0.031 
PMA = 36 0.053 0.029 0.076 0.000 
PMA = 40 0.095 0.053 0.124 0.000 
PMA = 44 0.161 0.141 0.183 0.000 
(PMA = 36)-(PMA = 32) 0.029 -0.003 0.056 0.037 
(PMA = 40)-(PMA = 36) 0.042 -0.001 0.073 0.027 
(PMA = 44)-(PMA = 40) 0.066 0.034 0.119 0.000 
(PMA = 44)-(PMA = 32) 0.136 0.106 0.169 0.000 

Legend: The upper four rows give estimates of the mean, lower, and upper endpoints of 95% probability credible intervals, and the probability of a sign error for the internetwork connectivity values at postmenstrual ages of 32, 36, 40, and 44 weeks. The lower four rows give the same estimates for the increases in internetwork connectivity from 32 to 36 weeks, from 36 to 40 weeks, from 40 to 44 weeks, and from 32 to 44 weeks.

Table 6.

Development of internetwork resting state functional connectivity between default mode and salience networks from 32 to 44 postmenstrual weeks.

Estimated quantityMeanLowerUpperpr (sign error)
PMA = 32 -0.016 -0.036 0.004 0.061 
PMA = 36 -0.030 -0.048 -0.011 0.002 
PMA = 40 -0.050 -0.083 -0.029 0.000 
PMA = 44 -0.056 -0.072 -0.038 0.000 
(PMA = 36)-(PMA = 32) -0.014 -0.037 0.010 0.088 
(PMA = 40)-(PMA = 36) -0.020 -0.057 0.000 0.025 
(PMA = 44)-(PMA = 40) -0.006 -0.028 0.035 0.246 
(PMA = 44)-(PMA = 32) -0.040 -0.065 -0.013 0.004 
Estimated quantityMeanLowerUpperpr (sign error)
PMA = 32 -0.016 -0.036 0.004 0.061 
PMA = 36 -0.030 -0.048 -0.011 0.002 
PMA = 40 -0.050 -0.083 -0.029 0.000 
PMA = 44 -0.056 -0.072 -0.038 0.000 
(PMA = 36)-(PMA = 32) -0.014 -0.037 0.010 0.088 
(PMA = 40)-(PMA = 36) -0.020 -0.057 0.000 0.025 
(PMA = 44)-(PMA = 40) -0.006 -0.028 0.035 0.246 
(PMA = 44)-(PMA = 32) -0.040 -0.065 -0.013 0.004 

Legend: The upper four rows give estimates of the mean, lower, and upper endpoints of 95% probability credible intervals, and the probability of a sign error for the internetwork connectivity values at postmenstrual ages of 32, 36, 40, and 44 weeks. The lower four rows give the same estimates for the increases in internetwork connectivity from 32 to 36 weeks, from 36 to 40 weeks, from 40 to 44 weeks, and from 32 to 44 weeks.

Table 7.

Development of internetwork resting state functional connectivity between executive control and salience networks from 32 to 44 postmenstrual weeks.

Estimated quantityMeanLowerUpperpr (sign error)
PMA= 32 -0.004 -0.033 0.024 0.376 
PMA= 36 -0.022 -0.045 0.000 0.027 
PMA= 40 -0.033 -0.059 0.008 0.043 
PMA= 44 -0.062 -0.084 -0.040 0.000 
(PMA = 36)-(PMA = 32) -0.018 -0.046 0.012 0.098 
(PMA = 40)-(PMA = 36) -0.011 -0.037 0.037 0.183 
(PMA = 44)-(PMA = 40) -0.028 -0.079 -0.000 0.023 
(PMA = 44)-(PMA = 32) -0.057 -0.093 -0.022 0.001 
Estimated quantityMeanLowerUpperpr (sign error)
PMA= 32 -0.004 -0.033 0.024 0.376 
PMA= 36 -0.022 -0.045 0.000 0.027 
PMA= 40 -0.033 -0.059 0.008 0.043 
PMA= 44 -0.062 -0.084 -0.040 0.000 
(PMA = 36)-(PMA = 32) -0.018 -0.046 0.012 0.098 
(PMA = 40)-(PMA = 36) -0.011 -0.037 0.037 0.183 
(PMA = 44)-(PMA = 40) -0.028 -0.079 -0.000 0.023 
(PMA = 44)-(PMA = 32) -0.057 -0.093 -0.022 0.001 

Legend: The upper four rows give estimates of the mean, lower, and upper endpoints of 95% probability credible intervals, and the probability of a sign error for the internetwork connectivity values at postmenstrual ages of 32, 36, 40, and 44 weeks. The lower four rows give the same estimates for the increases in internetwork connectivity from 32 to 36 weeks, from 36 to 40 weeks, from 40 to 44 weeks, and from 32 to 44 weeks.

Fig. 3.

Maturation of internetwork connectivity between DMN–FPN, DMN–SN, and FPN–SN over the third trimester and first postnatal month. Estimates of connectivity strength at the anchors (32-, 36-, 40-, and 44-week PMA) for the Bayesian B-spline growth curve are shown as the red bars. The shaded red area represents 95% confidence intervals. Lines indicate longitudinal data from the same participant scanned at multiple time points.

Fig. 3.

Maturation of internetwork connectivity between DMN–FPN, DMN–SN, and FPN–SN over the third trimester and first postnatal month. Estimates of connectivity strength at the anchors (32-, 36-, 40-, and 44-week PMA) for the Bayesian B-spline growth curve are shown as the red bars. The shaded red area represents 95% confidence intervals. Lines indicate longitudinal data from the same participant scanned at multiple time points.

Close modal

As shown in Table 5, the overall internetwork connectivity strength for the DMN–FPN exceeded 0 at 32-, 36-, 40-, and 44-week PMA (PMA 32: m = 0.024, pr = 0.031; PMA 36: m = 0.053, pr <0.001 PMA 40: m = 0.095, pr < 0.001; PMA 44: m = 0.161, pr < 0.001). In contrast, the DMN–SN internetwork analysis showed connectivity significantly < 0 across the 36, 40, and 44 weeks PMA time points (pr < 0.002, Table 6). Likewise, for the FPN–SN internetwork connectivity, the estimate was also significantly < 0 across the 36–44 week PMA time interval (p<0.05, Table 7). At 32 weeks, the DMN–SN and FPN–SN subtractions were not different from 0 (pr = 0.061 and pr = 0.376, respectively).

Longitudinal contrasts for internetwork connectivity (Tables S5, S6, and S7) revealed that DMN–FPN connectivity strength increased more than that for DMN–SN across the third trimester and perinatal transition (PMA 36–PMA 32: m = 0.046, pr = 0.024; PMA 40–PMA 36: m = 0.064, pr = 0.006; PMA 44–PMA 40: m = 0.067, pr = 0.005; PMA 44–PMA 32: m = 0.177; pr < 0.001). Likewise, the DMN–FPN connectivity increased more rapidly from PMA 32 weeks to PMA 44 weeks than that for the FPN–SN internetwork connectivity (PMA 36–PMA 32: m = 0.049, pr = 0.033; PMA 40–PMA 36: m = 0.050, pr = 0.076; PMA 44–PMA 40: m = 0.092; pr < 0.001; PMA 44–PMA 32: m = 0.195; pr < 0.001). In contrast, there were no significant differences in internetwork connectivity increases between the DMN–SN and FPN–SN connections across this time interval.

As shown in Table 5, the difference in the overall internetwork connectivity strength for the DMN–FPN compared with DMN–SN exceeded 0 across the third trimester and perinatal transition (PMA 32: pr = .012; pr < 0.001 for all other comparisons). The DMN–FPN internetwork connectivity was also greater than the FPN–SN connectivity between PMA 36 and PMA 44 (pr < 0.002 for all). In contrast, a comparison of connectivity between the FPN–SN and DMN–SN showed no values significantly different than 0 between PMA 32 and PMA 44 weeks (pr > 0.182).

3.4 Exploratory analysis of prenatal maternal mental health on network connectivity

To address a potential question related to the impact of the pandemic (Manning et al., 2022; Rajagopalan et al., 2022) on the Yale data, we compared the maternal mental health variables between the two groups who were scanned before and during the pandemic. Maternal mental health data were collected at a mean PMA of 28.36 (standard deviation = 2.29) weeks, or 3.17 (1.62) weeks before the first fetal scan, 6.91 (2.76) weeks before the second scan, and 14.75 (3.04) weeks before the neonatal scan. A descriptive summary of the maternal mental health variables is shown in Table 8. On the STAI screener, 4 of 29 (13.8%) mothers exceeded the cutoff of 40 during the prepartum period; 2 of 29 (6.9%) of the cohort exceeded the cutoff point of 11 on the EPDS, demonstrating that the prevalence of elevated symptoms of depression and anxiety in our sample was consistent with the prepandemic estimates observed in the general population (Dennis et al., 2017; Levis et al., 2020; Shorey et al., 2018). There were also no significant differences in scores on the maternal mental health index (PC1) between mothers who were enrolled before March 2020 (n = 14) vs. those enrolled after (n = 15), p = .930.

Table 8.

Summary of prenatal mental health measures for the fetal-neonatal cohort.

NMean +/- SD
PMA at interview (wks) 29 28.36 +/- 2.29 
Wks. before F1 scan 26 3.17 +/- 1.62 
Wks. before F2 scan 25 6.91 +/- 2.76 
Wks. before NN scan 17 14.75 +/- 3.04 
Prenatal mental health measures 
 EPDS score 29 4.21 +/- 3.38 
 PSS score 29 15.59 +/- 7.02 
 STAI state score 29 29.34 +/- 9.27 
 STAI trait score 29 30.79 +/- 8.42 
NMean +/- SD
PMA at interview (wks) 29 28.36 +/- 2.29 
Wks. before F1 scan 26 3.17 +/- 1.62 
Wks. before F2 scan 25 6.91 +/- 2.76 
Wks. before NN scan 17 14.75 +/- 3.04 
Prenatal mental health measures 
 EPDS score 29 4.21 +/- 3.38 
 PSS score 29 15.59 +/- 7.02 
 STAI state score 29 29.34 +/- 9.27 
 STAI trait score 29 30.79 +/- 8.42 

The estimated association between the maternal mental health index (first principal component) derived from the four maternal prenatal mental health variables and the DMN, FPN, and SN networks was negative for all three networks, with sign error probabilities of 0.079, 0.071, and 0.014, respectively. Thus, while the effects in the FPN and DMN approached significance, only the SN network had a (one-sided) sign error probability below 0.025. The relationship between maternal mental health index and functional connectivity of the SN network at the three longitudinal time points is illustrated in Figure 4, which suggests the significant negative impact of higher maternal mental health index across the third trimester and the first postnatal month on the development of the SN network.

Fig. 4.

Association between intranetwork SN connectivity at each data collection time point (i.e., F1—early third trimester, F2—mid-third trimester, NN—neonatal), and maternal stress collected in the third trimester. These associations change (possibly strengthening) as intranetwork SN connectivity develops.

Fig. 4.

Association between intranetwork SN connectivity at each data collection time point (i.e., F1—early third trimester, F2—mid-third trimester, NN—neonatal), and maternal stress collected in the third trimester. These associations change (possibly strengthening) as intranetwork SN connectivity develops.

Close modal

The default mode, frontoparietal, and salience networks develop rapidly across the third trimester of gestation and first postnatal weeks, showing significant changes in their inter- and intranetwork connectivity across this critical period.

At the intranetwork level, we report significant increases occurring during the prenatal to neonatal transition with all three networks showing significant increases in connectivity starting at 36 weeks PMA and network strength reaching positive values at 40 weeks PMA. These findings suggest that synchronization between the nodes within the three cortical networks previously reported in infants (Gao et al., 2015) begins before birth, laying foundation for experience-dependent social and cognitive development occurring during the postnatal months. Functional connectivity increased less rapidly in the DMN compared with both the FPN and SN networks, suggesting slower maturation of this network responsible for integrating autobiographical, self-monitoring, and social cognitive interactions (Supekar et al., 2010). Recent data suggest that a fully functional DMN may not be identified until 3 years of age (Richardson et al., 2018), and slower development of the DMN has been reported both in a cross-sectional study of typically developing fetuses and neonates and in extremely low gestational age neonates, a model of fetal neural connectivity (Doria et al., 2010; Karolis et al., 2023; Smyser et al., 2010). Fetuses recognize sound at 32 weeks (Krueger & Garvan, 2014) and more frequently turn their heads toward face-like lights than upside-down lights projected through the maternal uterine wall (Reid et al., 2017), but the role of DMN-mediated social cognition and fetal behavior has yet to be explored.

Analysis of the connectivity between the three networks revealed patterns of both increasing synchronization and segregation between the networks beginning before birth. A prior study demonstrated that connectivity between the SN and DMN nodes and between SN and FPN nodes decreases significantly over the course of infancy, while the connectivity between FPN and DMN increases during the same period (Gao et al., 2015). Here we report, for the first time, that these developmental trends are already present during the last trimester of pregnancy. Specifically, we found decreasing connectivity between the SN and the DMN and FPN during the prenatal period, with an overall linear decrease between 32 and 44 weeks and values falling significantly below 0 by 36 weeks PMA. The decreasing connectivity between the networks during the fetal to neonatal transition likely represents a wide-spread phenomenon of network segregation occurring in the first 2 years of life (Gao et al., 2015), an evolution toward mature segregation patterns observed in the adult brain (Seeley et al., 2007).

In contrast, the internetwork connectivity between DMN and FPN showed positive values at 32 weeks, and a linear increase in connectivity continued rapidly during the postnatal weeks. This increasing synchronization between the two networks during the third trimester is a novel finding, as later in development these two networks are expected to be anticorrelated at rest (Grecius et al., 2003; Menon & Uddin, 2010; Seeley et al., 2007). Functional connectivity develops from medial to lateral and posterior to anterior (Jakab et al., 2014; Thomason et al., 2013), and graph theory studies suggest that connectivity between the DMN and FPN during development results from anatomical proximity (Niu & Palomero-Gallagher, 2023; Power et al., 2010; Seghier, 2013). Fetuses selectively display more arm, head, and mouth movements when the mother touches her abdomen and decrease these movements to maternal voice, suggesting interplay between the DMN and FPN during the third trimester of gestation (Marx & Nagy, 2015). Alternatively, the increasing integration of these two networks from 30 weeks PMA through the first postnatal month may reflect connectivity in the experience-expectant fetal-to-neonatal brain awaiting cognitive demands (Mundy & Jarrold, 2010; Tierney & Nelson, 2009), focused internal attention (Beaty et al., 2015), variations in regional neurovascular reactivity (Hendrikx et al., 2019), or a prolonged “transient” local connection (Jakab et al., 2014; Thomason et al., 2013). Given the significance of the two networks for social attention and behavior and the recognition that the anticorrelation patterns of the DMN and FPN are likely hallmarks of the competitive nature of these social cognition networks, future research should assess the functional significance of this developmental dynamic through prospective longitudinal studies in infancy.

The impact of maternal mental health on the development of the DMN, FPN, and SN has been studied across the mid-second and third trimesters, in preterm neonates, and in term infants in the first postnatal month (Brady et al., 2022; De Asis-Cruz, Kapse, et al., 2020; Lee et al., 2013; Scheinost, Kwon, Lacadie, et al., 2016; Scheinost et al., 2020; Seshamani et al., 2016; Spann et al., 2018; Sylvester et al., 2018; Thomason et al., 2015). Prior fetal studies highlight that maternal distress alters the FPN before birth (Thomason et al., 2021), and prenatal maternal anxiety decreases both FPN and DMN prenatal connections (De Asis-Cruz, Krishnamurthy, et al., 2020). Our findings extend the published literature by demonstrating the adverse impact of increased prenatal mental health vulnerabilities including stress, depression, or anxiety on development of SN connectivity across the late third trimester and first postnatal month. Although exploratory, the associations between prenatal maternal mental health and SN connectivity appear to strengthen with age, reaching statistical significance by neonatal period, suggesting that more robust brain-behavioral associations are observable when functional networks are more developed. Alternatively, as maternal mental health—and its impact on the developing brain—is likely cumulative, these associations could be dose dependent. Thus, fetuses experiencing high maternal distress early in the third trimester will likely continue to experience maternal distress through the neonatal period, leading to more significant differences in brain connectivity. Maternal stress, depression, or anxiety may lead to reprogramming of the fetal hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis (Van den Bergh et al., 2017), downregulate placental 11β-hydroxysteroid dehydrogenase 2, an enzyme responsible for the metabolism of cortisol (Jensen Pena et al., 2012; O’Donnell et al., 2012), and result in epigenetic changes in both the placental and the fetal brain (Bock et al., 2015). Maternal stress elevates levels of immune response genes including IL-6 and IL1β (Bronson & Bale, 2014), impairs GABA interneuron maturation (Lussier & Stevens, 2016), and has been linked to altered structure and function of limbic, subcortical, and frontal regions (Bock et al., 2014; van den Heuvel et al., 2021). Regardless, these results highlight that the brain correlates of prenatal exposures may change depending on when the imaging and exposure data are collected (Dufford et al., 2021).

The strengths of this work include the longitudinal/cross-sectional design, robust participant numbers, extensive phenotyping of the fetuses and neonates, stringent inclusion and exclusion criteria, collection of multiple standardized maternal health variables early in the third trimester of gestation, and the PMA-appropriate MRI templates. The weaknesses include lacking behavioral data correlating with DMN, FPN, and SN connectivity, though follow-up data collection on the fetal and neonatal samples is ongoing. How these trajectories relate to later lateralization is unknown (Scheinost et al., 2015). As no apparent differences in maternal mental health due to COVID-19 was observed, we did not control for this index in our exploratory analyses, given the smaller sample and exploratory nature of the analysis as well as that the data were not available for the dHCP sample.

We choose to process the fetal and infant data with separate pipelines, specifically designed for each data type. This choice accounts for unique artifacts and processing challenges of each age group, producing the most valid data. Nevertheless, it may influence the behavior of our statistical models as processing is not consistent across all data. Previously described limitations of fetal functional imaging range from variations in fetal brain orientation, motion, the influence of placental, maternal, and fetal physiological signals, and the small head size to changes in fetal cerebral metabolism and the limited understanding of the physiological basis of the BOLD fMRI signals in the fetal brain (Thomason, 2020; Thomason et al., 2017). These are persisting concerns for MRI studies in neonates and young children, and we have used many of the previously published strategies to address these concerns (Avants et al., 2014; Glover et al., 2000; Power et al., 2012).

We report that the developmental trajectories of the SN, DMN, and FPN mapped out previously in infancy and early childhood have their origins in the last weeks of gestation, with significant changes in intra- and internetwork connectivity occurring during the pre- to postnatal transition. These fetal networks are plastic and responsive to their environment, and in exploratory analyses, we demonstrate the impact of maternal mental health on fetal network level connectivity. Abnormalities of intra- and internetwork connectivity are becoming well recognized as neuroimaging biomarkers of childhood neurobehavioral disorders such as ASD (Haghighat et al., 2021). The developmental/functional significance of the individual differences in the developmental dynamics of these three large scale cortical networks remains to be examined. Future work should target the developmental timing of these abnormalities, identify the genes that support them, and plan early fetal intervention for connectivity disorders of the developing brain.

The Fetal-neonatal Cohort and Neonatal Cohort datasets will be released on https://nda.nih.gov/ following an embargo period. The dHCP data can be accessed at http://www.developingconnectome.org/project/. The image analysis software (BioImage Suite) can be found at https://medicine.yale.edu/bioimaging/suite/ and https://bioimagesuiteweb.github.io/webapp/index.html.

D.S.: Methodology, Writing. J.C.: Methodology, Formal analysis. E.B.-W: Data curation. C.L.: Formal analysis. R.T.C.: Project administration. K.C.: Writing, Conceptualization. L.R.M.: Writing, Conceptualization

Research reported in this publication was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number P50MH115716. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.

All authors declare no competing interests

The authors thank the families for their participation. We are grateful to Hedy Sarofin, RTR, MR, ARRT, the ACE research fellows Hannah Feiner BS, Rachel Foster BS, Hannah Neiderman BA, and Carolyn Gershman BA for technical assistance, Sahand Negahban, PhD and the ACE Statistics Core for their input on statistical analysis, Kelly Powell PhD, Suzanne Macari PhD, Katherine Kohari MD, Soo Kwon MD, Chelsea Morgan PsyD, James McPartland PhD and Fred Volkmar MD, and Amy Giguere Carney, MSW for their contribution to sample recruitment and characterization.

Supplementary material for this article is available with the online version here: https://doi.org/10.1162/imag_a_00201.

Andrews-Hanna
,
J. R.
,
Smallwood
,
J.
, &
Spreng
,
R. N.
(
2014
).
The default network and self-generated thought: Component processes, dynamic control, and clinical relevance
.
Ann N Y Acad Sci
,
1316
(
1
),
29
. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.12360
Avants
,
B. B.
,
Tustison
,
N. J.
,
Stauffer
,
M.
,
Song
,
G.
,
Wu
,
B.
, &
Gee
,
J. C.
(
2014
).
The Insight ToolKit image registration framework
.
Front Neuroinform
,
8
,
44
. https://doi.org/10.3389/fninf.2014.00044
Beaty
,
R. E.
,
Benedek
,
M.
,
Kaufman
,
S. B.
, &
Silvia
,
P. J.
(
2015
).
Default and executive network coupling supports creative idea production
.
Sci Rep
,
5
,
10964
. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep10964
Bickel
,
D. R.
(
2021
).
Null hypothesis significance testing interpreted and calibrated by estimating probabilities of sign errors: A Bayes-Frequentist continuum
.
Am Stat
,
75
(
1
),
104
112
. https://doi.org/10.1080/00031305.2020.1816214
Bock
,
J.
,
Rether
,
K.
,
Groger
,
N.
,
Xie
,
L.
, &
Braun
,
K.
(
2014
).
Perinatal programming of emotional brain circuits: An integrative view from systems to molecules
.
Front Neurosci
,
8
,
11
. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2014.00011
Bock
,
J.
,
Wainstock
,
T.
,
Braun
,
K.
, &
Segal
,
M.
(
2015
).
Stress in utero: Prenatal programming of brain plasticity and cognition
.
Biol Psychiatry
,
78
(
5
),
315
326
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.02.036
Brady
,
R. G.
,
Rogers
,
C. E.
,
Prochaska
,
T.
,
Kaplan
,
S.
,
Lean
,
R. E.
,
Smyser
,
T. A.
,
Shimony
,
J. S.
,
Slavich
,
G. M.
,
Warner
,
B. B.
,
Barch
,
D. M.
,
Luby
,
J. L.
, &
Smyser
,
C. D.
(
2022
).
The effects of prenatal exposure to neighborhood crime on neonatal functional connectivity
.
Biol Psychiatry
,
92
(
2
),
139
148
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2022.01.020
Bronson
,
S. L.
, &
Bale
,
T. L.
(
2014
).
Prenatal stress-induced increases in placental inflammation and offspring hyperactivity are male-specific and ameliorated by maternal antiinflammatory treatment
.
Endocrinology
,
155
(
7
),
2635
2646
. https://doi.org/10.1210/en.2014-1040
Buckner
,
R. L.
, &
DiNicola
,
L. M.
(
2019
).
The brain’s default network: Updated anatomy, physiology and evolving insights
.
Nat Rev Neurosci
,
20
(
10
),
593
608
. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41583-019-0212-7
Canini
,
M.
,
Cavoretto
,
P.
,
Scifo
,
P.
,
Pozzoni
,
M.
,
Petrini
,
A.
,
Ladanza
,
A.
,
Pontesilli
,
S.
,
Scotti
,
R.
,
Candiani
,
M.
,
Falini
,
A.
,
Baldoli
,
C.
, &
Rosa
Della
,
A
P.
. (
2020
).
Subcortico-cortical functional connectivity in the fetal brain: A cognitive development blueprint
.
Cereb Cortex Commun
,
1
(
1
),
tgaa008
. https://doi.org/10.1093/texcom/tgaa008
Cohen
,
S.
,
Kamarck
,
T.
, &
Mermelstein
,
R.
(
1983
).
A global measure of perceived stress
.
J Health Soc Behav
,
24
(
4
),
385
396
. https://doi.org/10.2307/2136404
Cox
,
J. L.
,
Holden
,
J. M.
, &
Sagovsky
,
R.
(
1987
).
Detection of postnatal depression: Development of the 10-item Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale
.
Br J Psychiatry
,
150
(
6
),
782
786
. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.150.6.782
De Asis-Cruz
,
J.
,
Andersen
,
N.
,
Kapse
,
K.
,
Khrisnamurthy
,
D.
,
Quistorff
,
J.
,
Lopez
,
C.
,
Vezina
,
G.
, &
Limperopoulos
,
C.
(
2021
).
Global network organization of the fetal functional connectome
.
Cereb Cortex
,
31
(
6
),
3034
3046
. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhaa410
De Asis-Cruz
,
J.
,
Kapse
,
K.
,
Basu
,
S. K.
,
Said
,
M.
,
Scheinost
,
D.
,
Murnick
,
J.
,
Chang
,
T.
,
Plessis
du
, A., &
Limperopoulos
,
C.
(
2020
).
Functional brain connectivity in ex utero premature infants compared to in utero fetuses
.
Neuroimage
,
219
,
117043
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117043
De Asis-Cruz
,
J.
,
Krishnamurthy
,
D.
,
Zhao
,
L.
,
Kapse
,
K.
,
Vezina
,
G.
,
Andescavage
,
N.
,
Quistorff
,
J.
,
Lopez
,
C.
, &
Limperopoulos
,
C.
(
2020
).
Association of prenatal maternal anxiety with fetal regional brain connectivity
.
JAMA Netw Open
,
3
(
12
),
e2022349
. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.22349
De Asis-Cruz
,
J.
, &
Limperopoulos
,
C.
(
2023
).
Harnessing the power of advanced fetal neuroimaging to understand in utero footprints for later neuropsychiatric disorders
.
Biol Psychiatry
,
93
(
10
),
867
879
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2022.11.019
Dennis
,
C. L.
,
Falah-Hassani
,
K.
, &
Shiri
,
R.
(
2017
).
Prevalence of antenatal and postnatal anxiety: Systematic review and meta-analysis
.
Br J Psychiatry
,
210
(
5
),
315
323
. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.116.187179
Doria
,
V.
,
Beckmann
,
C. F.
,
Arichi
,
T.
,
Merchant
,
N.
,
Groppo
,
M.
,
Turkheimer
,
F. E.
,
Counsell
,
S. J.
,
Murgasova
,
M.
,
Aljabar
,
P.
,
Nunes
,
R. G.
,
Larkman
,
D. J.
,
Rees
,
G.
, &
Edwards
,
A. D.
(
2010
).
Emergence of resting state networks in the preterm human brain
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
,
107
(
46
),
20015
20020
. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1007921107
Dufford
,
A. J.
,
Spann
,
M.
, &
Scheinost
,
D.
(
2021
).
How prenatal exposures shape the infant brain: Insights from infant neuroimaging studies
.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev
,
131
,
47
58
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.09.017
Eyre
,
M.
,
Fitzgibbon
,
S. P.
,
Ciarrusta
,
J.
,
Cordero-Grande
,
L.
,
Price
,
A. N.
,
Poppe
,
T.
,
Schuh
,
A.
,
Hughes
,
E.
,
O’Keeffe
,
C.
,
Brandon
,
J.
,
Cromb
,
D.
,
Vecchiato
,
K.
,
Andersson
,
J.
,
Duff
,
E. P.
,
Counsell
,
S. J.
,
Smith
,
S. M.
,
Rueckert
,
D.
,
Hajnal
,
J. V.
,
Arichi
,
T.
, …
Edwards David
,
A.
(
2021
).
The Developing Human Connectome Project: Typical and disrupted perinatal functional connectivity
.
Brain
,
144
(
7
),
2199
2213
. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awab118
Gao
,
W.
,
Alcauter
,
S.
,
Smith
,
J. K.
,
Gilmore
,
J. H.
, &
Lin
,
W.
(
2015
).
Development of human brain cortical network architecture during infancy
.
Brain Struct Funct
,
220
(
2
),
1173
1186
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-014-0710-3
Gao
,
W.
,
Gilmore
,
J. H.
,
Giovanello
,
K. S.
,
Smith
,
J. K.
,
Shen
,
D.
,
Zhu
,
H.
, &
Lin
,
W.
(
2011
).
Temporal and spatial evolution of brain network topology during the first two years of life
.
PLoS One
,
6
(
9
),
e25278
. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0025278
Gao
,
W.
,
Gilmore
,
J. H.
,
Shen
,
D.
,
Smith
,
J. K.
,
Zhu
,
H.
, &
Lin
,
W.
(
2013
).
The synchronization within and interaction between the default and dorsal attention networks in early infancy
.
Cereb Cortex
,
23
(
3
),
594
603
. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhs043
Gao
,
W.
,
Zhu
,
H.
,
Giovanello
,
K. S.
,
Smith
,
J. K.
,
Shen
,
D.
,
Gilmore
,
J. H.
, &
Lin
,
W.
(
2009
).
Evidence on the emergence of the brain’s default network from 2-week-old to 2-year-old healthy pediatric subjects
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
,
106
(
16
),
6790
6795
. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0811221106
Gholipour
,
A.
,
Rollins
,
C. K.
,
Velasco-Annis
,
C.
,
Ouaalam
,
A.
,
Akhondi-Asl
,
A.
,
Afacan
,
O.
,
Ortinau
,
C. M.
,
Clancy
,
S.
,
Limperopoulos
,
C.
,
Yang
,
E.
,
Estroff
,
J. A.
, &
Warfield
,
S. K.
(
2017
).
A normative spatiotemporal MRI atlas of the fetal brain for automatic segmentation and analysis of early brain growth
.
Sci Rep
,
7
(
1
),
476
. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-00525-w
Glover
,
G. H.
,
Li
,
T. Q.
, &
Ress
,
D.
(
2000
).
Image-based method for retrospective correction of physiological motion effects in fMRI: RETROICOR
.
Magn Reson Med
,
44
,
162
167
. https://doi.org/10.1002/1522-2594(200007)44:1<162::aid-mrm23>3.3.co;2-5
Grecius
,
M. D.
,
Krasnow
,
B.
,
Reiss
,
A. L.
, &
Menon
,
V.
(
2003
).
Functional connectivity in the resting brain: A network analysis of the default mode hypothesis
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
,
100
,
253
258
. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0135058100
Hadlock
,
F. P.
,
Harrist
,
R. B.
, &
Martinez-Poyer
,
J.
(
1991
).
In utero analysis of fetal growth: A sonographic weight standard
.
Radiology
,
181
(
1
),
129
133
. https://doi.org/10.1148/radiology.181.1.1887021
Hadlock
,
F. P.
,
Harrist
,
R. B.
,
Shah
,
Y. P.
,
Sharman
,
R. S.
, &
Park
,
S. K.
(
1990
).
Sonographic fetal growth standards. Are current data applicable to a racially mixed population
?
J Ultrasound Med
,
9
(
3
),
157
160
. https://doi.org/10.7863/jum.1990.9.3.157
Haghighat
,
H.
,
Mirzarezaee
,
M.
,
Araabi
,
B. N.
, &
Khadem
,
A.
(
2021
).
Functional networks abnormalities in autism spectrum disorder: Age-related hypo and hyper connectivity
.
Brain Topogr
,
34
(
3
),
306
322
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10548-021-00831-7
Hendrikx
,
D.
,
Smits
,
A.
,
Lavanga
,
M.
,
De Wel
,
O.
,
Thewissen
,
L.
,
Jansen
,
K.
,
Caicedo
,
A.
,
Van Huffel
,
S.
, &
Naulaers
,
G.
(
2019
).
Measurement of neurovascular coupling in neonates
.
Front Physiol
,
10
,
65
. https://doi.org/10.3389/fphys.2019.00065
Hu
,
H.
,
Cusack
,
R.
, &
Naci
,
L.
(
2022
).
Typical and disrupted brain circuitry for conscious awareness in full-term and preterm infants
.
Brain Commun
,
4
(
2
),
fcac071
. https://doi.org/10.1093/braincomms/fcac071
Ihaka
,
R.
, &
Gentleman
,
R.
(
1996
).
R: A language for data analysis and graphics
.
J Comput Graph Stat
,
5
(
3
),
299
314
. https://doi.org/10.2307/1390807
Jakab
,
A.
,
Schwartz
,
E.
,
Kasprian
,
G.
,
Gruber
,
G. M.
,
Prayer
,
D.
,
Schopf
,
V.
, &
Langs
,
G.
(
2014
).
Fetal functional imaging portrays heterogeneous development of emerging human brain networks
.
Front Hum Neurosci
,
8
,
852
. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00852
Jensen Pena
,
C.
,
Monk
,
C.
, &
Champagne
,
F. A.
(
2012
).
Epigenetic effects of prenatal stress on 11beta-hydroxysteroid dehydrogenase-2 in the placenta and fetal brain
.
PLoS One
,
7
(
6
),
e39791
. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0039791
Joshi
,
A.
,
Scheinost
,
D.
,
Okuda
,
H.
,
Belhachemi
,
D.
,
Murphy
,
I.
,
Staib
,
L. H.
, &
Papademetris
,
X.
(
2011
).
Unified framework for development, deployment and robust testing of neuroimaging algorithms
.
Neuroinformatics
,
9
(
1
),
69
84
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12021-010-9092-8
Karolis
,
V. R.
,
Fitzgibbon
,
S. P.
,
Cordero-Grande
,
L.
,
Farahibozorg
,
S. R.
,
Price
,
A. N.
,
Hughes
,
E. J.
,
Fetit
,
A. E.
,
Kyriakopoulou
,
V.
,
Pietsch
,
M.
,
Rutherford
,
M. A.
,
Rueckert
,
D.
,
Hajnal
,
J. V.
,
Edwards
,
A. D.
,
O’Muircheartaigh
,
J.
,
Duff
,
E. P.
, &
Arichi
,
T.
(
2023
).
Maturational networks of human fetal brain activity reveal emerging connectivity patterns prior to ex-utero exposure
.
Commun Biol
,
6
(
1
),
661
. https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-023-04969-x
Kim
,
J. H.
,
De Asis-Cruz
,
J.
,
Cook
,
K. M.
, &
Limperopoulos
,
C.
(
2022
).
Gestational age-related changes in the fetal functional connectome: In utero evidence for the global signal
.
Cereb Cortex
,
33
(
5
),
2302
2314
. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhac209
Kim
,
J. H.
,
De Asis-Cruz
,
J.
,
Krishnamurthy
,
D.
, &
Limperopoulos
,
C.
(
2023
).
Toward a more informative representation of the fetal-neonatal brain connectome using variational autoencoder
.
Elife
,
12
. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.80878
Krueger
,
C.
, &
Garvan
,
C.
(
2014
).
Emergence and retention of learning in early fetal development
.
Infant Behav Dev
,
37
(
2
),
162
173
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2013.12.007
Kwon
,
S. H.
,
Scheinost
,
D.
,
Lacadie
,
C.
,
Benjamin
,
J.
,
Myers
,
E. H.
,
Qiu
,
M.
,
Schneider
,
K. C.
,
Rothman
,
D. L.
,
Constable
,
R. T.
, &
Ment
,
L. R.
(
2014
).
GABA, resting-state connectivity and the developing brain
.
Neonatology
,
106
(
2
),
149
155
. https://doi.org/10.1159/000362433
Kwon
,
S. H.
,
Scheinost
,
D.
,
Lacadie
,
C.
,
Sze
,
G.
,
Schneider
,
K. C.
,
Dai
,
F.
,
Constable
,
R. T.
, &
Ment
,
L. R.
(
2015
).
Adaptive mechanisms of developing brain: Cerebral lateralization in the prematurely-born
.
Neuroimage
,
108
,
144
150
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.12.032
Lee
,
W.
,
Morgan
,
B. R.
,
Shroff
,
M. M.
,
Sled
,
J. G.
, &
Taylor
,
M. J.
(
2013
).
The development of regional functional connectivity in preterm infants into early childhood
.
Neuroradiology, 55 Suppl
2
,
105
111
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00234-013-1232-z
Levis
,
B.
,
Negeri
,
Z.
,
Sun
,
Y.
,
Benedetti
,
A.
,
Thombs
,
B. D.
, &
Group Depression Screening Data (Depressd) EPDS
. (
2020
).
Accuracy of the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS) for screening to detect major depression among pregnant and postpartum women: Systematic review and meta-analysis of individual participant data
.
BMJ
,
371
,
m4022
. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.m4022
Lussier
,
S. J.
, &
Stevens
,
H. E.
(
2016
).
Delays in GABAergic interneuron development and behavioral inhibition after prenatal stress
.
Dev Neurobiol
,
76
(
10
),
1078
1091
. https://doi.org/10.1002/dneu.22376
Manning
,
K. Y.
,
Long
,
X.
,
Watts
,
D.
,
Tomfohr-Madsen
,
L.
,
Giesbrecht
,
G. F.
, &
Lebel
,
C.
(
2022
).
Prenatal maternal distress during the COVID-19 pandemic and associations with infant brain connectivity
.
Biol Psychiatry
,
92
(
9
),
701
708
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2022.05.011
Marshall
,
E.
,
Nomi
,
J. S.
,
Dirks
,
B.
,
Romero
,
C.
,
Kupis
,
L.
,
Chang
,
C.
, &
Uddin
,
L. Q.
(
2020
).
Coactivation pattern analysis reveals altered salience network dynamics in children with autism spectrum disorder
.
Netw Neurosci
,
4
(
4
),
1219
1234
. https://doi.org/10.1162/netn_a_00163
Marx
,
V.
, &
Nagy
,
E.
(
2015
).
Fetal behavioural responses to maternal voice and touch
.
PLoS One
,
10
(
6
),
e0129118
. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0129118
Menon
,
V.
(
2011
).
Large-scale brain networks and psychopathology: A unifying triple network model
.
Trends Cogn Sci
,
15
(
10
),
483
506
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.08.003
Menon
,
V.
, &
Uddin
,
L. Q.
(
2010
).
Saliency, switching, attention and control: A network model of insula function
.
Brain Struct Funct
,
214
(
5-6
),
655
667
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-010-0262-0
Molnar-Szakacs
,
I.
, &
Uddin
,
L. Q.
(
2022
).
Anterior insula as a gatekeeper of executive control
.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev
,
139
,
104736
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2022.104736
Mundy
,
P.
, &
Jarrold
,
W.
(
2010
).
Infant joint attention, neural networks and social cognition
.
Neural Netw
,
23
(
8-9
),
985
997
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neunet.2010.08.009
Niu
,
M.
, &
Palomero-Gallagher
,
N.
(
2023
).
Architecture and connectivity of the human angular gyrus and of its homolog region in the macaque brain
.
Brain Struct Funct
,
228
(
1
),
47
61
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-022-02509-7
O’Donnell
,
K. J.
,
Jensen
Bugge
, A.,
Freeman
,
L.
,
Khalife
,
N.
,
O’Connor
,
T. G.
, &
Glover
,
V.
(
2012
).
Maternal prenatal anxiety and downregulation of placental 11beta-HSD2
.
Psychoneuroendocrinology
,
37
(
6
),
818
826
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.09.014
Omidvarnia
,
A.
,
Fransson
,
P.
,
Metsaranta
,
M.
, &
Vanhatalo
,
S.
(
2014
).
Functional bimodality in the brain networks of preterm and term human newborns
.
Cereb Cortex
,
24
(
10
),
2657
2668
. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bht120
Padmanabhan
,
A.
,
Lynch
,
C. J.
,
Schaer
,
M.
, &
Menon
,
V.
(
2017
).
The default mode network in autism
.
Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging
,
2
(
6
),
476
486
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.04.004
Perperoglou
,
A.
,
Sauerbrei
,
W.
,
Abrahamowicz
,
M.
, &
Schmid
,
M.
(
2019
).
A review of spline function procedures in R
.
BMC Med Res Methodol
,
19
(
1
),
46
. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12874-019-0666-3
Plummer
,
M.
(
2003
).
JAGS: A program for analysis of Bayesian graphical models using GIbbs sampling
.
Proceedings of the 3rd international workshop on distributed statistical computing
(Vol.
124
, pp.
1
-
10
). https://www.r-project.org/conferences/DSC-2003/Drafts/Plummer.pdf
Power
,
J. D.
,
Barnes
,
K. A.
,
Snyder
,
A. Z.
,
Schlaggar
,
B. L.
, &
Petersen
,
S. E.
(
2012
).
Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion
.
Neuroimage
,
59
(
3
),
2142
2154
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.018
Power
,
J. D.
,
Fair
,
D. A.
,
Schlaggar
,
B. L.
, &
Petersen
,
S. E.
(
2010
).
The development of human functional brain networks
.
Neuron
,
67
(
5
),
735
748
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2010.08.017
Rajagopalan
,
V.
,
Reynolds
,
W. T.
,
Zepeda
,
J.
,
Lopez
,
J.
,
Ponrartana
,
S.
,
Wood
,
J.
,
Ceschin
,
R.
, &
Panigrahy
,
A.
(
2022
).
Impact of COVID-19 related maternal stress on fetal brain development: A multimodal MRI study
.
J Clin Med
,
11
(
22
),
6625
. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm11226635
Reid
,
V. M.
,
Dunn
,
K.
,
Young
,
R. J.
,
Amu
,
J.
,
Donovan
,
T.
, &
Reissland
,
N.
(
2017
).
The human fetus preferentially engages with face-like visual stimuli
.
Curr Biol
,
27
(
13
),
2052
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2017.06.036
Richardson
,
H.
,
Lisandrelli
,
G.
,
Riobueno-Naylor
,
A.
, &
Saxe
,
R.
(
2018
).
Development of the social brain from age three to twelve years
.
Nat Commun
,
9
(
1
),
1027
. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-018-03399-2
Rutherford
,
S.
,
Sturmfels
,
P.
,
Angstadt
,
M.
,
Hect
,
J.
,
Wiens
,
J.
,
van den Heuvel
,
I. M.
,
Scheinost
,
D.
,
Sripada
,
C.
, &
Thomason
,
M.
(
2022
).
Automated brain masking of fetal functional MRI with open data
.
Neuroinformatics
,
20
(
1
),
173
185
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12021-021-09528-5
Scheinost
,
D.
,
Chang
,
J.
,
Lacadie
,
C.
,
Brennan-Wydra
,
E.
,
Constable
,
T.
,
Chawarska
,
K.
, &
Ment
,
L. R.
(
2021
).
Functional connectivity for the language network in the developing brain: 30 weeks of gestation to 30 months of age
.
Cereb Cortex¸
32
(
15
),
3289
3301
. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhab415
Scheinost
,
D.
,
Kwon
,
S. H.
,
Lacadie
,
C.
,
Sze
,
G.
,
Sinha
,
R.
,
Constable
,
R. T.
, &
Ment
,
L. R.
(
2016
).
Prenatal stress alters amygdala functional connectivity in preterm neonates
.
Neuroimage Clin
,
12
,
381
388
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2016.08.010
Scheinost
,
D.
,
Kwon
,
S. H.
,
Lacadie
,
C.
,
Vohr
,
B. R.
,
Schneider
,
K. C.
,
Papademetris
,
X.
,
Constable
,
R. T.
, &
Ment
,
L. R.
(
2017
).
Alterations in anatomical covariance in the prematurely born
.
Cereb Cortex
,
27
(
1
),
534
543
. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhv248
Scheinost
,
D.
,
Kwon
,
S. H.
,
Shen
,
X.
,
Lacadie
,
C.
,
Schneider
,
K. C.
,
Dai
,
F.
,
Ment
,
L. R.
, &
Constable
,
R. T.
(
2016
).
Preterm birth alters neonatal, functional rich club organization
.
Brain Struct Funct
,
221
(
6
),
3211
3222
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-015-1096-6
Scheinost
,
D.
,
Lacadie
,
C.
,
Vohr
,
B. R.
,
Schneider
,
K. C.
,
Papademetris
,
X.
,
Constable
,
R. T.
, &
Ment
,
L. R.
(
2015
).
Cerebral lateralization is protective in the very prematurely born
.
Cereb Cortex
,
25
(
7
),
1858
1866
. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bht430
Scheinost
,
D
,
A.,
Onofrey, J.
Kwon
,
S. H.
,
Cross
,
S. N.
,
Sze
,
G., R.
,
Ment
,
L.
, &
Papademetris
,
X.
(
2018
).
A fetal fMRI specific motion correction algorithm using 2nd order edge features
.
Paper presented at the 2018 IEEE 15th International Symposium on Biomedical Imaging (ISBI 2018)
. https://doi.org/10.1109/isbi.2018.8363807
Scheinost
,
D.
,
Papademetris
,
X.
, &
Constable
,
R. T.
(
2014
).
The impact of image smoothness on intrinsic functional connectivity and head motion confounds
.
Neuroimage
,
95
,
13
21
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.03.035
Scheinost
,
D.
,
Spann
,
M. N.
,
McDonough
,
L.
,
Peterson
,
B. S.
, &
Monk
,
C.
(
2020
).
Associations between different dimensions of prenatal distress, neonatal hippocampal connectivity, and infant memory
.
Neuropsychopharmacology
,
45
(
8
),
1272
1279
. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-020-0677-0
Seeley
,
W. W
,
Menon
,
V.
,
Schatzberg
,
A. F.
,
Keller
,
J.
,
Glover
,
G. H.
,
Kenna
,
H.
,
Reiss
,
A. L.
, &
Greicius
,
M. D.
(
2007
).
Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control
.
J Neurosci
,
27
(
9
),
2349
2356
. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.5587-06.2007
Seghier
,
M. L.
(
2013
).
The angular gyrus: Multiple functions and multiple subdivisions
.
Neuroscientist
,
19
(
1
),
43
61
. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858412440596
Seshamani
,
S.
,
Blazejewska
,
A. I.
,
McKown
,
S.
,
Caucutt
,
J.
,
Dighe
,
M.
,
Gatenby
,
C.
, &
Studholme
,
C.
(
2016
).
Detecting default mode networks in utero by integrated 4D fMRI reconstruction and analysis
.
Hum Brain Mapp
,
37
(
11
),
4158
4178
. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23303
Shaw
,
S. B.
,
McKinnon
,
M. C.
,
Heisz
,
J.
, &
Becker
,
S.
(
2021
).
Dynamic task-linked switching between brain networks - A tri-network perspective
.
Brain Cogn
,
151
,
105725
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2021.105725
Shorey
,
S.
,
Chee
,
C. Y. I.
,
Ng
,
E. D.
,
Chan
,
Y. H.
,
Tam
,
W. W. S.
, &
Chong
,
Y. S.
(
2018
).
Prevalence and incidence of postpartum depression among healthy mothers: A systematic review and meta-analysis
.
J Psychiatr Res
,
104
,
235
248
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2018.08.001
Sigar
,
P.
,
Uddin
,
L. Q.
, &
Roy
,
D.
(
2023
).
Altered global modular organization of intrinsic functional connectivity in autism arises from atypical node-level processing
.
Autism Res
,
16
(
1
),
66
83
. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.2840
Smallwood
,
J.
,
Bernhardt
,
B. C.
,
Leech
,
R.
,
Bzdok
,
D.
,
Jefferies
,
E.
, &
Margulies
,
D. S.
(
2021
).
The default mode network in cognition: A topographical perspective
.
Nat Rev Neurosci
,
22
(
8
),
503
513
. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41583-021-00474-4
Smyser
,
C. D.
,
Inder
,
T. E.
,
Shimony
,
J. S.
,
Hill
,
J. E.
,
Degnan
,
A. J.
,
Snyder
,
A. Z.
, &
Neil
,
J. J.
(
2010
).
Longitudinal analysis of neural network development in preterm infants
.
Cereb Cortex
,
20
(
12
),
2852
2862
. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhq035
Smyser
,
C. D.
,
Snyder
,
A. Z.
, &
Neil
,
J. J.
(
2011
).
Functional connectivity MRI in infants: Exploration of the functional organization of the developing brain
.
Neuroimage
,
56
(
3
),
1437
1452
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.073
Snyder
,
W.
,
Uddin
,
L. Q.
, &
Nomi
,
J. S.
(
2021
).
Dynamic functional connectivity profile of the salience network across the life span
.
Hum Brain Mapp
,
42
(
14
),
4740
4749
. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.25581
Sobell
,
L. C.
, &
Sobell
,
M. B.
(
1992
).
Timeline follow-back: A technique for assessing selfreported alcohol consumption
. In
R. Z.
Litten
&
J.
Allen
(Eds.),
Measuring alcohol consumption: Psychosocial and biological methods
(pp.
41
72
).
New Jersey
:
Humana Press
. ISBN 1461203570, 9781461203575.
Spann
,
M. N.
,
Monk
,
C.
,
Scheinost
,
D.
, &
Peterson
,
B. S.
(
2018
).
Maternal immune activation during the third trimester is associated with neonatal functional connectivity of the salience network and fetal to toddler behavior
.
J Neurosci
,
38
(
11
),
2877
2886
. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2272-17.2018
Spielberger
,
C. D.
, &
Sydeman
,
S. J.
(
1994
).
State-Trait Anxiety Inventory and State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory
. In
M. E.
Maruish
(Ed.),
The Use of Psychological Testing for Treatment Planning and Outcome Assessment
(pp.
292
321
).
Hillsdale, NJ, USA
:
L. Erlbaum Associates
. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/9780470479216.corpsy0943
Sridharan
,
D.
,
Levitin
,
D. J.
, &
Menon
,
V.
(
2008
).
A critical role for the right fronto-insular cortex in switching between central-executive and default-mode networks
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
,
105
(
34
),
12569
12574
. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0800005105
Supekar
,
K.
,
Uddin
,
L. Q.
,
Prater
,
K.
,
Amin
,
H.
,
Greicius
,
M. D.
, &
Menon
,
V.
(
2010
).
Development of functional and structural connectivity within the default mode network in young children
.
Neuroimage
,
52
(
1
),
290
301
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.009
Sylvester
,
C. M.
,
Kaplan
,
S.
,
Myers
,
M. J.
,
Gordon
,
E. M.
,
Schwarzlose
,
R. F.
,
Alexopoulos
,
D.
,
Nielsen
,
A. N.
,
Kenley
,
J. K.
,
Meyer
,
D.
,
Yu
,
Q.
,
Graham
,
A. M.
,
Fair
,
D. A.
,
Warner
,
B. B.
,
Barch
,
D. M.
,
Rogers
,
C. E.
,
Luby
,
J. L.
,
Petersen
,
S. E.
, &
Smyser
,
C. D.
(
2023
).
Network-specific selectivity of functional connections in the neonatal brain
.
Cereb Cortex
,
33
(
5
),
2200
2214
. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhac202
Sylvester
,
C. M.
,
Smyser
,
C. D.
,
Smyser
,
T.
,
Kenley
,
J.
,
Ackerman
,
J. J.
, Jr.,
Shimony
,
J. S.
,
Petersen
,
S. E.
, &
Rogers
,
C. E.
(
2018
).
Cortical functional connectivity evident after birth and behavioral inhibition at Age 2
.
Am J Psychiatry
,
175
(
2
),
180
187
. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2017.17010018
Thomason
,
M. E.
(
2020
).
Development of brain networks in utero: Relevance for common neural disorders
.
Biol Psychiatry
,
88
(
1
),
40
50
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2020.02.007
Thomason
,
M. E.
,
Brown
,
J. A.
,
Dassanayake
,
M. T.
,
Shastri
,
R.
,
Marusak
,
H. A.
,
Hernandez-Andrade
,
E.
,
Yeo
,
L.
,
Mody
,
S.
,
Berman
,
S.
,
Hassan
,
S. S.
, &
Romero
,
R.
(
2014
).
Intrinsic functional brain architecture derived from graph theoretical analysis in the human fetus
.
PLoS One
,
9
(
5
),
e94423
. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0094423
Thomason
,
M. E.
,
Dassanayake
,
M. T.
,
Shen
,
S.
,
Katkuri
,
Y.
,
Alexis
,
M.
,
Anderson
,
A. L.
,
Yeo
,
L.
,
Mody
,
S.
,
Hernandez-Andrade
,
E.
,
Hassan
,
S. S.
,
Studholme
,
C.
,
Jeong
,
J. W.
, &
Romero
,
R.
(
2013
).
Cross-hemispheric functional connectivity in the human fetal brain
.
Sci Transl Med
,
5
(
173
),
173ra124
. https://doi.org/10.1126/scitranslmed.3004978
Thomason
,
M. E.
,
Grove
,
L. E.
,
Lozon
,
T. A.
, Jr.,
Vila
,
A. M.
,
Ye
,
Y.
,
Nye
,
M. J.
,
Manning
,
J. H.
,
Pappas
,
A.
,
Hernandez-Andrade
,
E.
,
Yeo
,
L.
,
Mody
,
S.
,
Berman
,
S.
,
Hassan
,
S. S.
, &
Romero
,
R.
(
2015
).
Age-related increases in long-range connectivity in fetal functional neural connectivity networks in utero
.
Dev Cogn Neurosci
,
11
,
96
104
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2014.09.001
Thomason
,
M. E.
,
Hect
,
J. L.
,
Waller
,
R.
, &
Curtin
,
P.
(
2021
).
Interactive relations between maternal prenatal stress, fetal brain connectivity, and gestational age at delivery
.
Neuropsychopharmacology
,
46
(
10
),
1839
1847
. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-021-01066-7
Thomason
,
M. E.
,
Scheinost
,
D.
,
Manning
,
J. H.
,
Grove
,
L. E.
,
Hect
,
J.
,
Marshall
,
N.
,
Hernandez-Andrade
,
E.
,
Berman
,
S.
,
Pappas
,
A.
,
Yeo
,
L.
,
Hassan
,
S. S.
,
Constable
,
R. T.
,
Ment
,
L. R.
, &
Romero
,
R.
(
2017
).
Weak functional connectivity in the human fetal brain prior to preterm birth
.
Sci Rep
,
7
,
39286
. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep39286
Tierney
,
A. L.
, &
Nelson
,
C. A.
, 3rd
. (
2009
).
Brain development and the role of experience in the early years
.
Zero Three
,
30
(
2
),
9
13
. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470939413.ch2
Turk
,
E.
,
van den Heuvel
,
I. M.
,
Benders
,
M. J.
,
de Heus
,
R.
,
Franx
,
A.
,
Manning
,
J. H.
,
Hect
,
J. L.
,
Hernandez-Andrade
,
E.
,
Hassan
,
S. S.
,
Romero
,
R.
,
Kahn
,
R. S.
,
Thomason
,
M. E.
, &
van den Heuvel
,
P. M.
(
2019
).
Functional connectome of the fetal brain
.
J Neurosci
,
39
(
49
),
9716
9724
. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2891-18.2019
Uddin
,
L. Q.
(
2015
).
Salience processing and insular cortical function and dysfunction
.
Nat Rev Neurosci
,
16
(
1
),
55
61
. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3857
Uddin
,
L. Q.
,
Supekar
,
K.
, &
Menon
,
V.
(
2010
).
Typical and atypical development of functional human brain networks: Insights from resting-state FMRI
.
Front Syst Neurosci
,
4
,
21
. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2010.00021
Uddin
,
L. Q.
,
Supekar
,
K. S.
,
Ryali
,
S.
, &
Menon
,
V.
(
2011
).
Dynamic reconfiguration of structural and functional connectivity across core neurocognitive brain networks with development
.
J Neurosci
,
31
(
50
),
18578
18589
. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4465-11.2011
Van den Bergh
,
B. R. H.
,
van den Heuvel
,
M. I.
,
Lahti
,
M.
,
Braeken
,
M.
,
de Rooij
,
S. R.
,
Entringer
,
S.
,
Hoyer
,
D.
,
Roseboom
,
T.
,
Raikkonen
,
K.
,
King
,
S.
, &
Schwab
,
M.
(
2017
).
Prenatal developmental origins of behavior and mental health: The influence of maternal stress in pregnancy
.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev
,
117
,
26
64
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.07.003
van den Heuvel
,
M. I.
,
Hect
,
J. L.
,
Smarr
,
B. L.
,
Qawasmeh
,
T.
,
Kriegsfeld
,
L. J.
,
Barcelona
,
J.
,
Hijazi
,
K. E.
, &
Thomason
,
M. E.
(
2021
).
Maternal stress during pregnancy alters fetal cortico-cerebellar connectivity in utero and increases child sleep problems after birth
.
Sci Rep
,
11
(
1
),
2228
. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-81681-y
van den Heuvel
,
M. I.
, &
Thomason
,
M. E.
(
2016
).
Functional connectivity of the human brain in utero
.
Trends Cogn Sci
,
20
(
12
),
931
939
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.10.001
Weinberger
,
A. H.
,
Reutenauer
,
E. L.
,
Allen
,
T. M.
,
Termine
,
A.
,
Vessicchio
,
J. C.
,
Sacco
,
K. A.
,
Easton
,
C. J.
,
McKee
,
S. A.
, &
George
,
T. P.
(
2007
).
Reliability of the Fagerstrom Test for Nicotine Dependence, Minnesota Nicotine Withdrawal Scale, and Tiffany Questionnaire for Smoking Urges in smokers with and without schizophrenia
.
Drug Alcohol Depend
,
86
(
2-3
),
278
282
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.06.005
Yeshurun
,
Y.
,
Nguyen
,
M.
, &
Hasson
,
U.
(
2021
).
The default mode network: Where the idiosyncratic self meets the shared social world
.
Nat Rev Neurosci
,
22
(
3
),
181
192
. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41583-020-00420-w
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International (CC BY 4.0) license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. For a full description of the license, please visit https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/legalcode.

Supplementary data