Dopaminergic neurons in the substantia nigra and the ventral tegmental area (SN/VTA) are classically viewed as key mediators in reward processing, while noradrenergic cells in the locus coeruleus (LC) are thought to modulate (negative) saliency processing. However, this conventional distinction is being revised by more recent research in animals. To explore the respective contributions of both the LC and SN/VTA in reward and valence processing in humans, we assessed fMRI data during stimulus encoding and response phase of a rewarded emotion-discrimination task (n = 38). Participants responded significantly faster to reward predicting and negative valence stimuli compared with their non-salient counterparts. LC activity was overall higher during trials involving reward prospect, and in particular for reward trials featuring positive valence, demonstrating an additive effect of reward and positive valence in LC. Moreover, LC activity was differentially increased for negative compared with positive valence in the response phase, indexing its role in invigorating responses to negative events. The SN/VTA showed increased activity in the response phase of reward trials (neutral valence) and negative valence trials (no reward), which aligns with coding relative saliency of these events in their respective contexts. LC modulations were accompanied by covariations in occipital cortex, suggesting noradrenergic contributions to visual prioritization of salient events. The findings underscore the sensitivity of both LC and SN/VTA to reward prospects and negative valence, challenging the dominant view of SN/VTA’s involvement in merely positive events and emphasizing their essential role in action invigoration above and beyond mere stimulus encoding. The intricate roles of the DA and NA system in reward and emotional valence processing in humans warrant further exploration and validation, given the limitations inherent to neuroimaging of deep brain structures.

Dopaminergic neurons of the substantia nigra pars compacta and the ventral tegmental area (referred to as SN/VTA) in the midbrain are classically considered key mediators of reward processing. Previous work has shown a consistent activation of the dopaminergic system in response to rewards and reward-predicting stimuli (Cohen et al., 2012; Schultz, 2002, 2013; Schultz et al., 1997; Steinberg et al., 2013). Dopaminergic signalling in mesolimbic areas also increases during processing positive valence stimuli (for reviews, see Burgdorf & Panksepp, 2006; Wise, 2004). However, the function of dopamine (DA) may not be specific to processing rewarding or other positive events as increased DA activation is also observed following novel (no reward) stimuli (Bunzeck & Düzel, 2006; Krebs et al., 2011), as well as negative, aversive events (Bromberg-Martin et al., 2010; Horvitz, 2000; Matsumoto & Hikosaka, 2009). The latter observation is particularly interesting as processing (negative) salient events has typically been ascribed to the noradrenergic system (Berridge et al., 1993; Berridge & Waterhouse, 2003).

The source region of noradrenaline (NA), the locus coeruleus (LC) extensively projects to the neocortex and subcortical structures, including the thalamus, septum, hippocampus, and basal lateral amygdala (Loughlin et al., 1986). These widespread connections allow the NA system to regulate general arousal states by modulating neuronal gain (Aston-Jones & Cohen, 2005). More recently, it has become clear that the LC pathways are particularly selective and organized in a modular fashion (for review, see Poe et al., 2020 and Sara & Bouret, 2012). For example, connectivity with the hippocampus and amygdala contributes to memory formation (McIntyre et al., 2012), whereas NA in the frontal areas is a key modulator of working memory, attention (Arnsten et al., 2012), and cognitive flexibility (Aston-Jones & Cohen, 2005; Bouret & Sara, 2005; Jahn et al., 2018).

Interestingly, previous work employing electroencephalography in humans suggested that the NA system is also sensitive to motivationally relevant stimuli (Nieuwenhuis et al., 2005). Consistent with these results, more recent animal studies have demonstrated that the NA system responds to reward-predicting cues (Bouret & Richmond, 2015; Jahn et al., 2018). For example, in an experimental task, four monkeys were presented with cues indicating whether a reward could be earned by timely releasing a bar (Bouret & Richmond, 2015). Both dopaminergic neurons in the SN and noradrenergic neurons in the LC showed transient activation at time of the cue, as well as during the release of the bar. Moreover, both SN and LC activity have been shown to increase when expecting a reward by coding value-based decision making (Varazzani et al., 2015). The magnitude of this increase in NA activity was also associated with effort, as measured by physical force produced. Varazzani and colleagues (2015) suggested that the role of NA could be complementary to that of DA, with NA regulating the effort required to obtain rewards by mobilizing resources to energize behaviour. While DA may have a stronger influence on reward-based decisions, several studies have shown that DA can also promote approach actions in anticipation of an upcoming reward (Bouret et al., 2012).

More generally, both NA and DA activity may be modulated by overall saliency (Pessoa, 2009). Salient stimuli, whether they are affective, unexpected, or rewarding, strengthen representations and capture attention. While saliency might seem an obvious common denominator to trigger DA and NA responses, a complete functional overlap would seem highly inefficient and unlikely considering that dissociations have been reported for other more complex cognitive operations in humans that go beyond ad hoc stimulus processing.

The current study focused on the neural activity within the DA and NA systems during reward and valence processing in humans using high-resolution fMRI. To this end, we assessed activity in the SN/VTA and the LC regions while participants performed a rewarded emotion-discrimination paradigm. Importantly, in this task, both reward prospect (reward, no reward) and emotional valence (positive, neutral, negative) were similarly relevant, and factorially combined from trial to trial, which allowed to assess their potential interaction on the neural level. Further, stimulus presentation and response phase were separated by design to assess evaluative processes and action invigoration, respectively, mirroring related animal work (Bouret et al., 2012; Bouret & Richmond, 2015). We expected both the DA and NA systems to be activated in reward trials, and most prominently during response implementation that will eventually lead to the rewarding outcomes (Bouret et al., 2012). In addition, we expected modulations based on stimulus valence, with increased DA activation during positive stimuli (Bradley & Lang, 2007; Park et al., 2018), and a dissociation between the DA and NA system for negative valence, with a differential increase in NA activity (Berridge, 2008; McCall et al., 2017). No a priori hypotheses were formed about whether these valence modulations would be mostly expected during the stimulus presentation or the response phase. Finally, we explored potential interactions between reward and valence processing, which could take the form of additive effects (e.g., a positive stimulus might further increase the reward response) or in terms of relative salience (e.g., a negative stimulus might be more salient in a no-reward context).

2.1 Participants

Based on our power analysis (see https://osf.io/2rsye), 40 participants were recruited through the Ghent University online recruiting website and social media websites. The inclusion criteria included (in addition to the safety screening for MRI participation) age between 18 and 35 years, no (history of) diagnosis of a neuropsychiatric disorder, (corrected) normal vision, and right-handedness. Six participants were excluded from the data analyses (behavioural and fMRI) due to low task performance (predefined as <60% correct trials), data coding issues, or motion artefacts that could not be corrected for using noise-reduction techniques. The final sample consisted of 38 participants (mean age = 23.1 years; SD = 5.2; 22 (58%) females). Participants were reimbursed 40€ (plus an additional performance-dependent reward of up to 10€) for the entire session, which lasted approximately 90 min. The experimental procedures were approved by the Ghent University Hospital Ethics Committee, and all participants gave their written informed consent before the start of the study. This study was preregistered on the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/2rsye).

2.2 Experimental paradigm

Participants performed a rewarded emotion-discrimination task in the fMRI scanner (Fig. 1A). In each trial, a unique face stimulus (referred to as stimulus event) with negative (angry), positive (happy), or neutral emotional expression was presented for 1000 ms. The gender (male/female) of the stimulus signalled reward prospect (of 7 cents per correct answer), or no-reward prospect (reward/no-reward). Participants were informed prior to the task that the gender of the stimulus would signal the prospect of a reward or no reward. The mapping between gender and reward prospect was counterbalanced across participants. Following this stimulus, two of the three response options (negative/positive/neutral) were shown in Dutch on the left and right sides of the screen (referred to as response event) for 1000 ms. These corresponded to the three valence categories, that is, angry (BOOS), neutral (NEUT), and happy (BLIJ). One word was matching the expression of the face stimulus, while one was not. Using an MR-compatible response box, participants had to select the word that matched the emotional expression of the preceding face stimulus by pressing the left or right response button corresponding to the word location on screen with the index and middle fingers of their right hand. Responses needed to be made within 1000 ms. The spatial location of the matching word was random to avoid any spatial biases for a specific valence category. Feedback was provided after each trial (referred to as feedback event) for 1000 ms, indicating whether the answer was correct (check mark), incorrect (cross), or too late (clock symbol), and whether or not a reward had been obtained: after correct responses in reward trials, the bonus (“7 ct”) was displayed underneath the check mark. In all other cases (incorrect or too late reward trials, as well as all no reward trials), the feedback display included “0 ct”. All stimuli were presented on a black background. In between the stimulus event, the response event, and feedback event, as well as between trials, a white fixation cross was shown for a variable duration ranging from 1090 to 7360 ms, resulting from a stimulus-onset-asynchrony (SOA) of 2090–8360 ms. These SOAs were distributed based on multiples of the scan repetition time (TR), with 80% at 1 TR, 10% at 2 TRs, 5% at 3 TRs, and 4 TRs, to allow for effective event-related BOLD response estimation across trials (Hinrichs et al., 2000). This experiment included stimuli from the NimStim face stimulus set (Tottenham et al., 2009) and included 72 happy, 72 neutral, and 72 angry facial expressions posed by 36 actors, who expressed all 3 emotions (and similar levels of arousal (Adolph & Alpers, 2010; Smith et al., 2013)). Participants completed three task blocks in the MR scanner (corresponding to three functional runs), which were programmed using PsychoPy version 2.3 (Python version 3.8) (Peirce, 2007). Each block consisted of 144 trials that lasted approximately 8 min. At the end of each block, the cumulative feedback about the total earned reward amount was shown. At the end of the experiment, each participant was paid his/her total amount of earned reward, in addition to the base reimbursement. Note that the project also included recognition memory data, which has not been considered here due to missing data and hence a lack of power.

Fig. 1.

(A) Exemplary trial of the rewarded emotion-discrimination task paradigm, featuring a stimulus, a response, and a feedback event. The time line indicates the duration of each event, as well as the duration of the fixation screens in between. Participants are asked to discriminate the emotional expression of the face by selecting the correct option during the response event. At the same time, the gender of the face signals whether a reward for a correct response in this trial. (B) Response times and accuracy according to the experimental factors reward (reward, no-reward) and valence (negative, neutral, positive). The black line represents the mean values of RT and accuracy in the different conditions, the violin plots the distribution of the data. BOOS: Angry; NEUT: Neutral (Dutch). Note that the angry face emoticon is a placeholder and the real experiment used images of the NimStim face stimulus set (Tottenham et al., 2009), which includes photographs of actors expressing different emotions.

Fig. 1.

(A) Exemplary trial of the rewarded emotion-discrimination task paradigm, featuring a stimulus, a response, and a feedback event. The time line indicates the duration of each event, as well as the duration of the fixation screens in between. Participants are asked to discriminate the emotional expression of the face by selecting the correct option during the response event. At the same time, the gender of the face signals whether a reward for a correct response in this trial. (B) Response times and accuracy according to the experimental factors reward (reward, no-reward) and valence (negative, neutral, positive). The black line represents the mean values of RT and accuracy in the different conditions, the violin plots the distribution of the data. BOOS: Angry; NEUT: Neutral (Dutch). Note that the angry face emoticon is a placeholder and the real experiment used images of the NimStim face stimulus set (Tottenham et al., 2009), which includes photographs of actors expressing different emotions.

Close modal

2.3 Image acquisition and pre-processing

MR images were acquired using a 3 Tesla Siemens Magnetom Prisma scanner (Siemens Medical Systems, Erlangen, Germany) with a 64-channel head coil at the Ghent Institute for Functional and Metabolic Imaging, Ghent University Hospital. For each functional run, we acquired T2*-weighted echo planar functional images in interleaved order with 69 transverse slices covering the whole brain, repetition time (TR)=2090 ms, echo time (TE)=27 ms, flip angle=79°, field of view (FOV)=112 mm, multiband acceleration factor=3; no interslice gap and voxel size=1.7×1.7x2 mm. T1-weighted structural volumes using a 3D magnetization-prepared rapid acquired gradient echo (MP-RAGE) sequence (TR=2250 ms; TE=4.18 ms; FOV=256 mm; flip angle=9°; 176 sagittal slices; matrix size=256 × 256; voxel size=1 × 1 × 1 mm; duration=5.14 min) were acquired for co-registration with functional scans. Additionally, a T2-weighted structural scan (TR=3200 ms; TE=4.08 ms; FOV=230 mm; flip angle=120°; 192 sagittal slices; matrix size=256 × 256; voxel size=0.4 × 0.4 × 0.9 mm; duration=5.09 min) was acquired for the localization of the dopaminergic SN/VTA complex. Finally, a neuromelanin-sensitive T1-weighted turbo spin echo (T1-TSE) scan (TR=559 ms; TE=98 ms; FOV=192 mm; flip angle 1=70°; flip angle 2=180°; 10 sagittal slices; matrix size=384x384; voxel size = 0.5 × 0. × 3 mm; duration=9.51 min) was acquired for the localization of the LC. The total scanning time, which included a localizer scan, the MP-RAGE scan, a fieldmap, three functional runs, a T2 and a T1-TSE scan was approximately 60 min.

Statistical Parametric Mapping Software (SPM12, Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, London, UK, https://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software) was used for image pre-processing. Functional scans were (i) realigned and unwarped; (ii) slice time corrected to the middle slice in each repetition time; (iii) spatially normalized using the T1-weighted image to improve segmentation accuracy to fit the standardized MNI-152 template. Normalized images were resampled into 1.5 × 1.5 × 1.5 mm voxels for ROI analyses and 3 × 3 × 3 mm for the whole brain analysis (see Supplementary Materials), (iv) and smoothed using a 3.5 mm for ROI analyses and 8 mm for the whole brain analysis full width at half-maximum isotropic Gaussian kernel. FSL’s tool ICA-AROMA was used to correct for (MB exacerbated) motion artefacts (Kelly et al., 2013). Anatomical images (T1, T2, and T1-TSE) were co-registered to the mean functional image and normalized to fit the standardized MNI-152 template.

2.4 Behavioural analyses

All behavioural statistical analyses were performed in Rstudio (R version 4.2.2). Accuracy data (%) and response times (RT, correct responses only) averaged across all blocks were submitted to a repeated-measures analysis of variance (rANOVA) with the factors reward (reward, no reward) and valence (negative, positive, neutral). Greenhouse–Geisser corrections are applied in case of sphericity violations. Significant interactions (and main effects of factors with more than two levels) were followed by post hoc t-test.

2.5 fMRI analysis

2.5.1 First level model

Statistical parametric maps were calculated for each participant using a general linear model analysis within an event-related design. The high-pass filter was set to 128 s to remove low-frequency noise, and each experimental condition was convolved with the canonical haemodynamic response function. Contrast images that represented the participant’s unique BOLD signal modulations in different conditions according to the experimental design (reward x valence) and trials events (stimulus, response, and feedback) were created and assigned to a total of 19 condition-specific regressors (including 1 regressor for incorrect trials). Finally, six motion regressors derived from the realignment procedure were included as additional regressors of no interest. Although the focus of this study is on modulations within LC and SN/VTA, we have included a whole-brain analysis to provide a complete overview in the Supplementary Materials.

2.5.2 ROI analysis

Individual masks for the LC and SN/VTA regions were manually drawn in MRIcroGL (Rorden & Brett, 2000) using the normalized single-subject T1-TSE and T2 scans, respectively. The localization of LC and SN/VTA is illustrated on single-subject anatomical images in Figure 2A, along with the respective averaged mask across all participants. Our segmentation procedures were carefully guided by anatomical landmarks as well as intensity differences in specific anatomical sequences that were established in previous work on the dopaminergic and noradrenergic systems in the human brain (Düzel et al., 2009, 2015; Keren et al., 2009; Krebs et al., 2011, 2012, 2018). Specifically, the SN is visible as bilateral low-intensity (dark) area on axial slices of the T2 scans across the medial midbrain, with the VTA adjacent to it in anterior-medial direction (Fig. 2A, cf Düzel et al., 2009). The LC is detected as bilateral high-intensity spot on the axial slices of the T1-TSE scan of the brainstem, close to the fourth ventricle (Fig. 2A, cf Keren et al., 2009). Note that we focused on the ventromedial part of the SN/VTA complex which has been considered particularly relevant for reward/valence processing (Düzel et al., 2015). The outer boundaries of the averaged bilateral masks in MNI coordinates and aligned to the brainstem axis are LC (left/right x = -8/8; ventral/dorsal y = -36/-41; rostral/caudal; z = -17/-30); SN/VTA (left/right x = -13/14; ventral/dorsal y = -7/-20; rostral/caudal z = -8/-14). Mean parameter estimates (beta values) were extracted from the individual masks using the MarsBaR toolbox version 0.44 averaged across runs for each condition and converted to an SPM-compatible format. The beta values from each ROI were submitted to a rANOVA with three factors: reward (reward, no reward), valence (negative, positive, neutral), and event (stimulus, response). Greenhouse–Geisser correction to degrees of freedom and p values was applied in case of sphericity violations. Significant interactions (and main effects of factors with more than two levels) were followed by post hoc t-test, with Sidak correction for multiple comparisons on estimated marginal means. The statistical analyses were performed in Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS), version 28.

Fig. 2.

(A) An illustration of the LC (left) and SN/VTA (right) masks of an individual and group average. (B) Mean beta values for each condition in the stimulus (top) and response condition (bottom) of the LC (left) and SN/VTA (right). The black line represents the mean values of RT and accuracy in the different conditions, the violon plots the distribution of the data.

Fig. 2.

(A) An illustration of the LC (left) and SN/VTA (right) masks of an individual and group average. (B) Mean beta values for each condition in the stimulus (top) and response condition (bottom) of the LC (left) and SN/VTA (right). The black line represents the mean values of RT and accuracy in the different conditions, the violon plots the distribution of the data.

Close modal

2.5.3 Generalized psychophysiological interactions (gPPI) analysis

To investigate the connectivity modulated in a task-dependent manner between pre-defined seed regions (LC and the SN/VTA) and other regions in the brain, we used a gPPI analysis (McLaren et al., 2012) implemented in CONN toolbox in Matlab (version 2022b). The gPPI analysis convolves the BOLD signal with the canonical haemodynamic response function for each condition before defining the contrast, creating a unique psychological regressor for each condition (unlike the standard PPI approach, which includes contrast information when forming a psychological regressor). To determine the interaction effects between the regressors (gPPI term, physiological vector, and psychological vector), random-effects analyses were performed for those contrasts that featured significant differential activity in the ROI analysis (p < .05, FDR corrected). In addition, covariations between LC and SN/VTA for both experimental factors were explored employing a direct ROI-to-ROI approach.

3.1 Behavioural results

Mean accuracy rates for negative (M = 0.91, SD = 0.29, positive (M = 0.89, SD = 0.32) and neutral valence trials (M = 0.88, SD = 0.32) did not differ significantly (main effect valence F(2,72) = 2.72, p = .066). Mean accuracies for reward (M = 0.90, SD = 0.31) and no-reward trials (M = 0.89, SD = 0.32) did not differ significantly either (main effect reward F(1,37) = 1.57, p = .211), and there was no interaction between the factors (p = .905).

In the RT data, we did observe a main effect of valence (F(2,74) = 7.80, p < .007 (sphericity corrected), with significantly shorter RTs for negative (M = 546 ms, SD = 108) compared with both positive (M = 560 ms, SD = 108; t(71) = -3.94, p < .001 Bonferroni corrected) and neutral valence trials (M = 556 ms, SD = 109 t(71) = -2.75, p = .015 Bonferroni corrected). Further, there was a main effect of reward (F(1,37) = 10.34, p = .005), with shorter RTs for reward (M = 549 ms, SD = 107) compared with no-reward trials (M = 559 ms, SD = 110). There was no interaction between the factors (F(2,74) = 0.09, p = .942). Performance data are shown in Figure 1B.

3.2 fMRI results

3.2.1 ROI analysis

An overview of the ROI analysis results can be found in Table 1. The rANOVA of beta values in the LC revealed a trend for the factor reward (F(1,37) = 3.64, p = .064) with higher activity in reward compared with no-reward trials. This trend was accompanied by a significant interaction between reward and valence (F(2,74) = 3.79, p = .027). Post hoc tests showed that this interaction was due to significantly increased activity for positive reward trials compared with both neutral reward trials (t(37) = 2.68, p = .032) and positive no-reward trials (t(37) = 3.32, p = .002). In addition, the analysis revealed a significant interaction between valence and event (F(2,74) = 5.11, p = .008), which arose from differentially increased activity for negative valence trials in the response phase in comparison with neutral (t(37) = 1.87, p = .022). In contrast, during stimulus presentation, negative valence was associated with reduced activity compared with positive valence (t(37) = -2.75, p = .028). Consistent with this reversal, activity for negative valence was significantly lower in the stimulus compared with the response phase (t(37) = -3.13, p = .003). The main effects of valence and event were not significant (both p > .200) beyond the above interactions. The remaining two interactions were not significant (both p > .100).

Table 1.

Significant effects in the ROI analysis in LC and SN/VTA.

LC
Main effects and interactions df F p η2p 
 Reward (reward—no-reward) 1, 37 3.64 .064 .090 
 Reward x valence 2, 74 3.79 .027 .093 
 Valence x event 2, 74 5.11 .008 .121 
Post hoc tests for interactions df t p  
 Positive reward - neutral reward 37 2.68 .032  
 Positive reward - positive no reward 37 3.32 .002  
 Negative - neutral response phase 37 1.87 .022  
 Negative - positive stimulus phase 37 -2.75 .028  
 Negative stimulus - negative response phase 37 -3.13 .003  
LC
Main effects and interactions df F p η2p 
 Reward (reward—no-reward) 1, 37 3.64 .064 .090 
 Reward x valence 2, 74 3.79 .027 .093 
 Valence x event 2, 74 5.11 .008 .121 
Post hoc tests for interactions df t p  
 Positive reward - neutral reward 37 2.68 .032  
 Positive reward - positive no reward 37 3.32 .002  
 Negative - neutral response phase 37 1.87 .022  
 Negative - positive stimulus phase 37 -2.75 .028  
 Negative stimulus - negative response phase 37 -3.13 .003  
SN/VTA
Main effects and interactions df F p η2p 
 Event (response - stimulus phase) 1, 37 6.18 .018 .143 
 Reward x valence x event 2, 74 3.52 .035 .087 
Post hoc tests for interactions df F/t p  
 Response phase: reward x valence 2, 74 4.52 .014  
 neutral reward - neutral no reward 37 2.61 .013  
 negative no reward - neutral no reward 37 3.38 .005  
SN/VTA
Main effects and interactions df F p η2p 
 Event (response - stimulus phase) 1, 37 6.18 .018 .143 
 Reward x valence x event 2, 74 3.52 .035 .087 
Post hoc tests for interactions df F/t p  
 Response phase: reward x valence 2, 74 4.52 .014  
 neutral reward - neutral no reward 37 2.61 .013  
 negative no reward - neutral no reward 37 3.38 .005  

Note. Only significant main effects and interactions are listed (p < .05, except one trend), as well as significant post hoc tests (p value corrected for multiple comparisons).

df = degrees of freedom; η2p = eta squared.

The corresponding analysis in the SN/VTA revealed a significant main effect of event (F(1,37) = 6.18, p = .018) with globally increased activity for the response compared with the stimulus event. This main effect was embedded in a significant three-way interaction between reward, valence, and event (F(2,74) = 5.52, p = .035). Breaking down this interaction by event type revealed a significant two-way interaction between reward and valence for the response event (F(2,74) = 4.52, p = .014), but not for the stimulus event (p > .200). The interaction in the response phase was characterized by increased activity for neutral reward compared with neutral no-reward trials (t(37) = 2.61, p = .013), as well as for negative no-reward compared with neutral no-reward trials (t(37) = 3.38, p = .005). The main effects of reward and valence were not significant (both p > .400) beyond the above three-way interaction. The three remaining interactions were not significant (all p > .100).

3.2.2 gPPI results

We also explored condition-dependent covariation patterns between the SN/VTA and LC seed regions and the rest of the brain via gPPI analyses. Note that only comparisons that yielded significant effects in the above ROI analyses were considered. An overview of significant whole-brain covariations can be found in Table 2 (corrected for multiple comparisons using the built-in false discovery rate (FDR)). For the contrast based on the reward x valence interaction, we found that across events, positive reward trials elicited connectivity between LC and right lateral occipital cortex (p < .025) compared with the neutral reward trials. No significant covariations were found between the LC and other regions for positive reward trials compared with positive no-reward trials (across event type). For the contrasts based on valence x event interaction, we found significant covariation between the LC and several clusters in the occipital cortex. Specifically, contrasting negative versus positive valence trials revealed negative covariations with the left and right occipital pole and right lateral occipital cortex in the response phase (left occipital pole: p = .006; right occipital pole: p < .001; right lateral occipital cortex: p < .001). The contrast negative versus neutral valence revealed a positive covariation with the right occipital pole (p < .001). No further significant covariations were observed for the LC or SN/VTA on the whole-brain level (all p > .05).

Table 2.

Significant generalized psychophysiological interactions (gPPI) between the LC and the whole brain.

Coordinates (MNI)
Interaction in ROIHBrain regionxyzt-valuek
Reward x valence: 
 Positive > neutral reward Lateral occipital cortex 50 -68 -06 3.57 24 
Valence x event: 
 Negative < positive response phase Occipital pole 14 -100 00 -6.97 45 
Occipital pole -16 -102 02 -5.21 27 
Lateral occipital cortex 42 -58 -6 -5.94 45 
 Negative > neutral response phase Occipital pole 04 -88 22 5.59 25 
Coordinates (MNI)
Interaction in ROIHBrain regionxyzt-valuek
Reward x valence: 
 Positive > neutral reward Lateral occipital cortex 50 -68 -06 3.57 24 
Valence x event: 
 Negative < positive response phase Occipital pole 14 -100 00 -6.97 45 
Occipital pole -16 -102 02 -5.21 27 
Lateral occipital cortex 42 -58 -6 -5.94 45 
 Negative > neutral response phase Occipital pole 04 -88 22 5.59 25 

Note. Brain region labels represent local maxima. Only clusters surviving an FDR-corrected threshold of p = .05 and consisting of at least five voxels (p < .001) are shown.

H = hemisphere (L = left, R = right); k = cluster size.

Finally, we assessed the connectivity between LC and SN/VTA directly (ROI-to-ROI approach) to explore potential concerted modulations during reward and valence processing. This analysis revealed increased connectivity between LC and SN/VTA for reward compared with no-reward trials (t(37) = 2.36, p = .024) in the response phase. There was no covariation related to the stimulus condition, or to the valence contrasts (negative vs. neutral; negative vs. positive, positive vs. neutral) in either the stimulus or response condition.

While animal studies have shown overlapping responses of the DA and NA systems to behaviourally relevant stimuli, modulations in these systems in response to different stimulus types have predominantly been studied independently from each other in humans (but see this study on conflict processing (Köhler et al., 2016)). The present study aimed to investigate the distinct and joint contributions of the SN/VTA and LC activity during reward and emotional valence processing in one experimental design. This design also allowed us to explore potential interactions between reward and emotional valence. Behaviourally, we found that participants were significantly faster to respond to both negative valence and reward-predicting stimuli compared with their respective neutral and no-reward counterparts. This pattern replicates the well-known motivational effect of reward cues on the one hand (Braver et al., 2014), as well as the arousing effect of negative saliency on the other hand (Carver & Harmon-Jones, 2009). Moreover, this pattern indicates that response facilitation in this task is driven by the specific stimulus type (negative or reward predicting) rather than general positive valence, which would be shared between reward-predicting and positive stimuli.

On the neural level, we found that LC activity was higher for trials entailing reward prospect—albeit only “trending” significant (p = .06). This is in line with animal studies demonstrating that noradrenergic cells are sensitive to reward contingencies (Bouret et al., 2012, Bouret & Richmond, 2015; Jahn et al., 2018). Importantly, this sensitivity to reward was qualified by emotional valence (reward x valence interaction), with a differential boost for reward-predicting positive valence trials as compared with reward prospect or positive valence alone. This could be interpreted as an additive effect of the two stimulus dimensions due to shared (or congruent) valence (Park et al., 2019). Notably, this effect (boost for reward-predicting positive trials) was observed across the trial, that is, not distinct for either the stimulus or response phase. Finally, we also found a significant relative increase of LC activity for negative compared with neutral valence in the response phase, and a relative decrease for negative valence overall in the stimulus phase compared with the response phase. The increase of activity in the response phase is in line with the LC’s sensitivity to aversive events in general (Aston-Jones & Cohen, 2005; Sara & Bouret, 2012), and also corresponds to the behavioural facilitation we observed. What is less intuitive is the relative activity decrease for negative valence during stimulus encoding. One possible interpretation could be that the negative valence is not driving LC activity as long as no action is required. This would, however, be in contrast with the involvement of the noradrenergic system in response to encountering infrequent stimuli in an oddball paradigm (Nieuwenhuis et al., 2005). However, it is possible that in the current paradigm, the interaction between different types of salient stimulus dimensions, as well as the separation of stimulus and response phase, gives rise to a more nuanced activation pattern in LC—which would also accommodate the increased activity for positive reward trials (or valence–congruency effect) in LC described above. An alternative interpretation could be related to response inhibition: since participants are instructed to not react until the response options are presented on the screen, LC activity, which may otherwise increase in the presence of negative valence, might be suppressed in the stimulus phase. One possible mechanism that may regulate these selective arousal states could involve the GABA neurons in the peri-LC. These neurons, which receive descending input from regions such as the mPFC (Lu et al., 2012), are known to directly inhibit the LC. Recent work in mice has suggested that in response to salient stimuli, the activity of these GABA neurons increases (Luskin et al., 2022). Additionally, activation of peri-LC GABA neurons has been shown to decrease exploratory behaviour. It is plausible that increased activity for negative stimuli during the response phase might reflect resource allocation needed for responding to behaviourally relevant events, similar to modulations observed in rewarded motor tasks (Bouret & Richmond, 2015). Since the movement itself (pressing the correct button) was the same across conditions, different salient outcomes (earn a reward, increase vigilance in the face of a potential threat) could increase the mobilization energy regulated by NA. Future research could investigate whether similar mechanisms are at play in humans, particularly in the context of emotion-discrimination tasks, to better understand the role of peri-LC GABA neurons in modulating selective arousal states.

Generally, investigations into the involvement of the LC in cognitive processes in humans are still scarce—which is partly due to challenges associated with scanning brain regions in the vicinity of ventricles and vessels (see below). The current results extend previous findings in other domains, including reward-based decision making (Varazzani et al., 2015), and the processing of infrequent (Krebs et al., 2018) or conflicting events (Hu et al., 2013; Köhler et al., 2016). What unifies these situations or stimuli is that they all are behaviourally relevant or at least salient with respect to other events in the respective task context. Moreover, the present LC modulations show that different sources of salient information can be integrated into an additive effect—in this case reward and positive valence.

Activity in the SN/VTA revealed differential modulations during the response phase only—that is, when actions had to be selected and implemented. Specifically, activity in the response phase was differentially increased for reward compared with no-reward trials of neutral valence, but interestingly, also for negative compared with neutral valence trials that entailed no reward prospect. While the first of these observations (sensitivity to reward) replicates a well-established phenomenon in both human and animal neuroscience (e.g., Düzel et al., 2009; Schott et al., 2008), the latter (sensitivity to negative emotional events) has not gained much attention in human neuroimaging studies. However, it is consistent with a more global role of the DA system in processing behaviourally salient stimuli, including reward-predicting, aversive, as well as surprising events (Bromberg-Martin et al., 2010). The observation of two almost independent modulations in the present data mirrors the performance effects and seems to be hinting at coding relative saliency, rather than an additive (or congruency) boost. While speculative, it could be possible that different SN/VTA neuron populations are coding for these different forms of saliency (see e.g., Krebs et al., 2009).

Finally, we explored functional covariations of LC and SN/VTA across the whole brain based on significant contrasts in the ROI analysis. Notably, the only covariations we found were between LC and occipital areas. Specifically, we found increased connectivity between LC and right occipital cortex during positive compared with neutral valence trials in the reward condition, independent of the phase (mirroring the prioritization of the positive reward condition observed in the LC ROI analysis). Moreover, connectivity was increased between LC and right occipital pole for negative compared with neutral valence in the response phase, independent of reward (mirroring the prioritization of negative valence especially during the response phase). These occipital regions are involved in visual processing, with a specific role in object recognition (including faces) and visuo-spatial processing (Haxby et al., 1991). The increased connectivity between LC and lateral occipital cortex for positive reward predicting as well as negative faces aligns with the finding that pharmacological up- versus down-regulation of noradrenergic signalling in humans improved versus impaired attentional selection and discrimination in lateral occipital cortex (Gelbard-Sagiv et al., 2018). In addition, we found a negative covariation between LC and bilateral occipital pole and right lateral occipital cortex when contrasting negative to positive valence in the response phase (again independent of reward). The occipital tip, which processes foveal vision necessary for activities requiring visual detail (Wong & Sharpe, 1999), and the right lateral occipital cortex, which is involved in object recognition (Malach et al., 1995), may have functions of lesser importance during this phase. Instead, participants are presented with word options (angry, neutral, happy) on the screen and are required to respond based on remembered emotional content. We tentatively suggest that this could be related to dampening of connectivity between LC and occipital areas due to higher cognitive load imposed by negative emotions, leading to network reconfiguration (Fraser et al., 2012). This reconfiguration may lead to a redistribution of attentional resources from visual processing to other cognitive or affective processes that are more relevant for the individual. Alternatively, there may be an attentional bias where resources are reallocated to focus on internal emotional states or response preparation. Note that these analyses were exploratory and partly contradictory and hence call for replication. Using fMRI, we are not able to consider the modular organization of LC’s projections; future research using optogenetics, chemogenetics, or viral tracing techniques will play a pivotal role in the understanding of the how the LC can regulate a broad range of behaviours and physiological processes. This will open avenues for investigating the specific roles of different LC neuron subpopulations and their potential as targets for therapeutic interventions in various neuropsychiatric and neurological disorders (Poe et al., 2020).

Finally, the ROI-to-ROI connectivity analysis revealed positive covariation between LC and SN/VTA for reward compared with no reward trials in the response phase. This aligns with their reciprocal neuronal connections (Chandler et al., 2014), which may drive the energization of actions associated with rewarding outcomes (Bouret & Richmond, 2015). Regarding the directionality of the influence, on the one hand it has been suggested based on animal models that the VTA triggers phasic activation of the LC in response to reward-predicting stimuli, which in turn serves further allocation of attention (Hofmeister & Sterpenich, 2015). On the other hand, animal addiction models posit that LC inputs to the VTA can modulate drug-seeking behaviour (Solecki et al., 2022)—which relies on the same dopaminergic circuits as reward processing. Aston-Jones and Cohen (2005) proposed that the anticipation of a reward enhances behaviour that leads to its attainment, driven by DA-dependent reinforcement learning. During this process, the phasic mode of LC-NE activity concurrently encourages the exploitation of the new reward source. Conversely, when the reward is no longer accessible or valuable, the phasic activity of the LC decreases, while its tonic mode increases, thereby promoting exploratory behaviour. This interaction between the VTA and LC is functionally important as it ensures that motivated behaviour is directed towards obtaining rewards efficiently, optimizing the balance between reward acquisition and resource allocation. While these are interesting avenues for further research, the present data are considered merely exploratory in this regard.

Previous work on the role of the NA and DA source regions in processing reward predicating and/or valenced stimuli in humans has mostly been selective for one of the systems or one of the stimulus dimensions. In the present study, we observed contributions from both regions to reward and valence processing within the same paradigm. First and foremost, we saw that especially negative valence stimuli were associated with increased activity in both LC and SN/VTA during the response phase. This confirms well-known sensitivity of the LC for aversive stimuli (Sara & Bouret, 2012). In contrast, sensitivity of the SN/VTA for negative stimuli is less well established but has been put forward in animal models (Bromberg-Martin et al., 2010; Horvitz, 2000). As such, the present data confirm that both systems are especially sensitive to behaviourally relevant negative valence stimuli—as one type of salient stimuli in the environment. Considering the other salient stimulus dimension, that is, reward prospect, the patterns were slightly diverging in the two regions: while the LC featured a valence-dependent boost in reward trials with highest activity for positive valence, the SN/VTA featured selective boosts for reward trials of neutral valence as well as for negative valence trials without reward prospect. The LC pattern is consistent with the notion of an additive (or congruency) effect of overlapping valence. Specifically, in a previous study, we found that signalling reward prospect by positive valence explicitly led to behavioural facilitation (Park et al., 2019). In the present study, however, there was no additional performance facilitation, which is likely due to the separation of stimulus encoding and response implementation. In contrast to LC, the SN/VTA appears to code trials in terms of relative saliency (reward or negative valence) but does not “perform” an integration at this stage in the present paradigm. It is likely that such integration is realized in higher-order regions, including the striatum and fronto-parietal cortex, as we have seen in our previous study (Park et al., 2019). Together, the prioritization of reward and negative valence stimuli independently in the SN/VTA—together with the distinct LC modulation for negative valence stimuli—is most consistent with the two independent main effects observed in task performance. Although the reward-related modulations in the present study are generally consistent with related animal work that looked at both dopaminergic and noradrenergic cells within the same task (Varazzani et al., 2015), a direct comparison seems challenging as we added an additional valence dimension here.

Animal work has shown that DA and NA activity differs: DA responses are stronger when a reward is anticipated, whereas NA responses increase for unrewarded actions (Bouret et al., 2012). An interesting general observation of the current study is that the modulations in both regions appear to be better aligned to the response phase following reward prospect rather than to the presentation of the reward-predicting stimulus itself. The same holds for modulations in response to negative valence. Importantly, the increase in activity in the response phase is not a mere reflection of motor implementation as it is selective for specific conditions. Moreover, the response phase entails more than implementing a response: participants select the appropriate response from two (conflicting) valence options, hence involving a more or less effortful decision process. In contrast, the stimulus phase mainly required participants to encode the two stimulus dimensions (reward and valence). Thus, the concerted activation of the DA and NA system may be particularly important to select and invigorate responses to behaviourally relevant stimuli rather than mere stimulus evaluation (Hofmans et al., 2022). As such, this novel paradigmatic distinction between stimulus encoding and response implementation sheds new light on the contributions of the LC and SN/VTA during the processing of reward prospect and valence.

Thus, our data support the notion that both LC and SN/VTA are involved in reward and emotional valence processing. Both regions seem to be mostly sensitive to reward predicting as well as negative events. However, differences between regions surfaced based on the trials phase, as well as when considering interactions between the different factors. While the LC featured an additional boost of the reward response by positive emotional valence, activity in SN/VTA was independently modulated by reward and emotional valence, aligning more with the coding of relative saliency. Hence, despite the overlap, they do likely fulfil slightly different roles, which might be related to their respective projection targets (Ranjbar-Slamloo & Fazlali, 2019). Previous work has shown saliency engages other regions such as the anterior insula and anterior cingulate cortex, ventromedial prefrontal cortex, amygdala, ventral striatum, thalamus, and occipito-temporal cortex. While our whole-brain results (see Supplementary Materials) did show involvement of cortical structures such as the occipital cortex and medial frontal cortex, only the occipital cortex covaried with LC activity in the connectivity analysis (see above).

In this final section we are considering potential limitations of the present study. First, neuroimaging of small, deep structures requires smaller voxel sizes for accurate localization and signal extraction, which in turn comes with the inherent limitation of a low signal-to-noise ratio (Edelstein et al., 1986). Hence, we might be underestimating, or even missing, potential existing modulations in the target regions. Moreover, the increased MB sampling frequency may induce noise amplification, especially in subcortical regions (Risk et al., 2021), but deemed necessary to acquire the ambitious sequence scanning both the brainstem with high resolution and the neocortex to allow for connectivity analyses. Another challenge is the close vicinity of LC and SN/VTA to adjacent ventricles and vessels, rendering them susceptible to physiological noise. To account for these interferences, we have applied ICA-AROMA to the functional data, which is a data-driven method to identify and remove motion artefacts stemming from head movements but also blood pulsation. Due to a technical artefact most likely induced by the interaction between the MB sequence and (head) motion, we needed to exclude six participants, and, therefore, did not reach our pre-registered sample of 40 participants. Further, it was surprising that no significant covariation clusters were detected when using SN/VTA as a seed region. This could be related to the signal-to-noise issue but might be further complicated by functional segregation within this region. Specifically, it has been suggested that different neuron populations in the SN/VTA complex subserve different functions (Düzel et al., 2009), which could dilute results from (potentially underpowered) connectivity analyses. That said, further segregation of the SN/VTA is beyond the scope of the present study and would require a larger sample. From a paradigmatic perspective, it is valuable to consider in how far the activity modulations observed in the regions of interest might have been influenced by the unconventional delaying of the response phase—which allowed us to assess stimulus encoding and response implementation separately. The main cost induced by this design might be that the strength of the neural response elicited by the reward and valence manipulation might decay during the interval and have less influence on action invigoration (as compared with a design which allows immediate responding). But since we still observe differential effects both on the behavioural and the neural levels, we do not consider this as a substantial concern—other than that we might underestimate the associated effects. Finally, we note that the results related to the negative valence condition are specific for angry facial expressions and may hence not necessarily generalize to other negative expressions (such as sadness, fear, or disgust) or other negative stimulus material.

Finally, in this article, we have considered the activity of LC as a proxy for NA. However, these neurotransmitters are synthesized from the same precursor, tyrosine, and undergo similar biochemical transformations, and the LC also co-releases dopamine (Kempadoo et al., 2016). Hence, the interaction between NA and DA in the LC may also have played a crucial role in modulating the studied processes. The co-existence and interaction of these neurotransmitters suggest a complex neurochemical balance within the LC, potentially contributing to its multifunctional role in brain regulation. Further studies are needed to elucidate the precise mechanisms and implications of dopaminergic and noradrenergic interactions in this and other brain structures. The present results should, therefore, be replicated and confirmed in future studies.

To conclude, the current study explored the intricate roles of the LC and SN/VTA system in the processing of reward and emotional valence. The findings underscore the sensitivity of both LC and SN/VTA to reward prospects as well as to negative valence. These findings challenge the dominant view of the SN/VTA involvement in merely positive events. Moreover, we found evidence for an additive effect (boost) of positive valence and reward in the LC, while responses in the SN/VTA were more aligned with coding relative saliency of reward and emotional events in their respective context. Intriguingly, both regions exhibited strongest modulations during the response phase, thereby emphasizing their essential role in goal-directed action invigoration, above and beyond stimulus encoding. However, the replication and validation of these findings in future studies are imperative to fortify our understanding of the interplay of DA and NA underlying reward and emotional processing.

The project is pre-registered at the Open Science Framework (OSF) at https://osf.io/2rsye). Data are available after submission of a formal (brief) project outline to the corresponding author and after a formal data sharing agreement has been signed. Code available on https://github.com/juliemaehall/LC_VTA_Reward_Valence/

J.M.H.: Investigation, formal analysis, writing—original draft. D.S.: Conceptualization, methods, investigation, writing—reviewing and editing. R.M.K.: Conceptualization, formal analysis, supervision, writing—reviewing and editing, funding acquisition.

This study was supported by a starting grant of the European Research Council (ERC) under the Horizon 2020 framework (grant No. 636110) awarded to R.M.K. and by a postdoctoral fellow grant of Fonds Wetenschappelijk Onderzoek awarded to J.M.H.

The authors have no competing interests to declare.

We would like to thank all volunteer participants in this study.

Supplementary material for this article is available with the online version here: https://doi.org/10.1162/imag_a_00336.

Adolph
,
D.
, &
Alpers
,
G. W.
(
2010
).
Valence and arousal: A comparison of two sets of emotional facial expressions
.
The American Journal of Psychology
,
123
(
2
),
209
219
. https://doi.org/10.5406/amerjpsyc.123.2.0209
Arnsten
,
A. F.
,
Wang
,
M. J.
, &
Paspalas
,
C. D.
(
2012
).
Neuromodulation of thought: Flexibilities and vulnerabilities in prefrontal cortical network synapses
.
Neuron
,
76
(
1
),
223
239
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2012.08.038
Aston-Jones
,
G.
, &
Cohen
,
J. D.
(
2005
).
An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: Adaptive gain and optimal performance
.
Annual Review of Neuroscience
,
28
(
1
),
403
450
. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604.135709
Berridge
,
C. W.
(
2008
).
Noradrenergic modulation of arousal
.
Brain Research Reviews
,
58
(
1
),
1
17
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.10.013
Berridge
,
C. W.
,
Arnsten
,
A. F. T.
, &
Foote
,
S. L.
(
1993
).
Noradrenergic modulation of cognitive function: Clinical implications of anatomical, electrophysiological and behavioural studies in animal models
.
Psychological Medicine
,
23
(
3
),
557
564
. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291700025332
Berridge
,
C. W.
, &
Waterhouse
,
B. D.
(
2003
).
The locus coeruleus–noradrenergic system: Modulation of behavioral state and state-dependent cognitive processes
.
Brain Research Reviews
,
42
(
1
),
33
84
. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0165-0173(03)00143-7
Bouret
,
S.
, &
Richmond
,
B. J.
(
2015
).
Sensitivity of locus ceruleus neurons to reward value for goal-directed actions
.
The Journal of Neuroscience
,
35
(
9
),
4005
4014
. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4553-14.2015
Bouret
,
S.
,
Richmond
,
B.
, &
Ravel
,
S.
(
2012
).
Complementary neural correlates of motivation in dopaminergic and noradrenergic neurons of monkeys
.
Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience
,
6
,
40
. https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnbeh.2012.00040
Bouret
,
S.
, &
Sara
,
S. J.
(
2005
).
Network reset: A simplified overarching theory of locus coeruleus noradrenaline function
.
Trends in Neurosciences
,
28
(
11
),
574
582
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2005.09.002
Bradley
,
M. M.
, &
Lang
,
P. J.
(
2007
).
Emotion and motivation
. In
J. T.
Cacioppo
,
L. G.
Tassinary
, &
G. G.
Berntson
(Eds.),
Handbook of psychophysiology
(3rd ed., pp.
581
607
).
Cambridge University Press
. http://lib.ysu.am/disciplines_bk/7674251ee5f4a71467e7c74fd7af6167.pdf
Braver
,
T. S.
,
Krug
,
M. K.
,
Chiew
,
K. S.
,
Kool
,
W.
,
Westbrook
,
J. A.
,
Clement
,
N. J.
,
Adcock
,
R. A.
,
Barch
,
D. M.
,
Botvinick
,
M. M.
, &
Carver
,
C. S.
(
2014
).
Mechanisms of motivation–cognition interaction: Challenges and opportunities
.
Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience
,
14
,
443
472
. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-014-0300-0
Bromberg-Martin
,
E. S.
,
Matsumoto
,
M.
, &
Hikosaka
,
O.
(
2010
).
Dopamine in motivational control: Rewarding, aversive, and alerting
.
Neuron
,
68
(
5
),
815
834
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2010.11.022
Bunzeck
,
N.
, &
Düzel
,
E.
(
2006
).
Absolute coding of stimulus novelty in the human substantia nigra/VTA
.
Neuron
,
51
(
3
),
369
379
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2006.06.021
Burgdorf
,
J.
, &
Panksepp
,
J.
(
2006
).
The neurobiology of positive emotions
.
Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews
,
30
(
2
),
173
187
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.06.001
Carver
,
C. S.
, &
Harmon-Jones
,
E.
(
2009
).
Anger is an approach-related affect: Evidence and implications
.
Psychological Bulletin
,
135
,
183
204
. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013965
Chandler
,
D. J.
,
Waterhouse
,
B. D.
, &
Gao
,
W.-J.
(
2014
).
New perspectives on catecholaminergic regulation of executive circuits: Evidence for independent modulation of prefrontal functions by midbrain dopaminergic and noradrenergic neurons
.
Frontiers in Neural Circuits
,
8
,
53
. https://doi.org/10.3389/fncir.2014.00053
Cohen
,
J. Y.
,
Haesler
,
S.
,
Vong
,
L.
,
Lowell
,
B. B.
, &
Uchida
,
N.
(
2012
).
Neuron-type-specific signals for reward and punishment in the ventral tegmental area
.
Nature
,
482
(
7383
),
85
88
. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature10754
Düzel
,
E.
,
Bunzeck
,
N.
,
Guitart-Masip
,
M.
,
Wittmann
,
B.
,
Schott
,
B. H.
, &
Tobler
,
P. N.
(
2009
).
Functional imaging of the human dopaminergic midbrain
.
Trends in Neurosciences
,
32
(
6
),
321
328
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2009.02.005
Düzel
,
E.
,
Guitart-Masip
,
M.
,
Maass
,
A.
,
Hämmerer
,
D.
,
Betts
,
M. J.
,
Speck
,
O.
,
Weiskopf
,
N.
, &
Kanowski
,
M.
(
2015
).
Midbrain fMRI: Applications, limitations and challenges
.
fMRI: From Nuclear Spins to Brain Functions
,
581
609
. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-7591-1_20
Edelstein
,
W.
,
Glover
,
G.
,
Hardy
,
C.
, &
Redington
,
R.
(
1986
).
The intrinsic signal‐to‐noise ratio in NMR imaging
.
Magnetic Resonance in Medicine
,
3
(
4
),
604
618
. https://doi.org/10.1002/mrm.1910030413
Fraser
,
K.
,
Ma
,
I.
,
Teteris
,
E.
,
Baxter
,
H.
,
Wright
,
B.
, &
McLaughlin
,
K.
(
2012
).
Emotion, cognitive load and learning outcomes during simulation training
.
Medical Education
,
46
(
11
),
1055
1062
. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2923.2012.04355.x
Gelbard-Sagiv
,
H.
,
Magidov
,
E.
,
Sharon
,
H.
,
Hendler
,
T.
, &
Nir
,
Y.
(
2018
).
Noradrenaline modulates visual perception and late visually evoked activity
.
Current Biology
,
28
(
14
),
2239
-
2249.e6
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.05.051
Haxby
,
J. V.
,
Grady
,
C. L.
,
Horwitz
,
B.
,
Ungerleider
,
L. G.
,
Mishkin
,
M.
,
Carson
,
R. E.
,
Herscovitch
,
P.
,
Schapiro
,
M. B.
, &
Rapoport
,
S. I.
(
1991
).
Dissociation of object and spatial visual processing pathways in human extrastriate cortex
.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
,
88
(
5
),
1621
1625
. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.88.5.1621
Hinrichs
,
H.
,
Scholz
,
M.
,
Tempelmann
,
C.
,
Woldorff
,
M. G.
,
Dale
,
A. M.
, &
Heinze
,
H.-J.
(
2000
).
Deconvolution of event-related fMRI responses in fast-rate experimental designs: Tracking amplitude variation
.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
,
12
(
Suppl. 2
),
76
89
. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892900564082
Hofmans
,
L.
,
Westbrook
,
A.
,
van den Bosch
,
R.
,
Booij
,
J.
,
Verkes
,
R.-J.
, &
Cools
,
R.
(
2022
).
Effects of average reward rate on vigor as a function of individual variation in striatal dopamine
.
Psychopharmacology
,
239
(
2
),
465
478
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-021-06017-0
Hofmeister
,
J.
, &
Sterpenich
,
V.
(
2015
).
A role for the locus ceruleus in reward processing: Encoding behavioral energy required for goal-directed actions
.
The Journal of Neuroscience
,
35
(
29
),
10387
10389
. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1734-15.2015
Horvitz
,
J. C.
(
2000
).
Mesolimbocortical and nigrostriatal dopamine responses to salient non-reward events
.
Neuroscience
,
96
(
4
),
651
656
. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0306-4522(00)00019-1
Hu
,
K.
,
Padmala
,
S.
, &
Pessoa
,
L.
(
2013
).
Interactions between reward and threat during visual processing
.
Neuropsychologia
,
51
(
9
),
1763
1772
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.05.025
Jahn
,
C. I.
,
Gilardeau
,
S.
,
Varazzani
,
C.
,
Blain
,
B.
,
Sallet
,
J.
,
Walton
,
M. E.
, &
Bouret
,
S.
(
2018
).
Dual contributions of noradrenaline to behavioural flexibility and motivation
.
Psychopharmacology
,
235
(
9
),
2687
2702
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-018-4963-z
Kelly
,
M. E
.,
Duff
,
E. P.
,
Bijsterbosch
,
J. D.
,
Voets
,
N. L.
,
Filippini
,
N.
,
Moeller
,
S.
,
Xu
,
J.
,
Yacoub
,
E. S.
,
Auerbach
,
E. J.
,
Ugurbil
,
K.
,
Smith
,
S. M.
, &
Miller
,
K. L.
(
2013
).
An assessment of motion artefacts in multi band EPI for high spatial and temporal resolution resting state fMRI
. In
Proceedings of the ISMRM
, Abstract #3275. https://archive.ismrm.org/2013/3275.html
Kempadoo
,
K. A.
,
Mosharov
,
E. V.
,
Choi
,
S. J.
,
Sulzer
,
D.
, &
Kandel
,
E. R.
(
2016
).
Dopamine release from the locus coeruleus to the dorsal hippocampus promotes spatial learning and memory
.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
,
113
(
51
),
14835
14840
. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1616515114
Keren
,
N. I.
,
Lozar
,
C. T.
,
Harris
,
K. C.
,
Morgan
,
P. S.
, &
Eckert
,
M. A.
(
2009
).
In vivo mapping of the human locus coeruleus
.
NeuroImage
,
47
(
4
),
1261
1267
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.012
Köhler
,
S.
,
Bär
,
K.-J.
, &
Wagner
,
G.
(
2016
).
Differential involvement of brainstem noradrenergic and midbrain dopaminergic nuclei in cognitive control
.
Human Brain Mapping
,
37
(
6
),
2305
2318
. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23173
Krebs
,
R. M.
,
Boehler
,
C. N.
,
Roberts
,
K. C.
,
Song
,
A. W.
, &
Woldorff
,
M. G.
(
2012
).
The involvement of the dopaminergic midbrain and cortico-striatal-thalamic circuits in the integration of reward prospect and attentional task demands
.
Cerebral Cortex
,
22
(
3
),
607
615
. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhr134
Krebs
,
R. M.
,
Heipertz
,
D.
,
Schuetze
,
H.
, &
Duzel
,
E.
(
2011
).
Novelty increases the mesolimbic functional connectivity of the substantia nigra/ventral tegmental area (SN/VTA) during reward anticipation: Evidence from high-resolution fMRI
.
NeuroImage
,
58
(
2
),
647
655
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.06.038
Krebs
,
R. M.
,
Park
,
H. R. P.
,
Bombeke
,
K.
, &
Boehler
,
C. N.
(
2018
).
Modulation of locus coeruleus activity by novel oddball stimuli
.
Brain Imaging and Behavior
,
12
(
2
),
577
584
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-017-9700-4
Krebs
,
R. M.
,
Schott
,
B. H.
, &
Düzel
,
E.
(
2009
).
Personality traits are differentially associated with patterns of reward and novelty processing in the human substantia nigra/ventral tegmental area
.
Biological Psychiatry
,
65
(
2
),
103
110
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.08.019
Loughlin
,
S. E.
,
Foote
,
S. L.
, &
Grzanna
,
R.
(
1986
).
Efferent projections of nucleus locus coeruleus: Morphologic subpopulations have different efferent targets
.
Neuroscience
,
18
(
2
),
307
319
. https://doi.org/10.1016/0306-4522(86)90156-9
Lu
,
Y.
,
Simpson
,
K. L.
,
Weaver
,
K. J.
, &
Lin
,
R. C. S.
(
2012
).
Differential distribution patterns from medial prefrontal cortex and dorsal raphe to the locus coeruleus in rats
.
Anatomical Record
,
295
(
7
),
1192
1201
. https://doi.org/10.1002/ar.22505
Luskin
,
A. T.
,
Li
,
L.
,
Fu
,
X.
,
Barcomb
,
K.
,
Blackburn
,
T.
,
Li
,
E. M.
,
Rana
,
A.
,
Simon
,
R. C.
,
Sun
,
L.
,
Murry
,
A. D.
,
Golden
,
S. A.
,
Stuber
,
G. D.
,
Ford
,
C. P.
,
Gu
,
L.
, &
Bruchas
,
M. R.
(
2022
).
A diverse network of pericoerulear neurons control arousal states
.
bioRxiv
, 2022.06.30.498327. https://doi.org/10.1101/2022.06.30.498327
Malach
,
R.
,
Reppas
,
J.
,
Benson
,
R.
,
Kwong
,
K.
,
Jiang
,
H.
,
Kennedy
,
W.
,
Ledden
,
P.
,
Brady
,
T.
,
Rosen
,
B.
, &
Tootell
,
R.
(
1995
).
Object-related activity revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging in human occipital cortex
.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
,
92
(
18
),
8135
8139
. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.92.18.8135
Matsumoto
,
M.
, &
Hikosaka
,
O.
(
2009
).
Two types of dopamine neuron distinctly convey positive and negative motivational signals
.
Nature
,
459
(
7248
),
837
841
. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature08028
McCall
,
J. G.
,
Siuda
,
E. R.
,
Bhatti
,
D. L.
,
Lawson
,
L. A.
,
McElligott
,
Z. A.
,
Stuber
,
G. D.
, &
Bruchas
,
M. R.
(
2017
).
Locus coeruleus to basolateral amygdala noradrenergic projections promote anxiety-like behavior
.
eLife
,
6
,
e18247
. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.18247
McIntyre
,
C. K.
,
McGaugh
,
J. L.
, &
Williams
,
C. L.
(
2012
).
Interacting brain systems modulate memory consolidation
.
Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews
,
36
(
7
),
1750
1762
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.11.001
McLaren
,
D. G.
,
Ries
,
M. L.
,
Xu
,
G.
, &
Johnson
,
S. C.
(
2012
).
A generalized form of context-dependent psychophysiological interactions (gPPI): A comparison to standard approaches
.
NeuroImage
,
61
(
4
),
1277
1286
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.068
Nieuwenhuis
,
S.
,
Aston-Jones
,
G.
, &
Cohen
,
J. D.
(
2005
).
Decision making, the P3, and the locus coeruleus—Norepinephrine system
.
Psychological Bulletin
,
131
(
4
),
510
532
. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.131.4.510
Park
,
H. R. P.
,
Kostandyan
,
M.
,
Boehler
,
C. N.
, &
Krebs
,
R. M.
(
2018
).
Smiling faces and cash bonuses: Exploring common affective coding across positive and negative emotional and motivational stimuli using fMRI
.
Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience
,
18
(
3
),
550
563
. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-018-0587-3
Park
,
H. R. P.
,
Kostandyan
,
M.
,
Boehler
,
C. N.
, &
Krebs
,
R. M.
(
2019
).
Winning smiles: Signalling reward by overlapping and non-overlapping emotional valence differentially affects performance and neural activity
.
Neuropsychologia
,
122
,
28
37
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.11.018
Peirce
,
J. W.
(
2007
).
PsychoPy—Psychophysics software in Python
.
Journal of Neuroscience Methods
,
162
(
1
),
8
13
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2006.11.017
Pessoa
,
L.
(
2009
).
How do emotion and motivation direct executive control?
Trends in Cognitive Sciences
,
13
(
4
),
160
166
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.006
Poe
,
G. R.
,
Foote
,
S.
,
Eschenko
,
O.
,
Johansen
,
J. P.
,
Bouret
,
S.
,
Aston-Jones
,
G.
,
Harley
,
C. W.
,
Manahan-Vaughan
,
D.
,
Weinshenker
,
D.
,
Valentino
,
R.
,
Berridge
,
C.
,
Chandler
,
D. J.
,
Waterhouse
,
B.
, &
Sara
,
S. J.
(
2020
).
Locus coeruleus: A new look at the blue spot
.
Nature Reviews Neuroscience
,
21
(
11
),
644
659
. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41583-020-0360-9
Ranjbar-Slamloo
,
Y.
, &
Fazlali
,
Z.
(
2019
).
Dopamine and noradrenaline in the brain; Overlapping or dissociate functions?
Frontiers in Molecular Neuroscience
,
12
,
334
. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnmol.2019.00334
Risk
,
B. B.
,
Murden
,
R. J.
,
Wu
,
J.
,
Nebel
,
M. B.
,
Venkataraman
,
A.
,
Zhang
,
Z.
, &
Qiu
,
D.
(
2021
).
Which multiband factor should you choose for your resting-state fMRI study?
NeuroImage
,
234
,
117965
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.117965
Rorden
,
C.
, &
Brett
,
M.
(
2000
).
Stereotaxic display of brain lesions
.
Behavioural Neurology
,
12
(
4
),
191
200
. https://doi.org/10.1155/2000/421719
Sara
,
S. J.
, &
Bouret
,
S.
(
2012
).
Orienting and reorienting: The locus coeruleus mediates cognition through arousal
.
Neuron
,
76
(
1
),
130
141
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.011
Schott
,
B. H.
,
Minuzzi
,
L.
,
Krebs
,
R. M.
,
Elmenhorst
,
D.
,
Lang
,
M.
,
Winz
,
O. H.
,
Seidenbecher
,
C. I.
,
Coenen
,
H. H.
,
Heinze
,
H.-J.
,
Zilles
,
K.
,
Düzel
,
E.
, &
Bauer
,
A.
(
2008
).
Mesolimbic functional magnetic resonance imaging activations during reward anticipation correlate with reward-related ventral striatal dopamine release
.
The Journal of Neuroscience
,
28
(
52
),
14311
14319
. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2058-08.2008
Schultz
,
W.
(
2002
).
Getting formal with dopamine and reward
.
Neuron
,
36
(
2
),
241
263
. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00967-4
Schultz
,
W.
(
2013
).
Updating dopamine reward signals
.
Current Opinion in Neurobiology
,
23
(
2
),
229
238
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2012.11.012
Schultz
,
W.
,
Dayan
,
P.
, &
Montague
,
P. R.
(
1997
).
A neural substrate of prediction and reward
.
Science (New York, N.Y.)
,
275
(
5306
),
1593
1599
. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.275.5306.1593
Smith
,
E.
,
Weinberg
,
A.
,
Moran
,
T.
, &
Hajcak
,
G.
(
2013
).
Electrocortical responses to NIMSTIM facial expressions of emotion
.
International Journal of Psychophysiology
,
88
(
1
),
17
25
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.12.004
Solecki
,
W. B.
,
Kielbinski
,
M.
,
Wilczkowski
,
M.
,
Zajda
,
K.
,
Karwowska
,
K.
,
Joanna
,
B.
,
Rajfur
,
Z.
, &
Przewłocki
,
R.
(
2022
).
Regulation of cocaine seeking behavior by locus coeruleus noradrenergic activity in the ventral tegmental area is time- and contingency-dependent
.
Frontiers in Neuroscience
,
16
,
967969
. https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnins.2022.967969
Steinberg
,
E. E.
,
Keiflin
,
R.
,
Boivin
,
J. R.
,
Witten
,
I. B.
,
Deisseroth
,
K.
, &
Janak
,
P. H.
(
2013
).
A causal link between prediction errors, dopamine neurons and learning
.
Nature Neuroscience
,
16
(
7
),
966
973
. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3413
Tottenham
,
N.
,
Tanaka
,
J. W.
,
Leon
,
A. C.
,
McCarry
,
T.
,
Nurse
,
M.
,
Hare
,
T. A.
,
Marcus
,
D. J.
,
Westerlund
,
A.
,
Casey
,
B. J.
, &
Nelson
,
C.
(
2009
).
The NimStim set of facial expressions: Judgments from untrained research participants
.
Psychiatry Research
,
168
(
3
),
242
249
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2008.05.006
Varazzani
,
C.
,
San-Galli
,
A.
,
Gilardeau
,
S.
, &
Bouret
,
S.
(
2015
).
Noradrenaline and dopamine neurons in the reward/effort trade-off: A direct electrophysiological comparison in behaving monkeys
.
The Journal of Neuroscience
,
35
(
20
),
7866
. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0454-15.2015
Wise
,
R. A.
(
2004
).
Dopamine, learning and motivation
.
Nature Reviews Neuroscience
,
5
(
6
),
483
494
. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1406
Wong
,
A. M. F.
, &
Sharpe
,
J. A.
(
1999
).
Representation of the visual field in the human occipital cortex: A magnetic resonance imaging and perimetric correlation
.
Archives of Ophthalmology
,
117
(
2
),
208
217
. https://doi.org/10.1001/archopht.117.2.208
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International (CC BY 4.0) license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. For a full description of the license, please visit https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/legalcode.

Supplementary data