Successful memory retrieval relies on memory processes to access an internal representation and decision processes to evaluate and respond to the accessed representation, both of which are supported by fluctuations in theta (4–8 Hz) activity. However, the extent to which decision-making processes are engaged following a memory response is unclear. Here, we recorded scalp electroencephalography (EEG) while human participants performed a recognition memory task. We focused on response-locked data, allowing us to investigate the processes that occur prior to and following a memory response. We replicate previous work and find that prior to a memory response theta power is greater for identification of previously studied items (hits) relative to rejection of novel lures (correct rejections; CRs). Following the memory response, the theta power dissociation “flips” whereby theta power is greater for CRs relative to hits. We find that the post-response “flip” is more robust for hits that are committed quickly, potentially reflecting a positive feedback signal for strongly remembered experiences. Our findings suggest that there are potentially distinct processes occurring before and after a memory response that are modulated by successful memory retrieval.

Successful remembering is dependent on both memory processes to access stored representations and decision processes to evaluate and respond to the accessed representation. How these processes unfold over time, and the underlying neural mechanisms, are critically important to memory success, yet our understanding of these processes remains limited. In particular, convergent evidence from scalp electroencephalography (EEG) studies in both the memory literature (Klimesch et al., 1997; Nyhus & Curran, 2010) and the decision-making literature (Frank et al., 2015; Pinner & Cavanagh, 2017; Senftleben & Scherbaum, 2021) suggest that frontocentral theta (4–8 Hz activity in EEG) supports both memory and decision-making processes. However, the extent to which theta dissociations in a memory task reflect decision-making processes is unclear, likely due to limited investigation of EEG signals following a memory response. The aim of this study is to investigate the neural correlates leading up to and following a memory response.

It is well established that successful remembering is characterized by electrophysiological changes around 300 to 800 ms following stimulus onset during a memory test (Addante et al., 2012; Friedman & Johnson Jr., 2000; Rugg & Wilding, 2000; Voss & Paller, 2008). Specifically, two event-related potentials (ERPs) distinguish successful remembering of a target or studied item (hit) from successful rejection of a lure or non-studied item (correct rejection, CR). The FN400, a negative going frontal ERP component thought to reflect familiarity (Curran & Hancock, 2007; Mecklinger, 2000; Rugg et al., 1998), is more negative for CRs than hits around 300 to 500 ms after stimulus onset (Curran, 2000, 2004; Curran & Cleary, 2003) and the LPC, a late positive going parietal ERP component thought to reflect recollection (Friedman & Johnson Jr., 2000; Mecklinger, 2000), is more positive for hits than CRs around 400 to 800 ms after stimulus onset (Curran, 2000, 2004; Curran & Cleary, 2003; Friedman & Johnson Jr., 2000). Similarly, theta power is greater for hits compared to CRs, most often around 500 to 1000 ms after stimulus onset (Burgess & Gruzelier, 1997; Düzel et al., 2003; Klimesch et al., 2000). According to the drift diffusion model, recognition memory is supported by a mechanism whereby evidence accumulates over time until a threshold is reached and a decision is made (Ratcliff & McKoon, 2008). Given evidence that theta power is positively correlated with reaction times, such that theta power increases until a response is made (Jacobs et al., 2006), theta power prior to a response may reflect evidence accumulation and/or reinstatement (Guan et al., 2023; Herweg et al., 2020; Kota et al., 2020; Nyhus & Curran, 2010). However, these effects are related to representation access prior to a response and as such, do not elucidate the processes that may unfold after a memory response is made.

Parallel findings from the decision-making and cognitive control literature have revealed that both ERPs and theta power track errors and negative feedback signals prior to and following a response (Cavanagh & Frank, 2014; Cavanagh et al., 2010; Luft, 2014; Luu et al., 2004; Trujillo & Allen, 2007). Specifically, the error-related negativity (ERN), a negative going fronto central ERP component that reflects decision conflict or error monitoring (Frank et al., 2005; L. Wang et al., 2020), is more negative following incorrect compared to correct responses (Cavanagh et al., 2012; Gehring et al., 1993). Similarly, across cognitive control tasks such as the Stroop task, the flanker task, and go/no-go tasks, theta power is greater following incorrect compared to correct responses and following negative relative to positive outcomes (Cavanagh & Frank, 2014; Cohen, 2014b; Mazaheri et al., 2009). Together, these findings suggest that post-response theta power may reflect a feedback signal or monitoring process.

Although there is evidence for post-retrieval monitoring during recognition memory (Hill et al., 2021; Rugg et al., 2003), these signals are often measured following access of the representation, but prior to the memory response itself. Further complicating interpretation is that reaction times are generally faster for hits than CRs (Uncapher et al., 2015; Weidemann & Kahana, 2016), meaning that stimulus-locked hit versus CR dissociations may include both pre- and post-response related processes. That is, insofar as an evaluative or updating decision-making process is engaged after a memory response has been made, stimulus-locked comparisons may contrast post-hit evaluative decision-making processing with pre-CR memory retrieval-related processing. There is ERP evidence that post-retrieval monitoring processes are supported by a late old/new effect over right frontal cortex (Johansson & Mecklinger, 2003; Wilding & Rugg, 1996), characterized by a positive voltage deflection that is greater for hits than CRs (Hayama et al., 2008). However, the majority of such post-retrieval monitoring signals occur after the putative representation has been accessed, but before a behavioral response is made (Cruse & Wilding, 2009, 2011; Woodruff et al., 2006), leaving open the question of whether decision-making mechanisms are engaged following a memory response. Greater conflict between memory decisions in a recognition task—created via differential payoff rates for correct old versus new responses—leads to a greater post-response ERN (Curran, DeBuse, et al., 2007), suggesting that control or monitoring processes may be engaged after a memory response is made.

Our hypothesis is that distinct processes occur prior to and following a memory response. To test our hypothesis, we conducted a human scalp EEG recognition memory study in which we specifically assessed response-locked theta power and provided no explicit feedback to participants. By investigating response-locked signals, we can separately assess pre- and post-response related processing during both hits and CRs. We expected to replicate prior work and find greater theta power for hits compared to CRs preceding the response (equivalent to the established stimulus-locked effects, e.g., Burgess & Gruzelier, 1997; Düzel et al., 2003; Nyhus & Curran, 2010). To the extent that distinct, and potentially decision-making-related, processes are engaged following a memory response, we expected to find a post-response theta pattern that differed from the pre-response hit versus CR effect. First, if there are neither memory nor decision-making-related signals following a memory response, theta power following both hits and CRs should return to baseline. Alternatively, because both hits and CRs are correct responses, theta power may be similarly decreased for both response types following a memory response. Finally, given that the reward system—in particular, the striatum—has previously been linked to successful memory retrieval, whereby the striatum shows greater activity for hits compared to CRs (Clos et al., 2015; Spaniol et al., 2009), successful retrieval may be intrinsically rewarding (Satterthwaite et al., 2012; Speer et al., 2014). Therefore, theta power may dissociate post-response hits from CRs such that theta power would be greater for CRs compared to hits following the memory response, reflecting a positive feedback signal for hits. The direct comparison of two classes of responses that are both accurate, but differ in terms of successful retrieval, enables adjudication between these alternative hypotheses.

2.1 Participants

Forty (30 female; age range = 18–42, mean age = 21.9 years) native English speakers from the University of Virginia community participated. Our sample size of N = 40 was selected based on prior scalp EEG studies conducted in our lab (Moore & Long, 2024; Smith et al., 2022). All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Informed consent was obtained in accordance with University of Virginia Institutional Review Board for Social and Behavioral Research, and participants were compensated for their participation. Two participants were excluded from the final dataset: one for technical difficulties that resulted in a subset of test items being presented twice during the test phase and one who failed to comply with task instructions. Thus, data are reported for the remaining 38 participants. All raw, de-identified data and the associated experimental and analysis codes used in this study can be accessed via the Long Term Memory Lab Website (https://longtermmemorylab.com).

2.2 Recognition task experimental design

Stimuli consisted of 1602 words, drawn from the Toronto Noun Pool (Friendly et al., 1982). From this set, 288 words were randomly selected for each participant. Of these words, 192 were presented in both the study and test phase while the remaining 96 served as lures in the test phase.

2.2.1 Study phase

In each of 12 runs, participants viewed a list containing 16 words, yielding a total of 192 trials. During each trial, participants saw a single word presented for 2000 ms followed by a 1000 ms inter-stimulus interval (ISI; Fig. 1A). As we did not want to bias participants to the semantic features of the study items (Moore & Long, 2024), we did not include an encoding task. Instead, participants were instructed to study the presented word in anticipation for a later memory test and did not make any behavioral responses. An earlier motivation of this work was to investigate how semantic associations among study items influence memory formation mechanisms. To that end, each list was split evenly into two parts containing 8 words (“first associates” and “second associates,” respectively) separated by a brief 2000 ms delay. Semantic association strength was determined using Word Association Space values (WAS; Nelson et al., 2001); “strong” semantic associates had a WAS value of 0.4 or greater, and “weak” semantic associates had a WAS value less than 0.4 (Long & Kahana, 2017). Half of the first and second associates were strongly semantically associated, and half of the first and second associates were weakly semantically associated. Both strong and weak semantic associates were weakly semantically associated to all other study words. Word lists were generated for each participant by randomly drawing a word from the pool of 1602 words and selecting either a strong or weak associate from the word pool and then removing the selected word, selected associate, and all other strong semantic associates of the selected word, from the pool. We iteratively repeated this process until a total of 192 words were selected.

Fig. 1.

Task design. (A) During the study phase, participants studied individual words in anticipation of a later memory test and made no behavioral responses. After 12 runs of 16-item word lists, participants completed a recognition test phase. On each trial, participants saw either a target, a word that was presented during the study phase, or a lure, a word that was not presented during the study phase. The participants’ task was to make an old or new judgment for each word. There were one of four possible response types, hits (teal; an “old” response to a target), correct rejections (orange; a “new” response to a lure), misses (dashed black lines; a “new” response to a target), and false alarms (dotted black lines; an “old” response to a lure). Lines and colors around the boxes are shown for illustrative purposes and were not present during the actual experiment. (B) We analyzed two regions of interest (ROIs), a left central ROI (FC5, FC1, C3, CP5, CP1, FC3, C1, C5, CP3) and a right central ROI (CP6, CP2, C4, FC6, FC2, CP4, C6, C2, FC4).

Fig. 1.

Task design. (A) During the study phase, participants studied individual words in anticipation of a later memory test and made no behavioral responses. After 12 runs of 16-item word lists, participants completed a recognition test phase. On each trial, participants saw either a target, a word that was presented during the study phase, or a lure, a word that was not presented during the study phase. The participants’ task was to make an old or new judgment for each word. There were one of four possible response types, hits (teal; an “old” response to a target), correct rejections (orange; a “new” response to a lure), misses (dashed black lines; a “new” response to a target), and false alarms (dotted black lines; an “old” response to a lure). Lines and colors around the boxes are shown for illustrative purposes and were not present during the actual experiment. (B) We analyzed two regions of interest (ROIs), a left central ROI (FC5, FC1, C3, CP5, CP1, FC3, C1, C5, CP3) and a right central ROI (CP6, CP2, C4, FC6, FC2, CP4, C6, C2, FC4).

Close modal

2.2.2 Test phase

Following the 12 study runs, participants completed the recognition test phase. On each trial, participants viewed a word which had either been presented during the study phase (target) or had not been presented (lure; Fig. 1A). Participants’ task was to make an old or new judgment for each word by pressing one of two buttons (“d” or “k”). Response mappings were counterbalanced across participants. Test trials were self-paced, and responses could occur anytime after the stimulus onset. Participants received no feedback on the accuracy of their responses. Test trials were separated by a 1000 ms ISI. There were a total of 288 test trials with all 192 study words presented along with 96 novel lures, half of which were semantically associated to a study word. As we find similar effects when accounting for semantic associations (see Results), we collapsed analyses across strong and weak semantic associates.

2.3 EEG data acquisition and preprocessing

All acquisition and preprocessing methods are based on our previous work (Smith et al., 2022); for clarity we use the same text as previously reported. EEG recordings were collected using a BrainVision system and an ActiCap equipped with 64 Ag/AgCl active electrodes positioned according to the extended 10–20 system. All electrodes were digitized at a sampling rate of 1000 Hz and were referenced to electrode FCz. Offline, electrodes were later converted to an average reference. Impedances of all electrodes were kept below 50 kΩ. Electrodes that demonstrated high impedance or poor contact with the scalp were excluded from the average reference. Bad electrodes were determined by voltage thresholding (see below).

Custom python codes were used to process the EEG data. We applied a high-pass filter at 0.1 Hz, followed by a notch filter at 60 Hz and harmonics of 60 Hz to each participant’s raw EEG data. We then performed three preprocessing steps (Nolan et al., 2010) to identify electrodes with severe artifacts. First, we calculated the mean correlation between each electrode and all other electrodes as electrodes should be moderately correlated with other electrodes due to volume conduction. We z-scored these means across electrodes and rejected electrodes with z-scores less than -3. Second, we calculated the variance for each electrode, as electrodes with very high or low variance across a session are likely dominated by noise or have poor contact with the scalp. We then z-scored variance across electrodes and rejected electrodes with a |z|> = 3. Finally, we expect many electrical signals to be autocorrelated, but signals generated by the brain versus noise are likely to have different forms of autocorrelation. Therefore, we calculated the Hurst exponent, a measure of long-range autocorrelation, for each electrode and rejected electrodes with a |z|> = 3. Electrodes marked as bad by this procedure were excluded from the average re-reference. We then calculated the average voltage across all remaining electrodes at each time sample and re-referenced the data by subtracting the average voltage from the filtered EEG data. We used wavelet-enhanced independent component analysis (Castellanos & Makarov, 2006) to remove artifacts from eyeblinks and saccades.

2.4 EEG data analysis

We applied the Morlet wavelet transform (wave number 6) to the entire EEG time series across electrodes, for each of 46 logarithmically spaced frequencies (2–100 Hz; Long & Kahana, 2015). Because we hypothesized distinct processes occur prior to and following a memory response, after log-transforming the power we focused exclusively on test-phase data. We then downsampled the test-phase data by taking a moving average across 100 ms time intervals from -1000 to 3000 ms relative to the response and sliding the window every 25 ms, resulting in 157 time intervals (40 non-overlapping). Mean and standard deviation power were calculated across all trials and across time points for each frequency. Power values were then z-transformed by subtracting the mean and dividing by the standard deviation power. We focus exclusively on the theta band (4–8 Hz) for all analyses.

2.5 Regions of interest

We examined theta power across two regions of interest (ROIs; Fig. 1B): left central (FC5, FC1, C3, CP5, CP1, FC3, C1, C5, CP3) and right central (CP6, CP2, C4, FC6, FC2, CP4, C6, C2, FC4). We specifically focus on the frontocentral region as prior work has demonstrated that theta power in this region dissociates both hits and correct rejections (Burgess & Gruzelier, 1997; Gruber et al., 2008; Klimesch et al., 1997; Nyhus & Curran, 2010) and positive and negative feedback (Cohen et al., 2007; Marco-Pallarés et al., 2008; Mas-Herrero et al., 2015).

2.6 Univariate analyses

To test the effect of response type on theta power leading up to and following memory responses, our two conditions of interest were hits (correctly recognized targets) and correct rejections (CRs, correctly rejected lures). We compared theta power across hits and CRs separately for each ROI. For each participant, we calculated z-transformed theta power across both ROIs in each of the two conditions, across 100 ms time intervals from 500 ms pre-response to 1000 ms post-response. For a direct comparison of pre-response and post-response theta power, we further averaged z-transformed theta power over the 500 ms pre-response and 500 ms post-response interval separately for hits and CRs. We selected 500 ms as our pre-response interval based on our prior work investigating contextually mediated retrieval processes in the hippocampus (Long et al., 2017).

2.7 Peak analysis

To measure the center frequency (CF) of theta leading up to and following memory responses, we used fitting oscillations and one over f (FOOOF; Ostlund et al., 2022). To specifically measure periodic signals, for each participant, we fit the FOOOF model to every test trial and extracted all identified CFs within the theta band prior to and following the response for both ROIs. We compared the averaged CF over the 500 ms pre-response and 500 ms post-response interval between hits and CRs.

2.8 Statistical analyses

We used a repeated-measures ANOVA (rmANOVA) to assess the distribution of reaction times (RTs) for hits and CRs. For post hoc comparisons across RTs, we used false discovery rate (FDR; p = 0.05) correction (Benjamini & Hochberg, 1995) to correct for multiple comparisons. We used rmANOVAs and paired-sample t-tests to assess the effect of response type (hits, CRs) and time interval (pre-response, post-response) on theta power.

Our first goal was to measure memory discrimination (d’) to ensure that participants were following directions and able to discriminate between targets and lures. For each participant, we calculated d’ by subtracting the normalized false alarm rate (the percentage of lures that were incorrectly identified as “old”) from the normalized hit rate (the percentage of targets that were correctly identified as “old”). The average d’ was 1.75 (SD = 0.58; Fig. 2A), indicating that participants were able to successfully distinguish targets from lures.

Fig. 2.

Memory discrimination and responses as a function of reaction time bin. (A) We used d’ to assess memory discrimination. Participants were able to correctly discriminate between targets and lures. (B) We assessed the proportion of hits (teal) and CRs (orange) as a function of RT bin. The highest proportion of hits and CRs occurs in the 750–1000 ms bin, and a significantly greater proportion of hits compared to CRs occur within the 500–750 ms bin. Error bars reflect standard error of the mean. *p< 0.05; ***p< 0.001 (FDR-corrected).

Fig. 2.

Memory discrimination and responses as a function of reaction time bin. (A) We used d’ to assess memory discrimination. Participants were able to correctly discriminate between targets and lures. (B) We assessed the proportion of hits (teal) and CRs (orange) as a function of RT bin. The highest proportion of hits and CRs occurs in the 750–1000 ms bin, and a significantly greater proportion of hits compared to CRs occur within the 500–750 ms bin. Error bars reflect standard error of the mean. *p< 0.05; ***p< 0.001 (FDR-corrected).

Close modal

Having found that participants are able to discriminate targets and lures, we next assessed median reaction times (RTs) for hits and CRs. To the extent that RTs reliably differ between hits and CRs, stimulus-locked neural dissociations between these conditions may be driven by engagement of different processes, for example, memory versus decision-making, rather than differential engagement of the same process. That is, if hits occur more quickly than CRs, a stimulus-locked comparison between the two conditions could reflect a comparison between post-response processes for hits versus pre-response processes for CRs. The average median RT for hits (M = 954.9, SD = 419.6) was significantly faster than for CRs (M = 1122.8, SD = 317.3, t37 = -4.427, p= 0.0001, d = 0.4514). This RT difference suggests that stimulus-locked theta dissociations could be driven by dissociations in pre- versus post-response processes across hits and CRs.

Given the dissociation in median RT between hits and CRs, we next sought to compare the distribution of RTs across conditions to determine when relative to stimulus onset the majority of hits and CRs occur. We grouped RTs into nine bins selected to cover the full range of RTs with higher resolution in the faster (<2500 ms) bins: 0–500 ms, 500–750 ms, 750–1000 ms, 1000–1250 ms, 1250–1500 ms, 1500–1750 ms, 1750–2000 ms, 2000–2500 ms, and 2500–10000 ms (Fig. 2B). We calculated the proportion of hits and CRs that occurred within each RT bin for each participant and then averaged those proportions across all participants. We conducted a 2 × 9 rmANOVA with factors of response type (hit, CR) and RT bin. We do not find a significant main effect of response type (F1,37 = 0.51, p= 0.479, ηp2= 0.01). We find a significant main effect of RT bin (F8,296 = 74.48, p< 0.0001, ηp2= 0.67) and a significant interaction between response type and RT bin (F8,296 = 22.36, p< 0.0001, ηp2= 0.38). We report the results of post hoc t-tests comparing proportions of hits and CRs within each RT bin in Table 1 and highlight the key findings below. We find that the highest proportion of responses occurs in the 750–1000 ms RT bin for both hits and CRs. However, a significantly greater proportion of hits (M = 0.28, SD = 0.19) occur within the 500–750 ms RT bin compared to CRs (M = 0.12, SD = 0.11). Thus, if participants evaluate or update a representation after responding, neural activity observed within the 500–750 ms interval may reflect a comparison of post-hit evaluation processes with pre-CR memory or retrieval-related processes.

Table 1.

Post hoc t-tests comparing the proportion of hits and CRs in each RT bin.

HitsCRsHits vs. CRs
RT bin (ms)MeanSDMeanSDt37pd
0–500 0.002 0.006 0.0004 0.002 1.579 0.1229 0.3252 
500–750 0.28 0.19 0.12 0.11 7.058 <0.0001 1.081 
750–1000 0.33 0.11 0.34 0.13 -0.466 0.6438 0.0898 
1000–1250 0.14 0.08 0.19 0.07 -4.198 0.0002 0.6536 
1250–1500 0.07 0.04 0.10 0.04 -4.425 0.0001 0.7708 
1500–1750 0.04 0.02 0.06 0.04 -2.549 0.02 0.4681 
1750–2000 0.02 0.02 0.04 0.03 -4.012 0.0003 0.7581 
2000–2500 0.03 0.03 0.05 0.03 -3.973 0.0003 0.6373 
2500–10000 0.07 0.10 0.09 0.10 -2.009 0.052 0.2192 
HitsCRsHits vs. CRs
RT bin (ms)MeanSDMeanSDt37pd
0–500 0.002 0.006 0.0004 0.002 1.579 0.1229 0.3252 
500–750 0.28 0.19 0.12 0.11 7.058 <0.0001 1.081 
750–1000 0.33 0.11 0.34 0.13 -0.466 0.6438 0.0898 
1000–1250 0.14 0.08 0.19 0.07 -4.198 0.0002 0.6536 
1250–1500 0.07 0.04 0.10 0.04 -4.425 0.0001 0.7708 
1500–1750 0.04 0.02 0.06 0.04 -2.549 0.02 0.4681 
1750–2000 0.02 0.02 0.04 0.03 -4.012 0.0003 0.7581 
2000–2500 0.03 0.03 0.05 0.03 -3.973 0.0003 0.6373 
2500–10000 0.07 0.10 0.09 0.10 -2.009 0.052 0.2192 

Note: Bold values indicate tests that survive FDR correction.

Our hypothesis is that distinct memory and decision-making processes occur preceding and following a memory response, meaning that we should find differential theta power engagement pre- and post-response. Specifically, we should replicate past findings of greater theta power for hits compared to CRs pre-response, reflecting memory-related processing. To the extent that decision-making processes are engaged following a response, we should either find decreased theta power for both hits and CRs—as both conditions are correct responses—or we may find a theta dissociation between hits and CRs. That is, to the extent that successful retrieval is intrinsically rewarding, post-response theta power should be decreased for hits compared to CRs. We conducted a 2 × 2 × 15 rmANOVA with factors of response type (hit, CR), ROI (left central; LC, right central; RC) and time interval (-500 to 1000 ms in fifteen 100 ms intervals). We report the results of this ANOVA in Table 2 and highlight the key findings here. We find a significant interaction between response type and time interval (F14,518 = 6.127, p< 0.0001, ηp2= 0.14) which indicates that theta power dissociations between hits and CRs changes over time.

Table 2.

Analysis of variance results for response type (hit, CR), ROI, and time interval (-500 to 1000 ms in fifteen 100 ms intervals) on theta power.

EffectdfFpηp2
Main effect of response type (1,37) 0.172 0.681 0.005 
Main effect of ROI (1,37) 11.36 0.002 0.23 
Main effect of time interval (14,518) 21.59 <0.0001 0.37 
Interaction of response type × ROI (1,37) 0.218 0.643 0.006 
Interaction of ROI × time interval (14,518) 3.408 <0.0001 0.08 
Interaction of response type × time interval (14,518) 6.127 <0.0001 0.14 
Interaction of response type × ROI × time interval (14,518) 2.23 0.006 0.06 
EffectdfFpηp2
Main effect of response type (1,37) 0.172 0.681 0.005 
Main effect of ROI (1,37) 11.36 0.002 0.23 
Main effect of time interval (14,518) 21.59 <0.0001 0.37 
Interaction of response type × ROI (1,37) 0.218 0.643 0.006 
Interaction of ROI × time interval (14,518) 3.408 <0.0001 0.08 
Interaction of response type × time interval (14,518) 6.127 <0.0001 0.14 
Interaction of response type × ROI × time interval (14,518) 2.23 0.006 0.06 

Note: Bold values indicate p< 0.05.

Given the significant three-way interaction between response type, ROI, and time interval, we next performed follow-up post hoc ANOVAs over time separately for each ROI. We conducted two 2 × 15 rmANOVAs with factors of response type (hit, CR) and time interval (-500 to 1000 ms in fifteen 100 ms intervals). We report the results of this ANOVA in Table 3 and highlight the key findings here. For both ROIs (Fig. 3), we find a significant interaction between response type and time interval (LC: F14,518 = 5.373, p< 0.0001, ηp2= 0.13; RC: F14,518 = 3.791, p< 0.0001, ηp2= 0.09). This interaction was also significant when we separately considered strong and weak semantic items (strong, LC: F14,518 = 3.297, p< 0.0001, ηp2= 0.08; strong, RC: F14,518 = 2.796, p= 0.0005, ηp2= 0.07; weak, LC: F14,518 = 4.232, p< 0.0001, ηp2= 0.10; weak, RC: F14,518 = 2.041, p= 0.0137, ηp2= 0.05). These results demonstrate that theta power dissociations between hits and CRs vary across time interval, suggesting that differential processes may be engaged in a pre- versus post-response that distinguishes these response types.

Table 3.

Analysis of variance results for response type (hit, CR) and time interval (-500 to 1000 ms in fifteen 100 ms intervals; pre-response, post-response) on theta power for the left and right central ROI.

Effectdf-500 to 1000 ms intervaldfPre vs. post response interval
Left centralRight centralLeft centralRight central
Fpηp2Fpηp2Fpηp2Fpηp2
Main effect of response type (1,37) 0.382 0.54 0.01 0.007 0.934 0.0002 (1,37) 0.009 0.925 0.0002 0.871 0.357 0.02 
Main effect of time interval (14,518) 19.76 <0.0001 0.35 20.91 <0.0001 0.36 (1,37) 5.832 0.0208 0.14 0.708 0.406 0.02 
Interaction of response type × time interval (14,518) 5.373 <0.0001 0.13 3.791 <0.0001 0.09 (1,37) 16.18 0.0003 0.30 5.306 0.027 0.13 
Effectdf-500 to 1000 ms intervaldfPre vs. post response interval
Left centralRight centralLeft centralRight central
Fpηp2Fpηp2Fpηp2Fpηp2
Main effect of response type (1,37) 0.382 0.54 0.01 0.007 0.934 0.0002 (1,37) 0.009 0.925 0.0002 0.871 0.357 0.02 
Main effect of time interval (14,518) 19.76 <0.0001 0.35 20.91 <0.0001 0.36 (1,37) 5.832 0.0208 0.14 0.708 0.406 0.02 
Interaction of response type × time interval (14,518) 5.373 <0.0001 0.13 3.791 <0.0001 0.09 (1,37) 16.18 0.0003 0.30 5.306 0.027 0.13 

Note: Bold values indicate p< 0.05.

Fig. 3.

Theta power dissociations across hits and correct rejections preceding and following memory responses. Response-locked z-transformed theta power (4–8 Hz) for the left and right central ROIs. The solid vertical black line indicates when the response was made. Hits are shown in teal, correct rejections (CRs) are shown in orange. Error bars reflect standard error of the mean. (A) Over the left central ROI, we find a significant interaction between response type and time interval (p< 0.001) driven by greater pre-response theta power for hits than CRs and numerically greater post-response theta power for CRs than hits. (B) Over the right central ROI, we find a significant interaction between response type and time interval (p = 0.027) driven by greater theta power for hits than CRs during the pre-response time interval.

Fig. 3.

Theta power dissociations across hits and correct rejections preceding and following memory responses. Response-locked z-transformed theta power (4–8 Hz) for the left and right central ROIs. The solid vertical black line indicates when the response was made. Hits are shown in teal, correct rejections (CRs) are shown in orange. Error bars reflect standard error of the mean. (A) Over the left central ROI, we find a significant interaction between response type and time interval (p< 0.001) driven by greater pre-response theta power for hits than CRs and numerically greater post-response theta power for CRs than hits. (B) Over the right central ROI, we find a significant interaction between response type and time interval (p = 0.027) driven by greater theta power for hits than CRs during the pre-response time interval.

Close modal

To specifically test for a pre- versus post-response dissociation in theta power for hits and CRs, we averaged signals within the 500 ms pre- and post-response time intervals (Fig. 3, insets). We conducted two 2 × 2 rmANOVAs, one for each ROI, with factors of response type (hit, CR) and time interval (pre-response, post-response). We report the results of this ANOVA in Table 3 and highlight the key findings here. In both ROIs, we find a significant interaction between response type and time interval. In LC, this interaction was driven by greater theta power for hits (M = 0.18, SD = 0.21) relative to CRs (M = 0.10, SD = 0.18) in the pre-response time interval (t37 = 2.559, p= 0.0147, d = 0.4459) and numerically greater theta power for CRs (M = 0.05, SD = 0.25) relative to hits (M = -0.03, SD = 0.29) in the post-response time interval (t37 = 2.021, p= 0.0506, d = 0.3019). In RC, this interaction was driven by numerically greater theta power for hits (M = 0.16, SD = 0.21) relative to CRs (M = 0.10, SD = 0.19) in the pre-response time interval (t37 = 1.911, p= 0.0637, d = 0.295) and no difference in theta power for hits (M = 0.08, SD = 0.24) relative to CRs (M = 0.08, SD = 0.29) in the post-response time interval (t37 = -0.1160, p= 0.9083, d = 0.015). We next tested the hemispheric specificity of this effect by directly comparing the post-response theta power dissociation across ROIs. We computed post-response difference scores (CRs minus hits) for each ROI. We find that the post-response theta power dissociation in LC (M = 0.081, SD = 0.25) does not significantly differ from the post-response theta power dissociation in RC (M = 0.004, SD = 0.21; t37 = 1.605, p= 0.1171), indicating that the effect is not specific to the left hemisphere. Together, these results indicate that theta power is modulated by successful retrieval leading up to and following memory responses.

As the theta band encompasses multiple frequencies (4–8 Hz), we next tested the extent to which the center frequency of theta differed across response type and time interval. To specifically measure periodic signals, we fit the fitting oscillations and one over f (FOOOF) model to every test trial and extracted all identified center frequencies within the theta band prior to and following the response for the left and right central ROI. We compared the center frequency between hits and CRs pre- and post-response. We do not find any differences in center frequency for either ROI or time interval (LC, pre-response: t37 = -0.0976, p= 0.9228; LC, post-response: t37 = 0.2201, p= 0.8270; RC, pre-response: t37 = -0.3237, p= 0.748; RC, post-response t37 = -1.072, p= 0.2908). The center frequency for all conditions is generally around 5.6 Hz.

Prior work (Guan et al., 2023; Herweg et al., 2020) suggests that the pre-response theta power dissociation that we observe specifically reflects evidence accumulation or reinstatement that ultimately supports recollection. The post-response theta power effects may likewise reflect a feedback signal in response to recollected content. Due to the current task design, we cannot directly measure recollection and familiarity; however, we can leverage reaction time (RT) as a coarse assay of confidence. The general assumption is that compared to trials with slow RTs, trials with fast RTs reflect faster evidence accumulation (Mulder & van Maanen, 2013; Rollwage et al., 2020; Shenhav et al., 2018) and greater confidence (Ratcliff, 1978; Ratcliff & Starns, 2009; Weidemann & Kahana, 2016). To divide the trials based on RT, we calculated the median RT across all correct responses (hits and CRs) for each participant, and labeled hits as either “fast” (those below the median RT) or “slow” (those above the median RT). To the extent that fast hits reflect strongly remembered experiences or the degree of evidence accumulation, we should find differential theta power engagement pre- and post-response. Specifically, we should find greater pre-response theta power for fast hits compared to CRs, as there is no experience to remember or reinstate during a CR. To the extent that post-response theta power reflects a feedback signal based on reinstated content, we should find decreased theta power for fast hits compared to CRs.

To specifically test for a pre- versus post-response dissociation in theta power for fast hits, slow hits, and CRs, we averaged signals within the 500 ms pre- and post-response time intervals (Fig. 4). We conducted two 2 × 3 rmANOVAs, one for each ROI, with factors of time interval (pre-response, post-response) and response type (fast hit, slow hit, CR). We report the results of this ANOVA in Table 4 and highlight the key findings here. In LC, we find a significant interaction between response type and time interval. This interaction was driven by a significant interaction between both fast hits and CRs (F1,37 = 24.56, p< 0.0001, ηp2= 0.40) and fast hits and slow hits (F1,37 = 15.02, p = 0.0004, ηp2= 0.29). The interaction between slow hits and CRs was not significant (F1,37 = 2.892, p= 0.0974, ηp2= 0.07). In the pre-response interval, theta power was significantly greater for fast hits (M = 0.22, SD = 0.23) relative to CRs (M = 0.10, SD = 0.18; t37 = 3.080, p = 0.0039, d = 0.5789) and numerically greater for fast hits relative to slow hits (M = 0.14, SD = 0.25; t37 = 1.908, p = 0.0642, d = 0.3256). In the post-response interval, theta power was significantly greater for CRs (M = 0.05, SD = 0.25) and slow hits (M = 0.02, SD = 0.28) relative to fast hits (M = -0.07, SD = 0.33; CRs vs. fast hits: t37 = 2.554, p= 0.0149, d = 0.4161; slow hits vs. fast hits: t37 = 2.606, p = 0.0131, d = 0.2918).

Table 4.

Analysis of variance results for response type (fast hit, slow hit, CR) and time interval (pre-response, post-response) on theta power for the left and right central ROI.

EffectdfLeft centralRight central
Fpηp2Fpηp2
Main effect of response type (2,74) 0.019 0.981 0.0005 1.938 0.151 0.05 
Main effect of time interval (1,37) 7.459 0.0096 0.17 0.87 0.357 0.02 
Interaction of response type × time interval (2,74) 14.93 <0.0001 0.29 4.095 0.0206 0.10 
EffectdfLeft centralRight central
Fpηp2Fpηp2
Main effect of response type (2,74) 0.019 0.981 0.0005 1.938 0.151 0.05 
Main effect of time interval (1,37) 7.459 0.0096 0.17 0.87 0.357 0.02 
Interaction of response type × time interval (2,74) 14.93 <0.0001 0.29 4.095 0.0206 0.10 

Note: Bold values indicate p< 0.05.

Fig. 4.

Theta power dissociations across fast hits, slow hits, and correct rejections preceding and following memory responses. Response-locked z-transformed theta power (4–8 Hz) for the left and right central ROIs. Fast hits are shown in dark teal, slow hits are shown in light teal, and correct rejections (CRs) are shown in orange. Error bars reflect standard error of the mean. (A) Over the left central ROI, we find a significant interaction between response type and time interval (p< 0.0001) driven by a significant pre-post interaction between fast hits and CRs (p< 0.0001). (B) Over the right central ROI, we find a significant interaction between response type and time interval (p = 0.0206) driven by a significant pre-post interaction between fast hits and CRs (p = 0.008).

Fig. 4.

Theta power dissociations across fast hits, slow hits, and correct rejections preceding and following memory responses. Response-locked z-transformed theta power (4–8 Hz) for the left and right central ROIs. Fast hits are shown in dark teal, slow hits are shown in light teal, and correct rejections (CRs) are shown in orange. Error bars reflect standard error of the mean. (A) Over the left central ROI, we find a significant interaction between response type and time interval (p< 0.0001) driven by a significant pre-post interaction between fast hits and CRs (p< 0.0001). (B) Over the right central ROI, we find a significant interaction between response type and time interval (p = 0.0206) driven by a significant pre-post interaction between fast hits and CRs (p = 0.008).

Close modal

In RC, we find a significant interaction between response type and time interval. This interaction was driven by a significant interaction between fast hits and CRs (F1,37 = 7.885, p= 0.008, ηp2= 0.18), whereby theta power was numerically greater for fast hits (M = 0.15, SD = 0.26) relative to CRs (M = 0.10, SD = 0.19) in the pre-response time interval (t37 = 1.274, p= 0.2107, d = 0.2278) and numerically greater for CRs (M = 0.08, SD = 0.29) relative to fast hits (M = 0.04, SD = 0.27) in the post-response time interval (t37 = 0.9688, p= 0.3389, d = 0.1549). The interaction between slow hits and CRs was not significant (F1,37 = 0.306, p= 0.583, ηp2= 0.008) nor was the interaction between fast hits and slow hits (F1,37 = 4.038, p= 0.0518, ηp2= 0.010). Together, these results suggest that post-response theta power dissociations may reflect a positive feedback signal in response to strongly remembered experiences.

The goal of the current study was to test the hypothesis that distinct processes occur prior to and following a memory response. We recorded scalp EEG while participants performed a recognition memory task and received no explicit feedback on their performance. Crucially, we focused our analyses on response-locked test-phase data to separate the processes occurring before and after a memory response. We show that reaction times (RTs) are faster for hits compared to correct rejections (CRs) and that a greater proportion of hits occur within 500–750 ms of stimulus onset compared to CRs. We replicate established findings (Nyhus & Curran, 2010) that preceding a memory response, theta power (4–8 Hz) in frontocentral electrodes is greater for hits compared to CRs. We show that these pre-response theta power dissociations “flip” in left central electrodes following the memory response. We find that this post-response “flip” is specific to hits committed quickly, potentially reflecting a positive feedback signal for strongly remembered experiences. Together, these findings suggest that there are potentially distinct memory and decision-making processes engaged preceding and following a memory response that are modulated by successful retrieval.

We find faster RTs for hits compared to CRs, replicating past findings (Uncapher et al., 2015; Weidemann & Kahana, 2016). Faster RTs for hits may be driven by greater memory strength (Verde & Rotello, 2007; Wixted, 2007) and/or greater contextual reinstatement (Gordon et al., 2014; Hanczakowski et al., 2015). However, these RT differences indicate that traditional hit versus CR comparisons of stimulus-locked data may capture both pre- and post-response related processes. We specifically find that a larger proportion of hits occur within 500–750 ms of stimulus onset compared to CRs meaning that if participants evaluate or update a representation after responding, and they respond at different times for hits relative to CRs, estimates of neural signals within this time window may reveal differences in post-hit evaluation processes following successful retrieval versus memory/retrieval-related processes related to correctly rejecting lures. Thus, our results highlight the importance of utilizing response-locked data to investigate the distinct processes leading up to and following a memory response.

Consistent with prior EEG work (Burgess & Gruzelier, 1997; Düzel et al., 2003; Klimesch et al., 1997, 2000; Nyhus & Curran, 2010), we find greater pre-response theta power for hits than CRs. EEG power in the theta frequency band has been proposed to coordinate cortical areas, supporting the ability to encode and retrieve contextual information about time and space that is central to episodic memory (Hasselmo & Stern, 2014). Successful memory retrieval depends on the access of an internal representation of a past experience. This access may take the form of reinstatement, wherein encoded content is reconstructed during retrieval (Danker & Anderson, 2010), which may specifically be supported by coordination between cortical areas (Herweg et al., 2020). Our findings are generally consistent with the drift diffusion model (Ratcliff & McKoon, 2008; Ratcliff et al., 2016) whereby the pre-response theta power dissociations may reflect evidence accumulation. Insofar as pre-response theta power reflects reinstatement of a past experience (Guan et al., 2023; Kota et al., 2020), our finding of generally lower pre-response theta power for CRs is consistent with the evidence accumulation account as there is no experience to reinstate during a CR. Although we did not fit the drift diffusion model to the neural data, we would predict that greater theta power preceding a response would be associated with a higher or faster drift rate, the model parameter that reflects the strength of the rate of evidence accumulation (Ratcliff et al., 2016). This interpretation is consistent with our finding of robust pre-response theta power dissociations specifically between slow hits and fast hits. Fast RTs are thought to reflect rapid evidence accumulation (Mulder & van Maanen, 2013; Rollwage et al., 2020; Shenhav et al., 2018), thus greater pre-response theta power for fast hits compared to slow hits may reflect faster evidence accumulation for strongly remembered experiences.

Following a memory response, we find a “flip” in theta power over the left central ROI, such that theta power is greater for CRs than hits. Participants did not receive feedback indicating that they were correct suggesting that the dissociation in theta power may occur in response to intrinsic feedback signals specifically following successful retrieval. Although prior work has demonstrated that post-retrieval monitoring processes are engaged following memory retrieval (Hill et al., 2021; Rugg et al., 2003), the majority of these findings reflect signals that precede a behavioral response. Our interpretation is that the post-response theta power dissociation between hits and CRs may reflect a feedback signal. This interpretation is consistent with work from the cognitive control literature showing that theta power increases for incorrect compared to correct responses and following negative relative to positive outcomes (Cavanagh & Frank, 2014; Cavanagh et al., 2010). Prior work has proposed that frontal midline theta (FMT) is associated with reward processing—specifically that the FMT is larger following negative feedback or monetary loss (Cohen et al., 2007; Marco-Pallarés et al., 2008)—and is a mechanism for communication between brain regions (Glazer et al., 2018). As both hits and CRs constitute correct responses, we may have anticipated decreased post-response theta power for both response types. However, only hits reflect successful retrieval. Thus, the post-response decrease in theta power for hits may reflect positive feedback specifically in response to successful retrieval.

We find that the pre- versus post-response dissociation in theta power is specific to fast hits. To the extent that fast hits reflect highly confident responses (Ratcliff, 1978; Ratcliff & Starns, 2009; Weidemann & Kahana, 2016), the post-response theta power decrease for fast hits may reflect a positive feedback signal. Neuro-imaging work has repeatedly shown that reward-related regions (e.g., striatum) are more active during hits compared to CRs in the absence of explicit reward (Achim & Lepage, 2005; Clos et al., 2015; de Zubicaray et al., 2005; Fliessbach et al., 2006; Henson, et al., 2005; Schwarze et al., 2013; Spaniol et al., 2009). As both responses are correct, the dissociation in striatal activity for hits compared to CRs suggests that the signal change is not driven by overall accuracy, but rather is a response to successful retrieval and indicates that successful retrieval may be intrinsically rewarding (Satterthwaite et al., 2012; Speer et al., 2014). Taken together, the post-response theta dissociation that we observe in the current study may represent a positive feedback signal in response to successful retrieval, though a direct test of this account is necessary to support this claim. The direct investigation of post-response test-phase feedback signals presents an exciting avenue for future work.

Our broad interpretation of the present findings is that the post-response theta power decrease reflects the last process in a cascade of processes that support “remembering,” generally construed. An individual begins remembering by perceptually processing an externally presented stimulus and then engages in a memory search, internal attention, evidence accumulation, and/or matching process in an attempt to access a stored representation (Kahana, 2012; Polyn & Kahana, 2007; Ratcliff et al., 2016; Smith & Long, 2024)—these two processes need not be serial and instead an individual may iterate between them. If these processes are successful, an item—and possibly its context or other associated information—may be reinstated, as in Tulving’s original proposal of ecphory (Tulving, 1983). Reinstatement is followed by a monitoring process in which the accessed representation is evaluated (Cruse & Wilding, 2009). A decision is then made in tandem with commission of a behavioral response. Finally, a decision- making post-response evaluative or representation updating process is engaged. Our interpretation is that the frontocentral post-response theta dissociation reflects this final decision making step. Given the limitations in estimating reinstatement of verbal, as opposed to visual, stimuli (Brunec et al., 2020), future work is needed to directly test this account and the timing and relationship between reinstatement and post-decision signals.

A critical open question is how the observed RT distributions and pre- versus post-response theta dissociations relate to the established processes of recollection and familiarity. Recollection is the retrieval of contextual details and familiarity is memory strength without detailed retrieval (Addante et al., 2012; Diana et al., 2005; Gimbel & Brewer, 2011; Yonelinas, 2001a). There is mixed evidence as regards RTs for recollection and familiarity, with some evidence that recollection responses are faster than familiarity responses (Diana et al., 2005; Gimbel & Brewer, 2011; Herweg et al., 2016) and some evidence that recollection responses are slower than familiarity responses (Atkinson & Juola, 1974; Besson et al., 2012; Jacoby, 1991). Due to our task design, we cannot disambiguate recollection from familiarity based responses, but we can leverage RT as a coarse assay of confidence. Our assumption is that compared to slow hits, fast hits reflect higher confident responses (Ratcliff, 1978; Ratcliff & Starns, 2009; Weidemann & Kahana, 2016). High confident responses may be supported by the recollection of contextual details (Yonelinas, 2001a, 2001b), in which case elevated pre-response theta power may reflect the reinstatement of contextual details during fast hits. Likewise, the decreased post-response theta power following fast hits may reflect a positive feedback signal in response specifically to recollected experiences. Future work will be needed to directly test this possibility.

Although we did not anticipate hemispheric differences, we consistently found influences of ROI on theta power. Overall, the effects that we observe are numerically stronger in the left central, relative to right central, ROI, although a direct test of the post-response theta dissociation for left versus right ROIs indicated that the effect is not specific to the left hemisphere. It is unlikely that motor responses contributed to the observed hemispheric effects given that responses were counterbalanced across participants and typically motor movements engage higher frequency bands (e.g., Crone et al., 1998). The hemispheric asymmetry may be driven by the stimuli used and/or intrinsic hemispheric connections (D. Wang et al., 2014). We used visually presented words in the current study and verbal stimuli are well known to recruit the left hemisphere (de Zubicaray et al., 2011; Ries et al., 2016; Price, 2012; Vigneau et al., 2011), including during memory tasks (Kelley et al., 1998; Kim, 2011). Investigation of resting state data has shown that cortical networks have intrinsic within-hemisphere connections which may enable control over the specific functions or processes that are engaged (D. Wang et al., 2014). Future work will be needed to probe both the hemispheric asymmetry of this effect as well as the overall spatial specificity, given that we chose to focus exclusively on frontocentral regions due to the convergence of memory and decision-making literature on this area. Although a region-frequency focused investigation is ideal for hypothesis testing (Cohen, 2014a), it does mean that other important neural substrates may be engaged following successful retrieval that we were not positioned to identify. Indeed, other areas are known to also support memory retrieval (e.g., right frontal, Evans et al., 2015; left parietal, Jacobs et al., 2006) which may also show post-response dissociations. Our findings generally show a frontocentral theta pattern consistent with pre-response memory-related processing and post-response decision-making-related processing.

Together, our findings suggest that distinct processes occur prior to and following a memory response and, in particular, that decision-making processes may follow successful retrieval. A direction for future research will be to directly investigate the extent to which the post-response theta dissociation reflects a feedback signal. More broadly, we contribute to a growing body of literature characterizing the role of theta activity in successful memory retrieval.

The raw, de-identified data and the associated experimental and analysis codes used in this study can be accessed via the Long Term Memory Lab Website (https://longtermmemorylab.com).

Devyn E. Smith: Conceptualization; Data curation; Formal analysis; Software; Supervision; Visualization; Writing—Original draft; Writing—Review and editing. Justin R. Wheelock: Formal analysis; Software; Visualization; Writing—Original draft; Writing—Review and editing. Nicole M. Long: Conceptualization; Formal analysis; Funding acquisition; Project administration; Software; Supervision; Visualization; Writing—Original draft; Writing—Review and editing.

This work was supported by a grant from the National Institutes of Health (NINDS R01 NS132872, PI: NML).

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

We thank Yuju Hong for assistance with data collection.

Achim
,
A. M.
, &
Lepage
,
M.
(
2005
).
Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex involvement in memory post-retrieval monitoring revealed in both item and associative recognition tests
.
NeuroImage
,
24
,
1113
1121
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.10.036
Addante
,
R. J.
,
Ranganath
,
C.
, &
Yonelinas
,
A. P.
(
2012
).
Examining ERP correlates of recognition memory: Evidence of accurate source recognition without recollection
.
NeuroImage
,
61
(
1
),
439
450
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.04.031
Atkinson
,
R. C.
, &
Juola
,
J. F.
(
1974
).
Search and decision processes in recognition memory
. In
D. H.
Krantz
,
R. C.
Atkinson
,
R. D.
Luce
, &
S.
Patrick
(Eds.),
Contemporary developments in mathematical psychology: I. Learning, memory and thinking
(pp.
243
293
).
W.H. Freeman
.
Benjamini
,
Y.
, &
Hochberg
,
Y.
(
1995
).
Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing
.
Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Methodological)
,
57
(
1
),
289
300
. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x
Besson
,
G.
,
Ceccaldi
,
M.
,
Didic
,
M.
, &
Barbeau
,
E. J.
(
2012
).
The speed of visual recognition memory
.
Visual Cognition
,
20
(
10
),
1131
1152
. https://doi.org/10.1080/13506285.2012.724034
Brunec
,
I. K.
,
Robin
,
J.
,
Olsen
,
R. K.
,
Moscovitch
,
M.
, &
Barense
,
M. D.
(
2020
).
Integration and differentiation of hippocampal memory traces
.
Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
,
118
,
196
208
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.07.024
Burgess
,
A. P.
, &
Gruzelier
,
J. H.
(
1997
).
Short duration synchronization of human theta rhythm during recognition memory
.
NeuroReport
,
8
(
4
),
1039
1042
. https://doi.org/10.1097/00001756-199703030-00044
Castellanos
,
N. P.
, &
Makarov
,
V. A.
(
2006
).
Recovering EEG brain signals: Artifact suppression with wavelet enhanced independent component analysis
.
Journal of Neuroscience Methods
,
158
(
2
),
300
312
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2006.05.033
Cavanagh
,
J. F.
, &
Frank
,
M. J.
(
2014
).
Frontal theta as a mechanism for cognitive control
.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences
,
18
(
8
),
414
421
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.04.012
Cavanagh
,
J. F.
,
Frank
,
M. J.
,
Klein
,
T. J.
, &
Allen
,
J. J.
(
2010
).
Frontal theta links prediction errors to behavioral adaptation in reinforcement learning
.
NeuroImage
,
49
(
4
),
3198
3209
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.11.080
Cavanagh
,
J. F.
,
Zambrano-Vazquez
,
L.
, &
Allen
,
J. J.
(
2012
).
Theta lingua franca: A common mid-frontal substrate for action monitoring processes
.
Psychophysiology
,
49
(
2
),
220
238
. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01293.x
Clos
,
M.
,
Schwarze
,
U.
,
Gluth
,
S.
,
Bunzeck
,
N.
, &
Sommer
,
T.
(
2015
).
Goal- and retrieval-dependent activity in the striatum during memory recognition
.
Neuropsychologia
,
72
,
1
11
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.04.011
Cohen
,
M. X.
(
2014a
).
Analyzing neural time series data: Theory and practice
.
MIT Press
. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9609.001.0001
Cohen
,
M. X.
(
2014b
).
A neural microcircuit for cognitive conflict detection and signaling
.
Trends in Neurosciences
,
37
(
9
),
480
490
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2014.06.004
Cohen
,
M. X.
,
Elger
,
C. E.
, &
Ranganath
,
C.
(
2007
).
Reward expectation modulates feedback-related negativity and EEG spectra
.
NeuroImage
,
35
,
968
978
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.11.056
Crone
,
N. E.
,
Miglioretti
,
D. L.
,
Gordon
,
B.
,
Sieracki
,
J. M.
,
Wilson
,
M. T.
,
Uematsu
,
S.
, &
Lesser
,
R. P.
(
1998
).
Functional mapping of human sensorimotor cortex with electrocorticographic spectral analysis. I. Alpha and beta event-related desynchronization
.
Brain
,
121
(
12
),
2271
2299
. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/121.12.2271
Cruse
,
D.
, &
Wilding
,
E. L.
(
2009
).
Prefrontal cortex contributions to episodic retrieval monitoring and evaluation
.
Neuropsychologia
,
47
,
2779
2789
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.06.003
Cruse
,
D.
, &
Wilding
,
E. L.
(
2011
).
Temporally and functionally dissociable retrieval processing operations revealed by event-related potentials
.
Neuropsychologia
,
49
,
1751
1760
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.02.053
Curran
,
T.
(
2000
).
Brain potentials of recollection and familiarity
.
Memory & Cognition
,
28
(
6
),
923
938
. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03209340
Curran
,
T.
(
2004
).
Effects of attention and confidence on the hypothesized ERP correlates of recollection and familiarity
.
Neuropsychologia
,
42
,
1088
1106
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.12.011
Curran
,
T.
, &
Cleary
,
A. M.
(
2003
).
Using ERPs to dissociate recollection from familiarity in picture recognition
.
Cognitive Brain Research
,
15
,
191
205
. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0926-6410(02)00192-1
Curran
,
T.
,
DeBuse
,
C.
, &
Leynes
,
P. A.
(
2007
).
Conflict and criterion setting in recognition memory
.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
,
33
(
1
),
2
17
. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.33.1.2
Curran
,
T.
, &
Hancock
,
J.
(
2007
).
The FN400 indexes familiarity-based recognition of faces
.
NeuroImage
,
36
(
2
),
464
471
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.12.016
Danker
,
J. F.
, &
Anderson
,
J. R.
(
2010
).
The ghosts of brain states past: Remembering reactivates the brain regions engaged during encoding
.
Psychological Bulletin
,
136
(
1
),
87
102
. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017937
de Zubicaray
,
G. I.
,
McMahon
,
K. L.
,
Eastburn
,
M. M.
,
Finnigan
,
S.
, &
Humphreys
,
M. S.
(
2005
).
fMRI evidence of word frequency and strength effects in recognition memory
.
Cognitive Brain Research
,
24
,
587
598
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.03.009
de Zubicaray
,
G. I.
,
Miozzo
,
M.
,
Johnson
,
K.
,
Schiller
,
N. O.
, &
McMahon
,
K. L.
(
2011
).
Independent distractor frequency and age-of-acquisition effects in picture–word interference: fMRI evidence for post-lexical and lexical accounts according to distractor type
.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
,
24
(
2
),
482
495
. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00141
Diana
,
R. A.
,
Vilberg
,
K. L.
, &
Reder
,
L. M.
(
2005
).
Identifying the ERP correlate of a recognition memory search attempt
.
Cognitive Brain Research
,
24
(
3
),
674
684
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.04.001
Düzel
,
E.
,
Habib
,
R.
,
Schott
,
B.
,
Schoenfeld
,
A.
,
Lobaugh
,
N.
,
McIntosh
,
A.
,
Scholz
,
M.
, &
Heinze
,
H.
(
2003
).
A multivariate, spatiotemporal analysis of electromagnetic time-frequency data of recognition memory
.
NeuroImage
,
18
(
2
),
185
197
. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1053-8119(02)00031-9
Evans
,
L. H.
,
Williams
,
A. N.
, &
Wilding
,
E. L.
(
2015
).
Electrophysiological evidence for retrieval mode immediately after a task switch
.
NeuroImage
,
108
,
435
440
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.12.068
Fliessbach
,
K.
,
Weis
,
S.
,
Klaver
,
P.
,
Elger
,
C. E.
, &
Weber
,
B.
(
2006
).
The effect of word concreteness on recognition memory
.
NeuroImage
,
32
,
1413
1421
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.06.007
Frank
,
M. J.
,
Gagne
,
C.
,
Nyhus
,
E.
,
Masters
,
S.
,
Wiecki
,
T. V.
,
Cavanagh
,
J. F.
, &
Badre
,
D.
(
2015
).
fMRI and EEG predictors of dynamic decision parameters during human reinforcement learning
.
The Journal of Neuroscience
,
35
(
2
),
485
494
. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.2036-14.2015
Frank
,
M. J.
,
Woroch
,
B. S.
, &
Curran
,
T.
(
2005
).
Error-related negativity predicts reinforcement learning and conflict biases
.
Neuron
,
47
,
495
501
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2005.06.020
Friedman
,
D.
, &
Johnson Jr
.,
R.
(
2000
).
Event-related potential (ERP) studies of memory encoding and retrieval: A selective review
.
Microscopy Research and Technique
,
51
,
6
28
. https://doi.org/10.1002/1097-0029(20001001)51:1<6::aid-jemt2>3.3.co;2-i
Friendly
,
M.
,
Franklin
,
P. E.
,
Hoffman
,
D.
, &
Rubin
,
D. C.
(
1982
).
The toronto word pool: Norms for imagery, concreteness, orthographic variables, and grammatical usage for 1,080 words
.
Behavior Research Methods & Instrumentation
,
14
,
375
399
. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03203275
Gehring
,
W. J.
,
Goss
,
B.
,
Coles
,
M. G.
,
Meyer
,
D. E.
, &
Donchin
,
E.
(
1993
).
A neural system for error detection and compensation
.
Psychological Science
,
4
(
6
),
385
390
. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1993.tb00586.x
Gimbel
,
S. I.
, &
Brewer
,
J. B.
(
2011
).
Reaction time, memory strength, and fMRI activity during memory retrieval: Hippocampus and default network are differentially responsive during recollection and familiarity judgments
.
Cognitive Neuroscience
,
2
(
1
),
19
23
. https://doi.org/10.1080/17588928.2010.513770
Glazer
,
J. E.
,
Kelley
,
N. J.
,
Pornpattananangkul
,
N.
,
Mittal
,
V. A.
, &
Nusslock
,
R.
(
2018
).
Beyond the FRN: Broadening the time-course of EEG and ERP components implicated in reward processing
.
International Journal of Psychophysiology
,
132
,
184
202
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2018.02.002
Gordon
,
A. M.
,
Rissman
,
J.
,
Kiani
,
R.
, &
Wagner
,
A. D.
(
2014
).
Cortical reinstatement mediates the relationship between content-specific encoding activity and subsequent recollection decisions
.
Cerebral Cortex
,
24
,
3350
3364
. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bht194
Gruber
,
T.
,
Tsivilis
,
D.
,
Giabbiconi
,
C.-M.
, &
Müller
,
M. M.
(
2008
).
Induced electroencephalogram oscillations during source memory: Familiarity is reflected in the gamma band, recollection in the theta band
.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
,
20
(
6
),
1043
1053
. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2008.20068
Guan
,
Q.
,
Ma
,
L.
,
Chen
,
Y.
,
Luo
,
Y.
, &
He
,
H.
(
2023
).
Midfrontal theta phase underlies evidence accumulation and response thresholding in cognitive control
.
Cerebral Cortex
,
33
,
8967
8979
. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhad175
Hanczakowski
,
M.
,
Zawadzka
,
K.
, &
Macken
,
B.
(
2015
).
Continued effects of context reinstatement in recognition
.
Memory & Cognition
,
43
(
5
),
788
797
. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-014-0502-2
Hasselmo
,
M. E.
, &
Stern
,
C. E.
(
2014
).
Theta rhythm and the encoding and retrieval of space and time
.
NeuroImage
,
85
,
656
666
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.022
Hayama
,
H. R.
,
Johnson
,
J. D.
, &
Rugg
,
M. D.
(
2008
).
The relationship between the right frontal old/new ERP effect and post-retrieval monitoring: Specific or non-specific
?
Neuropsychologia
,
46
,
1211
1223
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.11.021
Henson
,
R. N.
,
Hornberger
,
M.
, &
Rugg
,
M. D.
(
2005
).
Further dissociating the processes involved in recognition memory: An fMRI study
.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
,
17
(
7
),
1058
1073
. https://doi.org/10.1162/0898929054475208
Herweg
,
N. A.
,
Apitz
,
T.
,
Leicht
,
G.
,
Mulert
,
C.
,
Fuentemilla
,
L.
, &
Bunzeck
,
N.
(
2016
).
Theta-alpha oscillations bind the hippocampus, prefrontal cortex, and striatum during recollection: Evidence from simultaneous EEG–fMRI
.
The Journal of Neuroscience
,
36
(
12
),
3579
3587
. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.3629-15.2016
Herweg
,
N. A.
,
Solomon
,
E. A.
, &
Kahana
,
M. J.
(
2020
).
Theta oscillations in human memory
.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences
,
24
(
3
),
208
227
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2019.12.006
Hill
,
P. F.
,
Horne
,
E. D.
,
Koen
,
J. D.
, &
Rugg
,
M. D.
(
2021
).
Transcranial magnetic stimulation of right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex does not affect associative retrieval in healthy young or older adults
.
Neuroimage: Reports
,
1
,
1
9
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ynirp.2021.100027
Jacobs
,
J.
,
Hwang
,
G.
,
Curran
,
T.
, &
Kahana
,
M. J.
(
2006
).
EEG oscillations and recognition memory: Theta correlates of memory retrieval and decision making
.
NeuroImage
,
32
(
2
),
978
987
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.02.018
Jacoby
,
L. L.
(
1991
).
A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory
.
Journal of Memory and Language
,
30
,
513
541
. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596x(91)90025-f
Johansson
,
M.
, &
Mecklinger
,
A.
(
2003
).
The late posterior negativity in ERP studies of episodic memory: Action monitoring and retrieval of attribute conjunctions
.
Biological Psychology
,
64
,
91
117
. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0301-0511(03)00104-2
Kahana
,
M. J.
(
2012
).
Foundations in human memory
.
Oxford University
Press
.
Kelley
,
W. M.
,
Miezin
,
F. M.
,
McDermott
,
K. B.
,
Buckner
,
R. L.
,
Raichle
,
M. E.
,
Cohen
,
N. J.
,
Ollinger
,
J. M.
,
Akbudak
,
E.
,
Conturo
,
T. E.
,
Snyder
,
A. Z.
, &
Petersen
,
S. E.
(
1998
).
Hemispheric specialization in human dorsal frontal cortex and medial temporal lobe for verbal and nonverbal memory encoding
.
Neuron
,
20
(
5
),
927
936
. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80474-2
Kim
,
H.
(
2011
).
Neural activity that predicts subsequent memory and forgetting: A meta-analysis of 74 fMRI studies
.
NeuroImage
,
54
(
3
),
2446
2461
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.045
Klimesch
,
W.
,
Doppelmayr
,
M.
,
Schimke
,
H.
, &
Ripper
,
B.
(
1997
).
Theta synchronization and alpha desynchronization in a memory task
.
Psychophysiology
,
34
,
169
176
. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1997.tb02128.x
Klimesch
,
W.
,
Doppelmayr
,
M.
,
Schwaiger
,
J.
,
Winkler
,
T.
, &
Gruber
,
W.
(
2000
).
Theta oscillations and the ERP old/new effect: Independent phenomena
?
Clinical Neurophysiology
,
111
(
5
),
781
793
. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1388-2457(00)00254-6
Kota
,
S.
,
Rugg
,
M. D.
, &
Lega
,
B. C.
(
2020
).
Hippocampal theta oscillations support successful associative memory formation
.
The Journal of Neuroscience
,
40
(
49
),
9507
9518
. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.0767-20.2020
Long
,
N. M.
, &
Kahana
,
M. J.
(
2015
).
Successful memory formation is driven by contextual encoding in the core memory network
.
NeuroImage
,
119
,
332
337
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.06.073
Long
,
N. M.
, &
Kahana
,
M. J.
(
2017
).
Modulation of task demands suggests that semantic processing interferes with the formation of episodic associations
.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
,
43
(
2
),
167
176
. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000300
Long
,
N. M.
,
Sperling
,
M. R.
,
Worrell
,
G. A.
,
Davis
,
K. A.
,
Gross
,
R. E.
,
Lega
,
B. C.
,
Jobst
,
B. C.
,
Sheth
,
S. A.
,
Zaghloul
,
K.
,
Stein
,
J. M.
, &
Kahana
,
M. J.
(
2017
).
Contextually mediated spontaneous retrieval is specific to the hippocampus
.
Current Biology
,
27
,
1
6
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2017.02.054
Luft
,
C. D. B.
(
2014
).
Learning from feedback: The neural mechanisms of feedback processing facilitating better performance
.
Behavioural Brain Research
,
261
,
356
368
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2013.12.043
Luu
,
P.
,
Tucker
,
D. M.
, &
Makeig
,
S.
(
2004
).
Frontal midline theta and the error-related negativity: Neurophysiological mechanisms of action regulation
.
Clinical Neurophysiology
,
115
,
1821
1835
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2004.03.031
Marco-Pallarés
,
J.
,
Cucurell
,
D.
,
Cunillera
,
T.
,
García
,
R.
,
Andrés-Pueyo
,
A.
,
Münte
,
T. F.
, &
Rodríguez-Fornells
,
A.
(
2008
).
Human oscillatory activity associated to reward processing in a gambling task
.
Neuropsychologia
,
46
(
1
),
241
248
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.07.016
Mas-Herrero
,
E.
,
Ripollés
,
P.
,
HajiHosseini
,
A.
,
Rodríguez-Fornells
,
A.
, &
Marco-Pallarés
,
J.
(
2015
).
Beta oscillations and reward processing: Coupling oscillatory activity and hemodynamic responses
.
NeuroImage
,
119
,
13
19
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.05.095
Mazaheri
,
A.
,
Nieuwenhuis
,
I. L.
,
Dijk
van
, H., &
Jensen
,
O.
(
2009
).
Prestimulus alpha and mu activity predicts failure to inhibit motor responses
.
Human Brain Mapping
,
30
,
1791
1800
. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20763
Mecklinger
,
A.
(
2000
).
Interfacing mind and brain: A neurocognitive model of recognition memory
.
Psychophysiology
,
37
,
565
582
. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-8986.3750565
Moore
,
I. L.
, &
Long
,
N. M.
(
2024
).
Semantic associations restore neural encoding mechanisms
.
Learning & Memory
,
31
(
3
),
1
13
. https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.053996.124
Mulder
,
M. J.
, &
Maanen
van
, L
. (
2013
).
Are accuracy and reaction time affected via different processes
?
PLoS One
,
8
(
11
),
1
8
. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0080222
Nelson
,
D. L.
,
Zhang
,
N.
, &
McKinney
,
V. M.
(
2001
).
The ties that bind what is known to the recognition of what is new
.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
,
27
(
5
),
1147
1159
. https://doi.org/10.1037//0278-7393.27.5.1147
Nolan
,
H.
,
Whelan
,
R.
, &
Reilly
,
R.
(
2010
).
Faster: Fully automated statistical thresholding for EEG artifact rejection
.
Journal of Neuroscience Methods
,
192
(
1
),
152
162
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2010.07.015
Nyhus
,
E.
, &
Curran
,
T.
(
2010
).
Functional role of gamma and theta oscillations in episodic memory
.
Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
,
34
,
1023
1035
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.12.014
Ostlund
,
B.
,
Donoghue
,
T.
,
Anaya
,
B.
,
Gunther
,
K. E.
,
Karalunas
,
S. L.
,
Voytek
,
B.
, &
Pérez-Edgar
,
K. E.
(
2022
).
Spectral parameterization for studying neurodevelopment: How and why
.
Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience
,
54
(
101073
),
1
14
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2022.101073
Pinner
,
J. F.
, &
Cavanagh
,
J. F.
(
2017
).
Frontal theta accounts for individual differences in the cost of conflict on decision making
.
Brain Research
,
1672
,
73
80
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2017.07.026
Polyn
,
S. M.
, &
Kahana
,
M. J.
(
2007
).
Memory search and the neural representation of context
.
TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences
,
12
(
1
),
24
30
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.10.010
Price
,
C. J.
(
2012
).
A review and synthesis of the first 20 years of PET and fMRI studies of heard speech, spoken language and reading
.
NeuroImage
,
62
,
816
847
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.04.062
Ratcliff
,
R.
(
1978
).
A theory of memory retrieval
.
Psychological Review
,
85
(
2
),
59
108
. https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-295x.85.2.59
Ratcliff
,
R.
, &
McKoon
,
G.
(
2008
).
The diffusion decision model: Theory and data for two-choice decision tasks
.
Neural Computation
,
20
(
4
),
873
922
. https://doi.org/10.1162/neco.2008.12-06-420
Ratcliff
,
R.
,
Smith
,
P. L.
,
Brown
,
S. D.
, &
McKoon
,
G.
(
2016
).
Diffusion decision model: Current issues and history
.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences
,
20
(
4
),
260
281
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.01.007
Ratcliff
,
R.
, &
Starns
,
J. J.
(
2009
).
Modeling confidence and response time in recognition memory
.
Psychological Review
,
116
(
1
),
59
83
. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014086
Ries
,
S. K.
,
Dronkers
,
N. F.
, &
Knight
,
R. T.
(
2016
).
Choosing words: Left hemisphere, right hemisphere, or both? Perspective on the lateralization of word retrieval
.
Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences
,
1369
(
1
),
111
131
. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.12993
Rollwage
,
M.
,
Loosen
,
A.
,
Hauser
,
T. U.
,
Moran
,
R.
,
Dolan
,
R. J.
, &
Fleming
,
S. M.
(
2020
).
Confidence drives a neural confirmation bias
.
Nature Communications
,
11
(
2634
),
1
11
. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16278-6
Rugg
,
M. D.
,
Henson
,
R. N.
, &
Robb
,
W. G.
(
2003
).
Neural correlates of retrieval processing in the prefrontal cortex during recognition and exclusion tasks
.
Neuropsychologia
,
41
,
40
52
. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00129-x
Rugg
,
M. D.
,
Ruth
,
M. E.
,
Walla
,
P.
,
Schloerscheidt
,
A. M.
,
Birch
,
C. S.
, &
Allan
,
K.
(
1998
).
Dissociation of the neural correlates of implicit and explicit memory
.
Nature
,
392
,
595
598
. https://doi.org/10.1038/33396
Rugg
,
M. D.
, &
Wilding
,
E. L.
(
2000
).
Retrieval processing and episodic memory
.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences
,
4
(
3
),
108
115
. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01445-5
Satterthwaite
,
T. D.
,
Ruparel
,
K.
,
Elliott
,
M. A.
,
Gerraty
,
R. T.
,
Calkins
,
M. E.
,
Hakonarson
,
H.
,
Gur
,
R. C.
,
Gur
,
R. E.
, &
Wolf
,
D. H.
(
2012
).
Being right is its own reward: Load and performance related ventral striatum activation to correct responses during a working memory task in youth
.
NeuroImage
,
61
,
723
729
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.060
Schwarze
,
U.
,
Bingel
,
U.
,
Badre
,
D.
, &
Sommer
,
T.
(
2013
).
Ventral striatal activity correlates with memory confidence for old- and new-responses in a difficult recognition test
.
PLoS One
,
8
(
3
),
1
7
. https://doi.org/10.1371/annotation/bd94cd03-bc0e-4535-a628-1d3e6a9d6a5e
Senftleben
,
U.
, &
Scherbaum
,
S.
(
2021
).
Mid-frontal theta during conflict in a value-based decision task
.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
,
33
(
10
),
2109
2131
. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01741
Shenhav
,
A.
,
Straccia
,
M. A.
,
Musslick
,
S.
,
Cohen
,
J. D.
, &
Botvinick
,
M. M.
(
2018
).
Dissociable neural mechanisms track evidence accumulation for selection of attention versus action
.
Nature Communications
,
9
(
2485
),
1
10
. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-018-04841-1
Smith
,
D. E.
, &
Long
,
N. M.
(
2024
).
Top-down task goals induce the retrieval state
.
Journal of Neuroscience
. https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.03.04.583353
Smith
,
D. E.
,
Moore
,
I. L.
, &
Long
,
N. M.
(
2022
).
Temporal context modulates encoding and retrieval of overlapping events
.
The Journal of Neuroscience
,
42
(
14
),
3000
3010
. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.1091-21.2022
Spaniol
,
J.
,
Davidson
,
P. S.
,
Kim
,
A. S.
,
Han
,
H.
,
Moscovitch
,
M.
, &
Grady
,
C. L.
(
2009
).
Event-related fMRI studies of episodic encoding and retrieval: Meta-analyses using activation likelihood estimation
.
Neuropsychologia
,
47
,
1765
1779
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.02.028
Speer
,
M. E.
,
Bhanji
,
J. P.
, &
Delgado
,
M. R.
(
2014
).
Savoring the past: Positive memories evoke value representations in the striatum
.
Neuron
,
84
,
1
10
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2014.09.028
Trujillo
,
L. T.
, &
Allen
,
J. J.
(
2007
).
Theta EEG dynamics of the error-related negativity
.
Clinical Neurophysiology
,
118
,
645
668
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2006.11.009
Tulving
,
E.
(
1983
).
Elements of episodic memory
.
Oxford University
Press
.
Uncapher
,
M. R.
,
Boyd-Meredith
,
J. T.
,
Chow
,
T. E.
,
Rissman
,
J.
, &
Wagner
,
A. D.
(
2015
).
Goal-directed modulation of neural memory patterns: Implications for fMRI-based memory detection
.
The Journal of Neuroscience
,
35
(
22
),
8531
8545
. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.5145-14.2015
Verde
,
M. F.
, &
Rotello
,
C. M.
(
2007
).
Memory strength and the decision process in recognition memory
.
Memory & Cognition
,
35
(
2
),
254
262
. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03193446
Vigneau
,
M.
,
Beaucousin
,
V.
,
Hervé
,
P.-Y.
,
Jobard
,
G.
,
Petit
,
L.
,
Crivello
,
F.
,
Mellet
,
E.
,
Zago
,
L.
,
Mazoyer
,
B.
, &
Tzourio-Mazoyer
,
N.
(
2011
).
What is right-hemisphere contribution to phonological, lexico-semantic, and sentence processing? Insights from a meta-analysis
.
NeuroImage
,
54
,
577
593
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.036
Voss
,
J.
, &
Paller
,
K.
(
2008
).
Neural substrates of remembering: Electroencephalographic studies: Learning and memory: A comprehensive reference
. In
J. H.
Byrne
(Ed.),
Memory systems.
Elsevier
.
Wang
,
D.
,
Buckner
,
R. L.
, &
Liu
,
H.
(
2014
).
Functional specialization in the human brain estimated by intrinsic hemispheric interaction
.
The Journal of Neuroscience
,
34
(
37
),
12341
12352
. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.0787-14.2014
Wang
,
L.
,
Gu
,
Y.
,
Zhao
,
G.
, &
Chen
,
A.
(
2020
).
Error-related negativity and error awareness in a go/no-go task
.
Scientific Reports
,
10
(
4026
),
1
12
. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-60693-0
Weidemann
,
C. T.
, &
Kahana
,
M. J.
(
2016
).
Assessing recognition memory using confidence ratings and response times
.
Royal Society Open Science
,
3
(
150670
),
1
17
. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.150670
Wilding
,
E. L.
, &
Rugg
,
M. D.
(
1996
).
An event-related potential study of recognition memory with and without retrieval of source
.
Brain
,
119
,
889
905
. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/119.3.889
Wixted
,
J. T.
(
2007
).
Dual-process theory and signal-detection theory of recognition memory
.
Psychological Review
,
114
(
1
),
152
176
. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.114.1.152
Woodruff
,
C. C.
,
Uncapher
,
M. R.
, &
Rugg
,
M. D.
(
2006
).
Neural correlates of differential retrieval orientation: Sustained and item-related components
.
Neuropsychologia
,
44
,
3000
3010
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.06.019
Yonelinas
,
A. P.
(
2001a
).
Components of episodic memory: The contribution of recollection and familiarity
.
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences
,
356
(
1413
),
1363
1374
. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2001.0939
Yonelinas
,
A. P.
(
2001b
).
Consciousness, control, and confidence: The 3 Cs of recognition memory
.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
,
130
(
3
),
361
379
. https://doi.org/10.1037//0096-3445.130.3.361
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International (CC BY 4.0) license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. For a full description of the license, please visit https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/legalcode.