Adolescents and young adults born with a complex congenital heart defect (CHD) are at risk for executive function (ExF) impairments, which contribute to the psychological and everyday burden of CHD. Cortical dysmaturation has been well described in fetuses and neonates with CHD and early evidence suggests that cortical alterations in thickness, surface area, and gyrification index are non-transient and can be observed in adolescents with CHD. However, cortical alterations have yet to be correlated with ExF deficits in youth with CHD. This study aims to use a data-driven approach to identify the most important cortical features associated with ExF deficits in adolescents and young adults with CHD. To do so, we combined two comparable datasets acquired at the Research Institute of the McGill University Health Centre and the University Children’s Hospital Zurich, each including both youth with CHD and healthy controls. For each participant, a high-resolution T1-weighted magnetic resonance image, a self-reported ExF assessment (the Behaviour Rating Inventory of Executive Function – Adult Scale), and their clinical and demographic characteristics were available. Corticometric Iterative Vertex-Based Estimation of Thickness (CIVET) was used to extract cortical thickness, cortical surface area, and local gyrification index measures. Using orthogonal projective non-negative matrix factorization (OPNMF), we identified non-overlapping spatial components that integrate cortical thickness, cortical surface area, and local gyrification index and capture structural covariance across these features. Behavioral partial least squares correlation (bPLS) analysis was then used to compute correlations between the individual variability in the OPNMF covariance patterns and ExF outcomes for each subject. A total of 56 youth with CHD who underwent cardiopulmonary bypass surgery before 3 years of age and 56 age- and sex-matched healthy controls were included in our analyses. Cortical grey matter volume, cortical thickness, and cortical surface area were found to be significantly reduced in CHD patients compared to controls. OPNMF identified 12 stable cortex-wide components summarizing the inter-subject variability in cortical thickness, cortical surface area, and local gyrification index. bPLS revealed two significant latent variables (LV) accounting for a total of 82.8% of the variance in the sample, each describing distinct patterns between the brain and cognitive data. LV1 summarized a pattern of belonging to the CHD group, worse scores on most Behaviour Rating Inventory of Executive Function – Adult Scale (BRIEF-A) scales, younger age at MRI, and female sex. This pattern was associated with increased cortical thickness, local gyrification index, and decreased cortical surface area in several OPNMF components. Finally, we identified a positive relationship between the LV1 brain-behavior pattern and total aortic cross-clamp time in the CHD group, indicating that longer aortic cross-clamp time was associated with worse neuropsychological outcomes. In this study, we uncover novel multivariate relationships between ExF and alterations in cortical thickness, surface area, and local gyrification index in adolescents and young adults with CHD using a data-driven approach. Although our findings highlight the important role played by the cortex in higher-order cognitive processes, future studies are needed to elucidate the individual contribution of individual and clinical attributes into the deficits observed in this population.

Congenital heart defects (CHD) are the most common neonatal birth defects, affecting approximately 0.8% of live births, and are one of the leading causes of infant mortality (Bernier et al., 2010; Zimmerman et al., 2020). While advancements in healthcare have substantially increased survival age to throughout adulthood, it is now well recognized that many adolescents and adults with complex CHD will experience psychosocial and higher-order cognitive difficulties (Kovacs et al., 2022; Perrotta et al., 2020). Executive function (ExF) deficits are hallmarks of the cognitive challenges experienced by adolescents and adults with CHD (Calderon & Bellinger, 2015; Klouda et al., 2017; Marelli et al., 2016; Perrotta et al., 2020). ExF refers to a group of higher-order cognitive processes that are essential for self-regulation and behavioral control. ExF deficits may also negatively affect academic achievement, employability, and psychological well-being (Calderon & Bellinger, 2015). In survivors of CHD, worse ExF has been found to be associated with male sex, increased CHD severity, and poorer socioeconomic status (Cassidy et al., 2015; Klouda et al., 2017; Majeed et al., 2022; Sanz et al., 2017; Schlosser et al., 2022; Tyagi et al., 2017). While ExF deficits have been well characterized in the CHD population, the neural correlates of these deficits remain to be comprehensively assessed.

Several structure-function studies have found total and regional brain volume, sulcal pattern similarity, and white matter microstructural integrity to be associated with better ExF outcomes in adolescents and adults with CHD (Brossard-Racine & Panigrahy, 2023). Cortical dysmaturation, including reduction in cortical grey matter volume, surface area, and altered cortical folding, has been well documented in fetuses and infants with CHD (Bonthrone et al., 2021; Clouchoux et al., 2013; Kelly et al., 2017; C. Ortinau et al., 2013; C. M. Ortinau et al., 2019; Schellen et al., 2015; Skotting et al., 2021). However, they have not been extensively studied in older individuals with CHD. In adolescents with CHD, cortical grey matter volume was found to be significantly reduced when compared to healthy peers (von Rhein et al., 2014; Watson et al., 2016). Other studies assessing the more subtle features of the cerebral cortex in adolescents and adults with CHD, such as cortical thickness, cortical surface area, and gyrification, are even more scarce reporting sometimes divergent findings with little anatomical specificity or overlap (Cordina et al., 2014; von Rhein et al., 2014; Watson et al., 2016).

Recent studies have identified that sulcal pattern similarity in the left temporal and frontal lobes as well as the right and left hemispheres is associated with better ExF in adolescents and adults with CHD (Asschenfeldt et al., 2020; Morton et al., 2020, 2021). Nevertheless, many other cortical features, such as cortical thickness, cortical surface area, and gyrification, that have been associated with better ExF in other healthy and clinical populations, remain to be evaluated as possible correlates of ExF in individuals with CHD (Østgård et al., 2016; Sasabayashi et al., 2017; Tamnes et al., 2010).

Prior research efforts have generally overlooked the complex, inter-dependent relationship between cortical features and the interrelatedness of cortical regions when evaluating their association with ExF outcomes. Multivariate analytical frameworks that integrate multiple cortical features, including cortical thickness, cortical surface area, and local gyrification index, have recently been used to examine cortical development in healthy individuals and have highlighted associations between structural covariance patterns and demographics and cognitive ability (Kalantar-Hormozi et al., 2023). Structural covariance in the human cortex has been shown to give rise to higher-order cognitive functions such as ExF in previous studies (Mechelli et al., 2005). Moreover, the analytical framework proposed by Kalantar-Hormozi et al. (2023) has the additional benefit of preserving individual variability, which is critical when working with a heterogenous population such as CHD, where both cognitive outcomes and brain alterations exist on a spectrum. Therefore, the main objective of this study is to identify the cortical correlates of ExF deficits in adolescents and young adults with CHD. To do so, we choose to follow methodology similar to that proposed by Kalantar-Hormozi et al. (2023) to integrate MRI-derived measures of cortical thickness, cortical surface area, and local gyrification index and identify patterns of structural covariance using orthogonal projective non-negative matrix factorization (OPNMF). This allows us to account for the interdependent relationship between cortical features. Furthermore, we utilize behavioral partial least squares correlation (bPLS) to identify associations between structural covariance patterns and ExF.

2.1 Participants

A total of 128 participants with CHD (49.2% female) aged 16 to 32 years and 112 healthy controls (54.4% female) enrolled as part of two parallel case-control studies, one at the Research Institute of the McGill University Health Centre (MUHC) and the second at University Children’s Hospital Zurich (UCHZ), were considered for this study. The recruitment and enrollment procedures have been previously described (Ehrler et al., 2020; Fontes et al., 2019). Participants from these two cohorts were included in the current study if they had completed a high-resolution T1-weighted structural MRI and had available data on self-reported ExF. Only term-born participants (born >36 weeks of gestation) who underwent open-heart surgery using cardiopulmonary bypass before 3.0 years of age and who did not present with genetic abnormalities or intellectual disability (IQ < 70) were included in the CHD group in the current study. Healthy controls were excluded if they presented with any brain malformation, neurologic or neurodevelopmental conditions. From the original available datasets, a total of 75 participants (MUHC:15; UCHZ:60) had to be excluded as they did not meet the above criteria. Therefore, the sample proceeding to further analyses consisted of 62 adolescents and young adults with CHD and 102 healthy controls. Considering that the combined dataset consisted of a disproportionate number of controls to CHD participants, we implemented a matching method following exclusions due to quality control or processing failures to ensure that our analyses were not driven by the larger control group. Using the R (v4.0.3) (R Core Team, 2020) package MatchIt v4.5.0 (Ho et al., 2011), we matched healthy controls to CHD participants based on age and sex using Euclidean distance to compute the distance metrics and nearest neighbor method to identify matches.

This study was approved by the Pediatric Research Ethics Board of the MUHC and the ethical committee of the canton of Zurich in Switzerland. Written informed consent was collected from participants or legal guardians if they were younger than 18 years of age.

2.2 Executive function

To assess ExF, the Behaviour Rating Inventory of Executive Function— Adult Scale (BRIEF-A) was completed by all participants at both centers (Rouel et al., 2016). Participants from the UCHZ completed the German version of the BRIEF-A (Roth & Gioia, 2005). The BRIEF-A is a norm-referenced questionnaire that assesses ExF in everyday life and includes nine subscales: inhibit, shift, emotional control, self-monitor, initiate, working memory, plan and organize, task monitor, and organization of materials. These subscales can be combined to provide two summary scales: the behavior regulation index and metacognition index summary scores. The sum of all raw subscales provides the global composite score. Higher T-scores on the BRIEF-A indicate worse functioning and T-scores greater than or equal to 65 on any scales or indices are considered clinically abnormal (Roth & Gioia, 2005).

2.3 Individual and clinical variables

Clinical information that pertains to the ante, intra, and postsurgical period was collected from medical records for the CHD participants. The relevant clinical variables examined in the current study include CHD physiology (i.e., single or two-ventricle), the number of open-heart surgeries, age at first open-heart surgery, and total aortic cross-clamp time. We collected maternal education as a four-level ordinal categorical variable, with 1 being the lowest possible education level (elementary school or less) and 4 being the highest possible education level (standard College of General and Professional Teaching (CEGEP), college, or university education) as maternal education has been shown to be a reliable proxy of socioeconomic status (Asztalos et al., 2017). This scale was then dichotomized as high versus low, distinguishing maternal education that referred to a completed CEGEP, college, or university degree from maternal education lower than completed CEGEP or college, and was included in further multivariate analyses to facilitate the interpretation of the findings. Participant level of education was collected on a 5-level scale that recorded current or highest completed level of education depending on if participants were students or had graduated at the time of assessment.

2.4 MRI acquisition and processing

Participants from the MUHC underwent a brain MRI on a clinical 3.0 Tesla MRI system (Achieva X, Philips Healthcare, Best, The Netherlands). Three-dimensional T1-weighted images were acquired using a magnetization-prepared spoiled gradient-echo sequence (TE = 3.7 ms, TR = 8.1 ms, TI = 1,010 ms, shot interval = 3,000 ms, voxel size = 1.00 × 1.00 × 1.00 mm3, FOV = 240 × 240 × 180 mm, flip angle = 8°) using a 32-channel head coil.

Participants from the UCHZ also underwent a brain MRI on a clinical 3.0T MRI system (MR750, GE Healthcare, Waukesha, WI, United States). Three-dimensional T1-weighted images were acquired (TE = 5 ms, TR = 11 ms, TI = 600 ms, voxel size = 1.00 × 1.00 × 1.00 mm3, FOV = 256 x 256 x 154 mm3, flip angle = 8°) using a three-dimensional spoiled gradient echo (SPGR) pulse sequence. All images were then clinically reviewed for brain abnormalities by an experienced neuroradiologist who was blinded to the participants’ medical history at both centers. We categorized MRI abnormalities of potential clinical significance into two categories: 1) focal and multifocal abnormalities (e.g., gray matter heterotopia, white matter lesion/injury, focal infarction) and 2) global abnormalities (e.g., developmental anomaly, enlarged or asymmetrical ventricles). The range of abnormalities detected is reported in Supplementary Section 1.1. Raw images were visually inspected for any motion artifacts and processing quality prior to any other processing. Images were then preprocessed using the iterativeN3 preprocessing pipeline (https://github.com/CoBrALab/iterativeN3), which standardizes T1-weighted images by performing contrast inhomogeneity correction, intensity normalization, and masking the brain from non-brain tissue (e.g., neck and skull). Output images were then assessed for quality via visual inspection; any outputs that failed were not included in subsequent steps.

To acquire measures of cortical grey matter volume, cortical thickness, and cortical surface area, the corticometric iterative vertex-based estimation of thickness (CIVET; v2.1.1) processing pipeline was used (Ad-Dab’bagh et al., 2006; Lepage et al., 2017; A. P. Zijdenbos et al., 2002). Local gyrification index was estimated using the surface ratio method (Toro et al., 2008) and the CIVET pial surface (For details, see Supplementary Section 1.2). CIVET provides estimates of the cortical features at a total of 81,924 vertices across both hemispheres. Midline vertices were masked as cortical thickness and cortical surface area estimates in this region are unreliable. Therefore, the final analyses were performed across 77,122 vertices. Cortical feature extraction was performed on the CBRAIN platform (Sherif et al., 2014).

2.5 Harmonization

Following cortical feature extraction and participant matching, cortical grey matter volume, cortical thickness, cortical surface area, and local gyrification index were then harmonized to minimize the effects of scanner variability due to the multi-site nature of this study using CovBat (https://github.com/andy1764/CovBat_Harmonization). CovBat is a batch-correction tool that was applied to the cortical features to harmonize their mean, variance, and covariance across the two batches (Chen et al., 2019, 2022; Fortin et al., 2017, 2018; Johnson et al., 2007). For each of the four cortical features, a vertex-wise matrix was built where each row contained the cortical measure at each vertex, and each column contained all the cortical vertices of each subject. The four matrices were then concatenated vertically to form a vertex-by-subject matrix, which was then submitted to CovBat for batch correction in order to remove batch effects from the combined covariance of the four cortical features (Ziolkowski, 2022). Variability due to age, sex, and group status was preserved. The output harmonized vertex-by-subject matrix was then split to obtain four matrices containing each CovBat-corrected cortical feature. CovBat correction was performed in Python v3.9.7 (http://www.python.org). (For details on harmonization validation, see Supplementary Section 1.3.)

2.6 Statistical analysis

Descriptive statistics were first used to characterize the sample, and Shapiro–Wilk tests and normal quantile–quantile plots were used to assess the normality of variables. Group comparisons for participant characteristics and cortical features were assessed using analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) for continuous variables and Pearson χ2 or Fisher exact tests for categorical variables as appropriate. ANCOVA was used to control for potential confounders, including age, sex, and maternal education, when assessing group differences in BRIEF-A scores and cortical features. When the normality assumption was violated, Mann-Whitney U tests or log-transformed variables were used as appropriate. p-values less than 0.05 were considered significant. All descriptive statistics were performed in R v4.0.3. (R Core Team, 2020).

2.6.1 Orthogonal projective non-negative matrix factorization

Our analysis workflow closely followed that of Kalantar-Hormozi et al. (2023). First, to identify brain regions where inter-subject variability may exist in the examined cortical features and capture structural covariance, orthogonal projective non-negative matrix factorization (OPNMF) was employed (Lee & Seung, 1999; Patel et al., 2020, 2022; Sotiras et al., 2015). OPNMF approximates a decomposition of a given input matrix (V; m x n) into a component matrix (W; m x k) and weight matrix (H; k x n) such that every column of V can be recomputed as a linear combination of the columns in W using the coefficients (weights) supplied by the columns of H. The orthogonality constraint ensures that identified components are minimally overlapping and encourages sparsity in the solution. The result essentially identifies distinct brain regions, and helps with interpretation of the findings (Sotiras et al., 2015). Additionally, the non-negativity constraint of OPNMF ensures the data are represented in an additive and parts-based manner (Lee & Seung, 1999). The publicly available code at https://github.com/asotiras/brainparts was used to perform the OPNMF with Octave (https://www.gnu.org/software/octave/doc/v5.2.0/). OPNMF was initialized with a non-negative double singular value decomposition (SVD) with a maximum iteration of 100,000 and tolerance = 0.00001, as described by Kalantar-Hormozi et al. (2023).

Prior to submitting the input matrix into OPNMF, vertex-wise CovBat corrected cortical thickness, cortical surface area, and local gyrification index were residualized for CovBat corrected total cortical grey matter volume to ensure that any variability observed in the cortical features is independent of variability in total cortical grey matter volume across individuals. The three matrices of cortical thickness, cortical surface area, and local gyrification index were then concatenated to form the input matrix consisting of rows of vertices and columns of subject-metric combinations. The input matrix was then normalized using a z-scoring method on a per vertex basis as all structural metrics exist in scales of varying magnitude. To ensure non-negativity of the matrix, resulting z-scored metrics were shifted by the minimum z-scored value. The OPNMF input was prepared using the publicly available code at https://github.com/CoBrALab/cobra-nmf/tree/main/vertex.

To identify the optimal number of components, split-half stability analysis was carried out. To do so, the change in spatial stability and reconstruction accuracy of the decomposition was examined as the number of components increased, as previously described by Kalantar-Hormozi et al. (2023), Patel et al. (2020, 2022), and Robert et al. (2022). OPNMF was applied separately to a random two-group split of the participants, and the output similarity was compared to assess the spatial stability. The gain in accuracy was then estimated as we increased the number of components by observing the change in reconstruction error. A decomposition was considered optimal if it had high spatial stability and low reconstruction error with minimal complexity (i.e., lower number of components).

2.6.2 Behavioral partial least squares correlation analysis

To establish cortical features that covary with measures of ExF and participant characteristics, we implemented a behavioral partial least squares correlation (bPLS) model. bPLS is a multivariate analysis technique that can identify patterns of covariance between two sets of variables by applying SVD (Krishnan et al., 2011; McIntosh & Lobaugh, 2004). The output of bPLS is a set of latent variables (LVs) that describe the patterns of association between the cortical features and participant characteristics which maximally covary. bPLS was performed using the Python package pyls v0.0.1 (https://github.com/rmarkello/pyls).

The input to bPLS was a set of two matrices. The first contained the brain data in the form of the component subject-metric weights obtained during the OPNMF run, while the second, a participant characteristics matrix, contained the following information: disease status (coded as CHD = 1, no CHD = 0), age in years, sex (coded as female = 1, male = 0), the presence of brain lesions (coded as yes =1, no=0), maternal education (coded as high level = 1, low level = 0), and each of the BRIEF-A summary and subscales (continuous scores). Permutation testing, bootstrap resampling, and split-half stability were performed to assess significance and generalizability of results (For details, see Supplementary Section 1.4).

Although bPLS patterns were derived from analyses integrating both the CHD and the control groups, we were interested in investigating whether variations within the bPLS patterns in the CHD group aligned with clinical variables. As such, we additionally performed exploratory analyses to identify relationships between the participant expression of the brain (x-scores) and behavior (y-scores) patterns captured by the bPLS LVs and clinical and individual variables in the CHD participants. We performed Spearman’s rank-order correlation tests for continuous variables (age at first surgery and total aortic cross-clamp time). Associations with categorical clinical variables (CHD physiology and number of cardiopulmonary bypass surgeries) were not explored due to reduced distribution of the number of participants within the categories. p-values less than 0.05 were considered significant. Exploratory analyses were performed in R v4.0.3 (R Core Team, 2020).

3.1 Participant characteristics

A total of 13 participants were excluded for having not passed raw (n = 1) or post-processing (n = 12) image quality control. Following all exclusions and participant matching, the final dataset consisted of 56 participants with CHD (mean age 21.4 years) and 56 healthy controls (mean age 21.6 years) (For a complete sample breakdown, see Supplementary Fig. 1). Matched participants did not differ from the unmatched participants with respect to sex, maternal education, or performance on the BRIEF-A. The CHD and control groups did not differ with respect to maternal education; however, the participant level of education was significantly lower in the CHD group when compared to controls, with more healthy controls having completed or being in the process of completing a university degree than participants with CHD. A significantly greater proportion of CHD participants presented with MRI brain abnormalities than controls (p < 0.001; Supplementary Table 1). Participant characteristics are presented in Table 1.

Table 1.

Summary of participant individual and clinical characteristics.

Mean ± SD, n (%), median [range]CategoriesCHD (n = 56)Control (n = 56)p-value
Age at MRI (years)  21.4 ± 3.9 21.6 ± 3.7 0.692 
Sex Female 27 (48.2) 27 (48.2) 
 Male 29 (51.8) 29 (51.8) 
Maternal highest completed education levela Elementary school (6th grade) or less 1 (1.8) 0 (0) 0.059 
 High school (partially completed or graduated) 11 (19.6) 3 (5.4) 
 CEGEP/college partially completed (at least one year) or specialized training (e.g., apprenticeship) 16 (28.6) 17 (30.4) 
 Standard CEGEP, college, or university education 28 (50) 36 (64.3) 
Participant education levelb High school 11 (19.6) 6 (10.7) <0.001 
 Professional school or apprenticeship 16 (28.6) 8 (14.3) 
 CEGEP/College 19 (33.9) 12 (21.4) 
 University education/Technical College (undergraduate or graduate) 9 (16.1) 30 (53.6) 
 Other 1 (1.8) 0 (0) 
CHD physiology Single Ventricle 8 (14.3)   
 Two-Ventricle 48 (85.7)   
Age at first surgery (months; n = 50)  5.8 ± 8.4   
Number of cardiopulmonary bypass surgeries per individual (n = 52)  1 [1–3]   
Total aortic clamp time (n = 46)  72.7 ± 31.4   
MRI brain abnormalities  18 (32.1) 3 (5.4) <0.001 
Mean ± SD, n (%), median [range]CategoriesCHD (n = 56)Control (n = 56)p-value
Age at MRI (years)  21.4 ± 3.9 21.6 ± 3.7 0.692 
Sex Female 27 (48.2) 27 (48.2) 
 Male 29 (51.8) 29 (51.8) 
Maternal highest completed education levela Elementary school (6th grade) or less 1 (1.8) 0 (0) 0.059 
 High school (partially completed or graduated) 11 (19.6) 3 (5.4) 
 CEGEP/college partially completed (at least one year) or specialized training (e.g., apprenticeship) 16 (28.6) 17 (30.4) 
 Standard CEGEP, college, or university education 28 (50) 36 (64.3) 
Participant education levelb High school 11 (19.6) 6 (10.7) <0.001 
 Professional school or apprenticeship 16 (28.6) 8 (14.3) 
 CEGEP/College 19 (33.9) 12 (21.4) 
 University education/Technical College (undergraduate or graduate) 9 (16.1) 30 (53.6) 
 Other 1 (1.8) 0 (0) 
CHD physiology Single Ventricle 8 (14.3)   
 Two-Ventricle 48 (85.7)   
Age at first surgery (months; n = 50)  5.8 ± 8.4   
Number of cardiopulmonary bypass surgeries per individual (n = 52)  1 [1–3]   
Total aortic clamp time (n = 46)  72.7 ± 31.4   
MRI brain abnormalities  18 (32.1) 3 (5.4) <0.001 
a

One participant with CHD had missing maternal education; as such, we used the median maternal education (standard CEGEP, college, or university education) as an estimate of their maternal education.

b

Current or past highest completed education level depending on if participants were students or had graduated at the time of assessment.

CEGEP, College of General and Professional Teaching.

Significant p-values are indicated in bold.

Of the participants with CHD, 48 (85.7%) presented with two-ventricle CHD physiology, including 22 (39.3%) with transposition of the great arteries, 13 (23.2%) with tetralogy of Fallot, 7 (12.5%) with ventricular and atrial septal defects, 2 (3.6%) with double outlet right ventricle, 2 (3.6%) with truncus arteriosus type I, 1 (1.8%) with total anomalous pulmonary venous connection, and 1 (1.8%) with coarctation of the aorta. Single-ventricle physiology was present in 8 (14.3%) participants and included 3 (5.4%) with pulmonary atresia, 1 (1.8%) with Ebstein’s pulmonary atresia, 1 (1.8%) with double inlet left ventricle, 1 (1.8%) with hypoplastic left heart syndrome, 1 (1.8%) with tricuspid atresia, and 1 (1.8%) with unspecified single-ventricle physiology.

3.2 Executive function

Controlling for age, sex, and maternal education, mean scores were significantly higher on 9 out of 12 BRIEF-A summaries and subscales, indicating worse performance in the CHD group compared to the control group. Moreover, a greater proportion of individuals with CHD (12.5–28.6%) performed below the cut-off for clinically abnormal ExF as compared to controls (0–12.5%). The BRIEF-A results are presented in Table 2.

Table 2.

Comparison of mean outcome scores on the Behaviour Rating Inventory of Executive Function-Adult scale (BRIEF-A) in adolescents and young adults with CHD and healthy controls.

OutcomesCHD
Mean ± SD
Control
Mean ± SD
p-valueCHD
Abnormal n (%)
Control Abnormal n (%)p-value
Behavioral Regulation Index 53.9 ± 11.0 48.4 ± 8.1 0.001 9 (16.1) 4 (7.1) 0.237 
Inhibit 53.3 ± 11.2 49.9 ± 8.3 0.049 10 (17.9) 1 (1.8) 0.008 
Shift 53.4 ± 12.9 50.7 ± 10.2 0.17 8 (14.9) 5 (8.9) 0.557 
Emotional Control 53.6 ± 10.7 48.5 ± 9.7 0.004 9 (16.1) 4 (7.1) 0.237 
Self-Monitor 52.7 ± 11.8 46.2 ± 8.1 <0.001 9 (19.1) 1 (1.8) 0.016 
Metacognition Index 55.0 ± 10.9 50.0 ± 8.0 0.007 11 (19.6) 3 (5.4) 0.042 
Initiate 53.8 ± 11.7 50.9 ± 8.8 0.152 9 (16.1) 6 (10.7) 0.580 
Working Memory 56.2 ± 11.6 51.9 ± 9.4 0.031 16 (28.6) 5 (8.9) 0.014 
Plan and Organize 52.9 ± 9.5 49.4 ± 7.3 0.035 7 (12.5) 3 (5.4) 0.321 
Task Monitor 55.8 ± 10.9 52.9 ± 10.4 0.050 11 (19.6) 7 (12.5) 0.441 
Organization of materials 53.1 ± 12.0 46.2 ± 7.4 0.001 12 (21.4) 0 (0) < 0.001 
Global Executive Composite 54.9 ± 11.1 49.1 ± 7.5 0.001 13 (23.2) 2 (3.6) 0.004 
OutcomesCHD
Mean ± SD
Control
Mean ± SD
p-valueCHD
Abnormal n (%)
Control Abnormal n (%)p-value
Behavioral Regulation Index 53.9 ± 11.0 48.4 ± 8.1 0.001 9 (16.1) 4 (7.1) 0.237 
Inhibit 53.3 ± 11.2 49.9 ± 8.3 0.049 10 (17.9) 1 (1.8) 0.008 
Shift 53.4 ± 12.9 50.7 ± 10.2 0.17 8 (14.9) 5 (8.9) 0.557 
Emotional Control 53.6 ± 10.7 48.5 ± 9.7 0.004 9 (16.1) 4 (7.1) 0.237 
Self-Monitor 52.7 ± 11.8 46.2 ± 8.1 <0.001 9 (19.1) 1 (1.8) 0.016 
Metacognition Index 55.0 ± 10.9 50.0 ± 8.0 0.007 11 (19.6) 3 (5.4) 0.042 
Initiate 53.8 ± 11.7 50.9 ± 8.8 0.152 9 (16.1) 6 (10.7) 0.580 
Working Memory 56.2 ± 11.6 51.9 ± 9.4 0.031 16 (28.6) 5 (8.9) 0.014 
Plan and Organize 52.9 ± 9.5 49.4 ± 7.3 0.035 7 (12.5) 3 (5.4) 0.321 
Task Monitor 55.8 ± 10.9 52.9 ± 10.4 0.050 11 (19.6) 7 (12.5) 0.441 
Organization of materials 53.1 ± 12.0 46.2 ± 7.4 0.001 12 (21.4) 0 (0) < 0.001 
Global Executive Composite 54.9 ± 11.1 49.1 ± 7.5 0.001 13 (23.2) 2 (3.6) 0.004 

Significant p-values are indicated in bold.

3.3 Cortical features

Adolescents and young adults with CHD presented with significantly lower total cortical grey matter volume (F(1,107) = 26.78, p < 0.001) and mean cortical thickness (F(1,107) = 5.87, p = 0.017) and cortical surface area (F(1,107) = 23.46, p < 0.001) than healthy controls when controlling for age, sex, and maternal education. Mean local gyrification index did not differ significantly between the two groups (F(1,107) = 0.32, p = 0.57). Boxplots of cortical features can be seen in Figure 1. Univariate analyses between cortical features and ExF can be found in Supplementary Section 1.5.

Fig. 1.

Harmonized cortical features examined in the current study. (A) Total cortical grey matter volume (mm3), (B) total cortical surface area (mm2), (C) mean cortical thickness (mm), and (D) mean gyrification index. *p < 0.05, ***p < 0.001.

Fig. 1.

Harmonized cortical features examined in the current study. (A) Total cortical grey matter volume (mm3), (B) total cortical surface area (mm2), (C) mean cortical thickness (mm), and (D) mean gyrification index. *p < 0.05, ***p < 0.001.

Close modal

3.4 Orthogonal projective non-negative matrix factorization

The input matrix to OPNMF consisted of the harmonized cortical thickness, cortical surface area, and local gyrification index measurements of all subjects residualized for cortical grey matter volume. The optimal number of components that balanced both stability and reconstruction error was identified to be 12 through stability analysis (Supplementary Fig. 2). The cortical mappings of the spatial components can be seen in Figure 2. The following are descriptions of each identified component:

Fig. 2.

(A) Lateral and medial views of the left and right hemispheres of the 12 components identified through orthogonal projective non-negative matrix factorization (OPNMF). Darker regions on the cortical mappings signify vertices that load more heavily onto the component. (B) OPNMF normalized weight matrix highlighting the subject-specific weightings for each of the 12 components. Matrix was normalized for visualization purposes only. Distributions of un-normalized OPNMF weights for each metric are provided in Supplementary Figure 3. CT, cortical thickness; SA, cortical surface area; GI, local gyrification index.

Fig. 2.

(A) Lateral and medial views of the left and right hemispheres of the 12 components identified through orthogonal projective non-negative matrix factorization (OPNMF). Darker regions on the cortical mappings signify vertices that load more heavily onto the component. (B) OPNMF normalized weight matrix highlighting the subject-specific weightings for each of the 12 components. Matrix was normalized for visualization purposes only. Distributions of un-normalized OPNMF weights for each metric are provided in Supplementary Figure 3. CT, cortical thickness; SA, cortical surface area; GI, local gyrification index.

Close modal
  • 1)

    Component 1 (Motor): this component is largely localized to the primary and supplementary motor areas.

  • 2)

    Component 2 (Visual): strong localization to the visual cortex, encompassing both the primary and secondary visual areas.

  • 3)

    Component 3 (Language): this component occupies the posterior language area with some extension into the temporal lobe.

  • 4)

    Component 4 (Superior Temporal): most strongly encompassing the superior temporal gyrus, lateral sulcus, and supramarginal gyrus.

  • 5)

    Component 5 (Superior Parietal): largely localized in the superior parietal cortex and the more lateral regions of the precuneus.

  • 6)

    Component 6 (Temporal Pole): the strongest loadings into this component are in the temporal pole with some contributions from the medial temporal areas.

  • 7)

    Component 7 (Superior Frontal): this component occupies the superior frontal areas bilaterally.

  • 8)

    Component 8 (Middle Frontal): the middle frontal lobe loads heavily onto this component with some contribution from the left frontal pole only.

  • 9)

    Component 9 (Orbitofrontal): this component is heavily localized to the lateral and medial orbitofrontal cortex.

  • 10)

    Component 10 (Inferior Temporal): largely localized in the medial inferior temporal lobe.

  • 11)

    Component 11 (Anterior Cingulate): this component consists of the anterior cingulate cortex with some extension into superior frontal and paracentral areas.

  • 12)

    Component 12 (Posterior-Isthmus Cingulate): this component occupies the isthmus cingulate and portions of the posterior cingulate as well as the more medial regions of the precuneus.

3.5 Behavioral partial least squares correlation

The weights of the 12 components described above for each participant were subsequently related to participant characteristics and BRIEF-A measures to identify brain-cognition relationships. bPLS analysis revealed two statistically significant LVs each describing a unique relationship between the variables of interest and accounting for a total of 82.8% of the variability in the sample.

The first significant LV (LV1; p = 0.001) directly addressed the primary aim of this study. LV1 accounted for 66.1% of the variance (Fig. 3). The participant characteristics contributing to this LV included: CHD group, younger age at MRI, female sex, and higher scores (indicating worse performance) on 8 out of 12 BRIEF-A scales. These characteristics were associated with increases in cortical thickness in 7 out of 12 components, a decrease in cortical surface area in component 4, and increases in local gyrification index in 8 out of 12 components. The p-values corresponding to the left (brain pattern) and right (behavioral pattern) singular vectors from the split-half analysis are 0.029 and 0.071 respectively.

Fig. 3.

First significant (p < 0.001) latent variable (LV) identified by behavioral partial least squares analysis describing a pattern of belonging to the CHD group and worse performance on 8 scales of the Behaviour Rating Inventory of Executive Function-Adult (BRIEF-A). The bar plot (top) is describing the contribution of participant characteristics and each scale on the BRIEF-A. The x-axis denotes the correlation of each variable in the LV. Error bars are bootstrapped 95% confidence intervals. Green bars indicate variables that are contributing to the LV, and grey bars are variables that do not contribute. Each cortical map (bottom) shows the contribution of each component in the LV. Red indicates an increase of a cortical feature in a given component, and blue indicates a decrease in the cortical feature based on bootstrapping. CT, cortical thickness; SA, cortical surface area; GI, local gyrification index.

Fig. 3.

First significant (p < 0.001) latent variable (LV) identified by behavioral partial least squares analysis describing a pattern of belonging to the CHD group and worse performance on 8 scales of the Behaviour Rating Inventory of Executive Function-Adult (BRIEF-A). The bar plot (top) is describing the contribution of participant characteristics and each scale on the BRIEF-A. The x-axis denotes the correlation of each variable in the LV. Error bars are bootstrapped 95% confidence intervals. Green bars indicate variables that are contributing to the LV, and grey bars are variables that do not contribute. Each cortical map (bottom) shows the contribution of each component in the LV. Red indicates an increase of a cortical feature in a given component, and blue indicates a decrease in the cortical feature based on bootstrapping. CT, cortical thickness; SA, cortical surface area; GI, local gyrification index.

Close modal

The second significant LV (LV2; p < 0.001) accounted for 16.7% of the variance. This LV was marked only by younger age at MRI and female sex. These characteristics were associated with increases in cortical thickness across 10 components, decreases in cortical surface area across 3 components, and decreases in local gyrification index across 6 components. The p-values corresponding to the left and right singular vectors are 0.004 and 0.046 respectively. Considering that LV2 did not relate to the primary research question, further analyses were not performed on this LV.

Following the identification of the LVs, we were interested in assessing how clinical variables vary with respect to the brain and behavior patterns described by LV1. LV1 did not capture a consistent relationship between its brain or behavioral scores and age at first surgery (Fig. 4). This was confirmed by the exploratory Spearman’s correlation tests, which showed no significant relationship between the brain or behavioral patterns captured by LV1 and age at first surgery (r = -0.11, p = 0.46; r = -0.27, p = 0.07 respectively). Individuals who spent more time in surgery with a clamped aorta exhibited the brain and behavior pattern captured by LV1 more strongly than those who spent a shorter amount of time with a clamped aorta (r = 0.37, p = 0.01; r = 0.49, p < 0.001 respectively; Fig. 5).

Fig. 4.

Brain-behavior patterns captured by latent variable 1 (LV1) identified by bPLS and its association with age at first cardiopulmonary bypass surgery. The x-axis denotes the age at first cardiopulmonary bypass surgery in months. The y-axis on (A) denotes the projection of the brain pattern exhibited by LV1 onto the participant data (brain scores). The y-axis on (B) denotes the projection of the behavior pattern exhibited by LV1 onto the participant data (behavior scores).

Fig. 4.

Brain-behavior patterns captured by latent variable 1 (LV1) identified by bPLS and its association with age at first cardiopulmonary bypass surgery. The x-axis denotes the age at first cardiopulmonary bypass surgery in months. The y-axis on (A) denotes the projection of the brain pattern exhibited by LV1 onto the participant data (brain scores). The y-axis on (B) denotes the projection of the behavior pattern exhibited by LV1 onto the participant data (behavior scores).

Close modal
Fig. 5.

Brain-behavior patterns captured by latent variable 1 (LV1) identified by bPLS and its association with total aortic cross-clamp time. The x-axis denotes the total aortic cross-clamp time in minutes. The y-axis on (A) denotes the projection of the brain pattern exhibited by LV1 onto the participant data (brain scores). The y-axis on (B) denotes the projection of the behavior pattern exhibited by LV1 onto the participant data (behavior scores).

Fig. 5.

Brain-behavior patterns captured by latent variable 1 (LV1) identified by bPLS and its association with total aortic cross-clamp time. The x-axis denotes the total aortic cross-clamp time in minutes. The y-axis on (A) denotes the projection of the brain pattern exhibited by LV1 onto the participant data (brain scores). The y-axis on (B) denotes the projection of the behavior pattern exhibited by LV1 onto the participant data (behavior scores).

Close modal

Using a data driven approach, this study identified alterations in cortical thickness, cortical surface area, and local gyrification index on cerebral MRI that are associated with worse ExF in adolescents and young adults with CHD. We found that total cortical grey matter volume, mean cortical thickness, and total cortical surface area were significantly lower in the CHD group compared to the control group when controlling for age, sex, and maternal education. In contrast, mean local gyrification index did not significantly differ between the two groups. Our results converge with the few previous studies that have also observed global reductions in cortical grey matter volume and cortical surface area and no differences in local gyrification index in adolescents and adults with CHD as compared to controls (Morton et al., 2021; von Rhein et al., 2014; Watson et al., 2016). In contrast, previous studies assessing cortical thickness in this population have yielded conflicting results, reporting both widespread cortical thickness reductions in adults with CHD (Cordina et al., 2014) as well as no cortical thickness differences in young adolescents with CHD when compared to healthy controls (von Rhein et al., 2014). The discrepancy in cortical thickness findings may be explained by the differences in participant age and processing techniques between studies.

We discovered two significant LVs using bPLS describing distinct patterns of covariance between the OPNMF weightings of each participant and their characteristics and ExF abilities. The first LV described a pattern of worse performance on 8 out of 12 BRIEF-A subscales, belonging to the CHD group, as well as a substantial contribution of younger age at MRI and female sex. This pattern was associated with marked cortical thickness increases in the precuneus, posterior language, superior temporal, superior parietal, superior frontal, middle frontal, and cingulate regions, as well as reduced cortical surface area in the superior temporal area, and increased local gyrification index in all components except those encompassing the visual, posterior language, superior parietal, and orbitofrontal regions. Several of the identified components contributing to LV1 captured alterations within cortical regions that overlapped with the salience network (SN), default mode network (DMN), and fronto-parietal central executive network (CEN) nodes. This is interesting because EFs are thought to arise through interactions between the SN, DMN, and CEN rather than being mediated by a single cortical network (Menon & D’Esposito, 2022). Moreover, we identified cortical alterations within the lateral sulcus (i.e., component 4), which houses the insular cortex. This region, a key node in the SN, is hypothesized to regulate the activation of the DMN and CEN during stimulus-dependent and independent cognitive tasks and is also known to be structurally connected to nodes within these networks (Molnar-Szakacs & Uddin, 2022; Uddin et al., 2017). LV1 also revealed alterations within the anterior cingulate cortex, an SN node captured by our component 11, which is also thought to control the activity of the CEN, a network involved in goal-directed behavior such as decision-making (Sridharan et al., 2008). Furthermore, alterations were observed in the precuneus, encompassed by both components 5 and 12, which has been identified to be an important structure for cognitive flexibility, monitoring, and task switching (Schroeter et al., 2023; Yeager et al., 2022). Together, these findings suggest that LV1 captured underlying structural networks connecting multiple cortical regions known to be involved in ExF. These findings support the notion that alterations within these interconnected regions in adolescents and young adults with CHD likely drive ExF deficits.

ExF challenges are recurrent issues identified in survivors of CHD, often impacting various facets of the individual’s everyday life. Among these, working memory impairments are the most commonly reported in this population (Asschenfeldt et al., 2020; Badaly et al., 2022; Bellinger et al., 2015; Calderon & Bellinger, 2015; Cassidy et al., 2015; Sanz et al., 2017). When examining the contribution of specific BRIEF-A scores within our model, we found working memory to have the strongest contribution to LV1. Our results are in line with a previous study in typically developing individuals across the lifespan which also observed associations between higher cortical thickness and worse performance on working memory tasks in younger participants (i.e., children, adolescents, and younger adults), and no associations in adults aged 40 years and older (Krogsrud et al., 2021). This is congruent with the pattern observed in LV1 in our sample, which has a significant contribution of younger age at MRI. Although cognitive decline is thought to arise as a consequence of cortical thinning in older adults (Fjell & Walhovd, 2010), the contribution of younger age in the association between higher cortical thickness and ExF deficits may be explained, in part, by the ongoing brain maturation processes still occurring during adolescence and early adulthood. Throughout this period, cortical thinning is thought to reflect ongoing myelination (Natu et al., 2019) and synaptic pruning (Huttenlocher & Dabholkar, 1997). The rate of synaptic pruning is thought to be higher during adolescence in brain regions involved in higher-order cognitive functions, suggesting a critical role of synaptic pruning in the development of ExF (Selemon, 2013). Delayed or reduced synaptic pruning in these regions, as estimated by higher cortical thickness, may be one of the mechanisms that underlie the ExF deficits in CHD. Previous studies in healthy participants have reported associations between higher values of cortical thickness in the temporal and frontal regions, as well as the anterior cingulate cortex, with worse ExF such as cognitive control, working memory, and inhibition in children, adolescents, and young adults which support our hypothesis (Burzynska et al., 2011; Kharitonova et al., 2013; Krogsrud et al., 2021; Squeglia et al., 2013; Tamnes et al., 2010). While the correlation between higher cortical thickness and worse ExF is conflicting with the finding of overall reduced cortical thickness in the CHD group compared to the controls, we believe it is important to note that the bPLS model highlighted increased cortical thickness in specific cortical regions in relation to worse ExF, whereas the group differences do not account for regional variability in cortical features.

While we could not find previous investigations of other cortical features than cortical thickness in relation to ExF in CHD, we could compare our findings to studies in other clinical populations. For instance, in adolescents born preterm, a group that presents with similar patterns of brain and outcome profiles (Easson et al., 2019; Miller & McQuillen, 2007), lower levels of cortical surface area in the temporal areas of the right hemisphere were similarly correlated with worse ExF (Østgård et al., 2016). In young adults with first-episode schizophrenia, higher gyrification index in frontal, parietal, and occipital regions was associated with worse ExF, in line with our findings (Sasabayashi et al., 2017). This overlap across populations suggests a potential common signature of cortical alterations associated with ExF deficits that may not be diagnosis-specific and should be investigated further in future cross-diagnostic studies.

Poorer ExF outcomes in CHD can be driven by several clinical risk factors such as cardiac physiology and CHD severity (Cassidy et al., 2015; Klouda et al., 2017; Sanz et al., 2017; Schlosser et al., 2022; Tyagi et al., 2017). Although we found no relationship between the behavioral pattern of LV1 and age at first surgery, adolescents and young adults with CHD who had longer total aortic cross-clamp time expressed both the brain and behavior patterns captured by LV1 more strongly than those who spent less time on the aortic clamp. Longer cross-clamp time may be indicative of a confounding effect of longer or a greater number of surgical procedures in the presence of more complex cardiac physiology which we were not able to tease out due to the limitations that come with retrospective collection of clinical data.

From a methodological perspective, we want to recognize that although we use the same framework as Kalantar-Hormozi et al. (2023), the components identified by OPNMF differed between our two studies. The data-driven nature of OPNMF may give rise to components that are specific to each dataset and depend on a number of technical choices. Our diverging findings with Kalantar-Hormozi et al. (2023) may be explained in part in the different normalization methods used between the two studies. Specifically, as our research question sought to address how interindividual differences in cortical covariance patterns impact ExF in CHD, we chose to normalize on a per-vertex basis to ensure that the resulting OPNMF components capture interindividual variability. In comparison, Kalantar-Hormozi et al. (2023) normalized their data across both individuals and vertices which accounts for both inter- and intra-individual variability in the covariance patterns and identifies more metric-specific components. Nevertheless, the components we identified in the current study did overlap qualitatively with components described in another study (Patel et al., 2022) performed in a different sample, suggesting that the components we identified represent nonetheless a stable configuration of the human cortex.

Our findings must be interpreted within the context of the study’s limitations. First, the split-half analysis conducted on LV1 revealed an insignificant p-value (according to a threshold of p < 0.05) for the behavioral pattern of LV1, indicating an unstable LV. As described in Supplementary Section 1.4, the p-value obtained from split-half analysis is calculated by correlating the singular vectors between subsamples of our dataset repeatedly. Considering our total sample of 112 participants (56 with CHD, 56 without), we would expect that analyses performed on half of this sample would not have enough power to capture this pattern. Data-driven approaches benefit from large sample sizes, especially in highly variable populations such as CHD. While the merging of two datasets in this study intended to address this limitation, our rigorous inclusion and exclusion criteria resulted in the exclusion of about half of the individuals from the original datasets. Nonetheless, this study provides compelling evidence of a significant relationship between altered cortical morphometry and ExF deficits. Further validation in larger cohorts may improve the generalizability and robustness of these findings. The clinical and surgical variables included in the current study were extracted from medical charts completed more than 20 years ago; as a result, many participants had missing information. As such, our investigation of the relationships between LV patterns and perioperative variables was only performed on the subsamples with available data for each variable. Finally, due to the low number of participants in some of the categorical clinical variables, we were unable to assess other proxies of severity, such as CHD physiology or the number of cardiopulmonary bypass surgeries. As such, future studies with larger, more diverse participant samples should continue to explore these proxies of severity as potential risk factors for ExF deficits and cortical dysmaturation in youth with CHD.

This is the first study to use a data-driven approach to determine the contribution of cortical features to ExF variability in adolescents and young adults with CHD. This work reveals a significant relationship between worse ExF and variability in cortical thickness, cortical surface area, and local gyrification index across multiple cortical regions, particularly in the lateral sulcus, the precuneus, and in regions overlapping network nodes known to be involved in ExF. We hope that this study will pave the way for future work investigating the complex roles of the cortex in the functional deficits observed in CHD and emphasize the importance of optimizing care for promoting healthy brain development.

The data was prepared, analyzed, and visualized using publicly available code at github.com/CoBrALab/cobra-nmf/tree/main/vertex, github.com/asotiras/brainparts, github.com/alyssadai/nmf_viz. The Pediatric Research Ethics Board of the MUHC and the canton of Zurich in Switzerland prohibit us from making the data publicly available. However, data may be acquired through a formal data-sharing agreement and with appropriate ethics approval.

Fatme Abboud: Conceptualization, Data Curation, Formal Analysis, Methodology, Software, Visualization, Writing—Original Draft, and Writing—Reviewing and Editing. Kaitlyn Easson: Data Curation, Writing—Reviewing and Editing. Melanie Ehrler: Data Curation, Writing—Reviewing and Editing. Justine Ziolkowski: Methodology, Writing—Reviewing and Editing. Charles V. Rohlicek: Resources, Writing—Reviewing and Editing. Bea Latal: Funding Acquisition, Writing—Reviewing and Editing. Christine Saint-Martin: Resources, Writing—Reviewing and Editing. Guillaume Gilbert: Resources, Writing—Reviewing and Editing. Matthias Greutmann: Resources, Writing—Reviewing and Editing. Gabriel A. Devenyi: Software, Writing—Reviewing and Editing. Ruth O’Gorman Tuura: Resources, Writing—Reviewing and Editing. M. Mallar Chakravarty: Methodology, Resources, Supervision, and Writing—Reviewing and Editing. Marie Brossard-Racine: Conceptualization, Funding Acquisition, Methodology, Resources, Supervision, and Writing—Reviewing and Editing.

The study was funded by the Research Institute of the McGill University Health Center, McGill University, the Swiss Heart Foundation, and the Mäxi Foundation. At the time of the study, F.A. was financially supported by Canada Graduate Scholarships—Master’s (Canadian Institutes of Health Research). Marie Brossard-Racine received funding support from the Canada Research Chair Program.

Guillaume Gilbert is an employee of Philips Healthcare. The other authors have no conflict of interest to disclose.

We would like to thank the participants and their families for taking the time to participate in this study, as well as the research assistants and clinicians who were involved in this project. Computation for the above research was performed on the Niagara supercomputers at the SciNet HPC Consortium and the CBRAIN platform. SciNet is funded by the Canada Foundation for Innovation under the auspices of Compute Canada, the Government of Ontario, The Ontario Research Fund-Research Excellence, and the University of Toronto. CBRAIN is funded by Canada’s Advanced Research and Innovation Network and McGill University.

Supplementary material for this article is available with the online version here: https://doi.org/10.1162/imag_a_00371.

Ad-Dab’bagh
,
Y.
,
Lyttelton
,
O.
,
Muehlboeck
,
J. S.
,
Lepage
,
C.
,
Einarson
,
D.
,
Mok
,
K.
,
Ivanov
,
O.
,
Vincent
,
R. D.
,
Lerch
,
J.
, &
Fombonne
,
E.
(
2006
).
The CIVET image-processing environment: A fully automated comprehensive pipeline for anatomical neuroimaging research
. In
Proceedings of the 12th Annual Meeting of the Organization for Human Brain Mapping
,
2266
. https://www.bic.mni.mcgill.ca/users/yaddab/Yasser-HBM2006-Poster.pdf
Asschenfeldt
,
B.
,
Evald
,
L.
,
Heiberg
,
J.
,
Salvig
,
C.
,
Østergaard
,
L.
,
Dalby
,
R. B.
,
Eskildsen
,
S. F.
, &
Hjortdal
,
V. E.
(
2020
).
Neuropsychological status and structural brain imaging in adults with simple congenital heart defects closed in childhood
.
Journal of the American Heart Association
,
9
(
11
),
e015843
. https://doi.org/10.1161/JAHA.120.015843
Asztalos
,
E. V.
,
Church
,
P. T.
,
Riley
,
P.
,
Fajardo
,
C.
,
Shah
,
P. S.
, &
Investigators, for the C. N. N. and C. N. F. N.
(
2017
).
Association between primary caregiver education and cognitive and language development of preterm neonates
.
American Journal of Perinatology
,
34
(
4
),
364
371
. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-0036-1592080
Badaly
,
D.
,
Beers
,
S. R.
,
Ceschin
,
R.
,
Lee
,
V. K.
,
Sulaiman
,
S.
,
Zahner
,
A.
,
Wallace
,
J.
,
Berdaa-Sahel
,
A.
,
Burns
,
C.
,
Lo
,
C. W.
, &
Panigrahy
,
A.
(
2022
).
Cerebellar and prefrontal structures associated with executive functioning in pediatric patients with congenital heart defects
.
Frontiers in Neurology
,
13
,
827780
. https://doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2022.827780
Bellinger
,
D. C.
,
Watson
,
C. G.
,
Rivkin
,
M. J.
,
Robertson
,
R. L.
,
Roberts
,
A. E.
,
Stopp
,
C.
,
Dunbar-Masterson
,
C.
,
Bernson
,
D.
,
DeMaso
,
D. R.
,
Wypij
,
D.
, &
Newburger
,
J. W.
(
2015
).
Neuropsychological status and structural brain imaging in adolescents with single ventricle who underwent the Fontan procedure
.
Journal of the American Heart Association
,
4
(
12
),
e002302
. https://doi.org/10.1161/JAHA.115.002302
Bernier
,
P.-L.
,
Stefanescu
,
A.
,
Samoukovic
,
G.
, &
Tchervenkov
,
C. I.
(
2010
).
The challenge of congenital heart disease worldwide: Epidemiologic and demographic facts
.
Seminars in Thoracic and Cardiovascular Surgery. Pediatric Cardiac Surgery Annual
,
13
(
1
),
26
34
. https://doi.org/10.1053/j.pcsu.2010.02.005
Bonthrone
,
A. F.
,
Kelly
,
C. J.
,
Ng
,
I. H. X.
, &
Counsell
,
S. J.
(
2021
).
MRI studies of brain size and growth in individuals with congenital heart disease
.
Translational Pediatrics
,
10
(
8
),
2171
2181
. https://doi.org/10.21037/tp-20-282
Brossard-Racine
,
M.
, &
Panigrahy
,
A.
(
2023
).
Structural brain alterations and their associations with function in children, adolescents, and young adults with congenital heart disease
.
Canadian Journal of Cardiology
,
39
(
2
),
123
132
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cjca.2022.10.028
Burzynska
,
A. Z.
,
Nagel
,
I. E.
,
Preuschhof
,
C.
,
Gluth
,
S.
,
Bäckman
,
L.
,
Li
,
S.
,
Lindenberger
,
U.
, &
Heekeren
,
H. R.
(
2011
).
Cortical thickness is linked to executive functioning in adulthood and aging
.
Human Brain Mapping
,
33
(
7
),
1607
1620
. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21311
Calderon
,
J.
, &
Bellinger
,
D. C.
(
2015
).
Executive function deficits in congenital heart disease: Why is intervention important?
Cardiology in the Young
,
25
(
7
),
1238
1246
. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1047951115001134
Cassidy
,
A. R.
,
White
,
M. T.
,
DeMaso
,
D. R.
,
Newburger
,
J. W.
, &
Bellinger
,
D. C.
(
2015
).
Executive function in children and adolescents with critical cyanotic congenital heart disease
.
Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society: JINS
,
21
(
1
),
34
49
. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617714001027
Chen
,
A. A.
,
Beer
,
J. C.
,
Tustison
,
N. J.
,
Cook
,
P. A.
,
Shinohara
,
R. T.
,
Shou
,
H.
, &
for the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative
. (
2019
).
Removal of scanner effects in covariance improves multivariate pattern analysis in neuroimaging data
.
bioRxiv
, 858415. https://doi.org/10.1101/858415
Chen
,
A. A.
,
Beer
,
J. C.
,
Tustison
,
N. J.
,
Cook
,
P. A.
,
Shinohara
,
R. T.
,
Shou
,
H.
, &
Initiative
for the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging
. (
2022
).
Mitigating site effects in covariance for machine learning in neuroimaging data
.
Human Brain Mapping
,
43
(
4
),
1179
1195
. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.25688
Clouchoux
,
C.
,
du Plessis
,
A. J.
,
Bouyssi-Kobar
,
M.
,
Tworetzky
,
W.
,
McElhinney
,
D. B.
,
Brown
,
D. W.
,
Gholipour
,
A.
,
Kudelski
,
D.
,
Warfield
,
S. K.
,
McCarter
,
R. J.
,
Robertson
,
R. L.
,
Jr, Evans
,
A. C.
,
Newburger
,
J. W.
, &
Limperopoulos
,
C.
(
2013
).
Delayed cortical development in fetuses with complex congenital heart disease
.
Cerebral Cortex
,
23
(
12
),
2932
2943
. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhs281
Cordina
,
R.
,
Grieve
,
S.
,
Barnett
,
M.
,
Lagopoulos
,
J.
,
Malitz
,
N.
, &
Celermajer
,
D. S.
(
2014
).
Brain volumetrics, regional cortical thickness and radiographic findings in adults with cyanotic congenital heart disease
.
NeuroImage: Clinical
,
4
,
319
325
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2013.12.011
Easson
,
K.
,
Dahan-Oliel
,
N.
,
Rohlicek
,
C.
,
Sahakian
,
S.
,
Brossard-Racine
,
M.
,
Mazer
,
B.
,
Riley
,
P.
,
Maltais
,
D. B.
,
Nadeau
,
L.
,
Hatzigeorgiou
,
S.
,
Schmitz
,
N.
, &
Majnemer
,
A.
(
2019
).
A comparison of developmental outcomes of adolescent neonatal intensive care unit survivors born with a congenital heart defect or born preterm
.
The Journal of Pediatrics
,
207
,
34.e2
41.e2
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2018.11.002
Ehrler
,
M.
,
Schlosser
,
L.
,
Brugger
,
P.
,
Greutmann
,
M.
,
Oxenius
,
A.
,
Kottke
,
R.
,
Tuura
O’Gorman
, R., &
Latal
,
B.
(
2020
).
Altered white matter microstructure is related to cognition in adults with congenital heart disease
.
Brain Communications
,
3
(
1
),
fcaa224
. https://doi.org/10.1093/braincomms/fcaa224
Fjell
,
A. M.
, &
Walhovd
,
K. B.
(
2010
).
Structural brain changes in aging: Courses, causes and cognitive consequences
.
Reviews in the Neurosciences
,
21
(
3
),
187
222
. https://doi.org/10.1515/REVNEURO.2010.21.3.187
Fontes
,
K.
,
Rohlicek
,
C. V.
,
Saint-Martin
,
C.
,
Gilbert
,
G.
,
Easson
,
K.
,
Majnemer
,
A.
,
Marelli
,
A.
,
Chakravarty
,
M. M.
, &
Brossard-Racine
,
M.
(
2019
).
Hippocampal alterations and functional correlates in adolescents and young adults with congenital heart disease
.
Human Brain Mapping
,
40
(
12
),
3548
3560
. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24615
Fortin
,
J.-P.
,
Cullen
,
N.
,
Sheline
,
Y. I.
,
Taylor
,
W. D.
,
Aselcioglu
,
I.
,
Cook
,
P. A.
,
Adams
,
P.
,
Cooper
,
C.
,
Fava
,
M.
,
McGrath
,
P. J.
,
McInnis
,
M.
,
Phillips
,
M. L.
,
Trivedi
,
M. H.
,
Weissman
,
M. M.
, &
Shinohara
,
R. T.
(
2018
).
Harmonization of cortical thickness measurements across scanners and sites
.
NeuroImage
,
167
,
104
120
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.11.024
Fortin
,
J.-P.
,
Parker
,
D.
,
Tunç
,
B.
,
Watanabe
,
T.
,
Elliott
,
M. A.
,
Ruparel
,
K.
,
Roalf
,
D. R.
,
Satterthwaite
,
T. D.
,
Gur
,
R. C.
,
Gur
,
R. E.
,
Schultz
,
R. T.
,
Verma
,
R.
, &
Shinohara
,
R. T.
(
2017
).
Harmonization of multi-site diffusion tensor imaging data
.
NeuroImage
,
161
,
149
170
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.08.047
Ho
,
D.
,
Imai
,
K.
,
King
,
G.
, &
Stuart
,
E. A.
(
2011
).
MatchIt: Nonparametric preprocessing for parametric causal inference
.
Journal of Statistical Software
,
42
,
1
28
. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v042.i08
Huttenlocher
,
P. R.
, &
Dabholkar
,
A. S.
(
1997
).
Regional differences in synaptogenesis in human cerebral cortex
.
Journal of Comparative Neurology
,
387
(
2
),
167
178
. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19971020)387:2<167::AID-CNE1>3.0.CO;2-Z
Johnson
,
W. E.
,
Li
,
C.
, &
Rabinovic
,
A.
(
2007
).
Adjusting batch effects in microarray expression data using empirical Bayes methods
.
Biostatistics
,
8
(
1
),
118
127
. https://doi.org/10.1093/biostatistics/kxj037
Kalantar-Hormozi
,
H.
,
Patel
,
R.
,
Dai
,
A.
,
Ziolkowski
,
J.
,
Dong
,
H.-M.
,
Holmes
,
A.
,
Raznahan
,
A.
,
Devenyi
,
G. A.
, &
Chakravarty
,
M. M.
(
2023
).
A cross-sectional and longitudinal study of human brain development: The integration of cortical thickness, surface area, gyrification index, and cortical curvature into a unified analytical framework
.
NeuroImage
,
268
,
119885
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2023.119885
Kelly
,
C. J.
,
Makropoulos
,
A.
,
Cordero-Grande
,
L.
,
Hutter
,
J.
,
Price
,
A.
,
Hughes
,
E.
,
Murgasova
,
M.
,
Teixeira
,
R. P. A. G.
,
Steinweg
,
J. K.
,
Kulkarni
,
S.
,
Rahman
,
L.
,
Zhang
,
H.
,
Alexander
,
D. C.
,
Pushparajah
,
K.
,
Rueckert
,
D.
,
Hajnal
,
J. V.
,
Simpson
,
J.
,
Edwards
,
A. D.
,
Rutherford
,
M. A.
, &
Counsell
,
S. J.
(
2017
).
Impaired development of the cerebral cortex in infants with congenital heart disease is correlated to reduced cerebral oxygen delivery
.
Scientific Reports
,
7
(
1
),
15088
. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-14939-z
Kharitonova
,
M.
,
Martin
,
R. E.
,
Gabrieli
,
J. D. E.
, &
Sheridan
,
M. A.
(
2013
).
Cortical gray-matter thinning is associated with age-related improvements on executive function tasks
.
Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience
,
6
,
61
71
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2013.07.002
Klouda
,
L.
,
Franklin
,
W. J.
,
Saraf
,
A.
,
Parekh
,
D. R.
, &
Schwartz
,
D. D.
(
2017
).
Neurocognitive and executive functioning in adult survivors of congenital heart disease
.
Congenital Heart Disease
,
12
(
1
),
91
98
. https://doi.org/10.1111/chd.12409
Kovacs
,
A. H.
,
Brouillette
,
J.
,
Ibeziako
,
P.
,
Jackson
,
J. L.
,
Kasparian
,
N. A.
,
Kim
,
Y. Y.
,
Livecchi
,
T.
,
Sillman
,
C.
,
Kochilas
,
L. K.
,
American Heart Association Council on Lifelong Congenital Heart Disease and Heart Health in the Young, & Stroke Council
. (
2022
).
Psychological outcomes and interventions for individuals with congenital heart disease: A scientific statement from the American Heart Association
.
Circulation: Cardiovascular Quality and Outcomes
,
15
(
8
),
e000110
. https://doi.org/10.1161/HCQ.0000000000000110
Krishnan
,
A.
,
Williams
,
L. J.
,
McIntosh
,
A. R.
, &
Abdi
,
H.
(
2011
).
Partial Least Squares (PLS) methods for neuroimaging: A tutorial and review
.
NeuroImage
,
56
(
2
),
455
475
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.034
Krogsrud
,
S. K.
,
Mowinckel
,
A. M.
,
Sederevicius
,
D.
,
Vidal-Piñeiro
,
D.
,
Amlien
,
I. K.
,
Wang
,
Y.
,
Sørensen
,
Ø.
,
Walhovd
,
K. B.
, &
Fjell
,
A. M.
(
2021
).
Relationships between apparent cortical thickness and working memory across the lifespan—Effects of genetics and socioeconomic status
.
Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience
,
51
,
100997
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2021.100997
Lee
,
D. D.
, &
Seung
,
H. S.
(
1999
).
Learning the parts of objects by non-negative matrix factorization
.
Nature
,
401
(
6755
), Article 6755. https://doi.org/10.1038/44565
Lepage
,
C.
,
Lewis
,
L.
,
Jeun
,
S.
,
Bermudez
,
P.
,
Khalili-Mahani
,
N.
,
Omidyegaheh
,
M.
,
Zijdenbos
,
A.
,
Vincent
,
R. D.
,
Adalat
,
R.
, &
Evans
,
A. C.
(
2017
).
Human MR evaluation of cortical thickness using CIVET v2. 1
.
Organization for Human Brain Mapping
.
Majeed
,
A.
,
Rofeberg
,
V.
,
Bellinger
,
D. C.
,
Wypij
,
D.
, &
Newburger
,
J. W.
(
2022
).
Machine learning to predict executive function in adolescents with repaired D-transposition of the great arteries, tetralogy of Fallot, and Fontan palliation
.
The Journal of Pediatrics
,
246
,
145
153
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2022.03.021
Marelli
,
A.
,
Miller
,
S. P.
,
Marino
,
B. S.
,
Jefferson
,
A. L.
, &
Newburger
,
J. W.
(
2016
).
Brain in congenital heart disease across the lifespan
.
Circulation
,
133
(
20
),
1951
1962
. https://doi.org/10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.115.019881
McIntosh
,
A. R.
, &
Lobaugh
,
N. J.
(
2004
).
Partial least squares analysis of neuroimaging data: Applications and advances
.
NeuroImage
,
23
,
S250
S263
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.020
Mechelli
,
A.
,
Friston
,
K. J.
,
Frackowiak
,
R. S.
, &
Price
,
C. J.
(
2005
).
Structural covariance in the human cortex
.
Journal of Neuroscience
,
25
(
36
),
8303
8310
. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0357-05.2005
Menon
,
V.
, &
D’Esposito
,
M.
(
2022
).
The role of PFC networks in cognitive control and executive function
.
Neuropsychopharmacology
,
47
(
1
), Article 1. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-021-01152-w
Miller
,
S. P.
, &
McQuillen
,
P. S.
(
2007
).
Neurology of congenital heart disease: Insight from brain imaging
.
Archives of Disease in Childhood. Fetal and Neonatal Edition
,
92
(
6
),
F435
F437
. https://doi.org/10.1136/adc.2006.108845
Molnar-Szakacs
,
I.
, &
Uddin
,
L. Q.
(
2022
).
Anterior insula as a gatekeeper of executive control
.
Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews
,
139
,
104736
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2022.104736
Morton
,
S. U.
,
Maleyeff
,
L.
,
Wypij
,
D.
,
Yun
,
H. J.
,
Newburger
,
J. W.
,
Bellinger
,
D. C.
,
Roberts
,
A. E.
,
Rivkin
,
M. J.
,
Seidman
,
J. G.
,
Seidman
,
C. E.
,
Grant
,
P. E.
, &
Im
,
K.
(
2020
).
Abnormal left-hemispheric sulcal patterns correlate with neurodevelopmental outcomes in subjects with single ventricular congenital heart disease
.
Cerebral Cortex (New York, NY)
,
30
(
2
),
476
487
. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhz101
Morton
,
S. U.
,
Maleyeff
,
L.
,
Wypij
,
D.
,
Yun
,
H. J.
,
Rollins
,
C. K.
,
Watson
,
C. G.
,
Newburger
,
J. W.
,
Bellinger
,
D. C.
,
Roberts
,
A. E.
,
Rivkin
,
M. J.
,
Grant
,
P. E.
, &
Im
,
K.
(
2021
).
Abnormal right-hemispheric sulcal patterns correlate with executive function in adolescents with tetralogy of Fallot
.
Cerebral Cortex (New York, NY)
,
31
(
10
),
4670
4680
. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhab114
Natu
,
V. S.
,
Gomez
,
J.
,
Barnett
,
M.
,
Jeska
,
B.
,
Kirilina
,
E.
,
Jaeger
,
C.
,
Zhen
,
Z.
,
Cox
,
S.
,
Weiner
,
K. S.
,
Weiskopf
,
N.
, &
Grill-Spector
,
K.
(
2019
).
Apparent thinning of human visual cortex during childhood is associated with myelination
.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
,
116
(
41
),
20750
20759
. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1904931116
Ortinau
,
C.
,
Alexopoulos
,
D.
,
Dierker
,
D.
,
Van Essen
,
D.
,
Beca
,
J.
, &
Inder
,
T.
(
2013
).
Cortical folding is altered before surgery in infants with congenital heart disease
.
The Journal of Pediatrics
,
163
(
5
),
1507
1510
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2013.06.045
Ortinau
,
C. M.
,
Rollins
,
C. K.
,
Gholipour
,
A.
,
Yun
,
H. J.
,
Marshall
,
M.
,
Gagoski
,
B.
,
Afacan
,
O.
,
Friedman
,
K.
,
Tworetzky
,
W.
,
Warfield
,
S. K.
,
Newburger
,
J. W.
,
Inder
,
T. E.
,
Grant
,
P. E.
, &
Im
,
K.
(
2019
).
Early-emerging sulcal patterns are atypical in fetuses with congenital heart disease
.
Cerebral Cortex (New York, NY)
,
29
(
8
),
3605
3616
. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhy235
Østgård
,
H. F.
,
Sølsnes
,
A. E.
,
Bjuland
,
K. J.
,
Rimol
,
L. M.
,
Martinussen
,
M.
,
Brubakk
,
A.-M.
,
Håberg
,
A. K.
,
Skranes
,
J.
, &
Løhaugen
,
G. C. C.
(
2016
).
Executive function relates to surface area of frontal and temporal cortex in very-low-birth-weight late teenagers
.
Early Human Development
,
95
,
47
53
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2016.01.023
Patel
,
R.
,
Mackay
,
C. E.
,
Jansen
,
M. G.
,
Devenyi
,
G. A.
,
O’Donoghue
,
M. C.
,
Kivimäki
,
M.
,
Singh-Manoux
,
A.
,
Zsoldos
,
E.
,
Ebmeier
,
K. P.
,
Chakravarty
,
M. M.
, &
Suri
,
S.
(
2022
).
Inter- and intra-individual variation in brain structural-cognition relationships in aging
.
NeuroImage
,
257
,
119254
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.119254
Patel
,
R.
,
Steele
,
C. J.
,
Chen
,
A. G. X.
,
Patel
,
S.
,
Devenyi
,
G. A.
,
Germann
,
J.
,
Tardif
,
C. L.
, &
Chakravarty
,
M. M.
(
2020
).
Investigating microstructural variation in the human hippocampus using non-negative matrix factorization
.
NeuroImage
,
207
,
116348
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116348
Pedregosa
,
F.
,
Varoquaux
,
G.
,
Gramfort
,
A.
,
Michel
,
V.
,
Thirion
,
B.
,
Grisel
,
O.
,
Blondel
,
M.
,
Prettenhofer
,
P.
,
Weiss
,
R.
,
Dubourg
,
V.
,
Vanderplas
,
J.
,
Passos
,
A.
,
Cournapeau
,
D.
,
Brucher
,
M.
,
Perrot
,
M.
, &
Duchesnay
,
É.
(
2011
).
Scikit-learn: Machine Learning in Python
.
Journal of Machine Learning Research
,
12
(
85
),
2825
2830
. https://www.jmlr.org/papers/volume12/pedregosa11a/pedregosa11a.pdf
Perrotta
,
M. L.
,
Saha
,
P.
,
Zawadzki
,
R.
,
Beidelman
,
M.
,
Ingelsson
,
E.
,
Lui
,
G. K.
, &
Priest
,
J. R.
(
2020
).
Adults with mild‐to‐moderate congenital heart disease demonstrate measurable neurocognitive deficits
.
Journal of the American Heart Association
,
9
(
19
),
e015379
. https://doi.org/10.1161/JAHA.119.015379
R Core Team
. (
2020
).
R: A language and environment for statistical computing
(4.0.3) [Computer software]. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://www.R-project.org/
Robert
,
C.
,
Patel
,
R.
,
Blostein
,
N.
,
Steele
,
C. J.
, &
Chakravarty
,
M. M.
(
2022
).
Analyses of microstructural variation in the human striatum using non-negative matrix factorization
.
NeuroImage
,
246
,
118744
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118744
Roth
,
R. M.
, &
Gioia
,
G. A.
(
2005
).
Behavior rating inventory of executive function—Adult version
.
Psychological Assessment Resources
.
Rouel
,
M.
,
Raman
,
J.
,
Hay
,
P.
, &
Smith
,
E.
(
2016
).
Validation of the behaviour rating inventory of executive function-adult version (BRIEF-A) in the obese with and without binge eating disorder
.
Eating Behaviors
,
23
,
58
65
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2016.07.010
Sanz
,
J. H.
,
Berl
,
M. M.
,
Armour
,
A. C.
,
Wang
,
J.
,
Cheng
,
Y. I.
, &
Donofrio
,
M. T.
(
2017
).
Prevalence and pattern of executive dysfunction in school age children with congenital heart disease
.
Congenital Heart Disease
,
12
(
2
),
202
209
. https://doi.org/10.1111/chd.12427
Sasabayashi
,
D.
,
Takayanagi
,
Y.
,
Nishiyama
,
S.
,
Takahashi
,
T.
,
Furuichi
,
A.
,
Kido
,
M.
,
Nishikawa
,
Y.
,
Nakamura
,
M.
,
Noguchi
,
K.
, &
Suzuki
,
M.
(
2017
).
Increased frontal gyrification negatively correlates with executive function in patients with first-episode schizophrenia
.
Cerebral Cortex
,
27
(
4
),
2686
2694
. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhw101
Schellen
,
C.
,
Ernst
,
S.
,
Gruber
,
G. M.
,
Mlczoch
,
E.
,
Weber
,
M.
,
Brugger
,
P. C.
,
Ulm
,
B.
,
Langs
,
G.
,
Salzer-Muhar
,
U.
,
Prayer
,
D.
, &
Kasprian
,
G.
(
2015
).
Fetal MRI detects early alterations of brain development in Tetralogy of Fallot
.
American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology
,
213
(
3
),
392.e1
392.e7
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajog.2015.05.046
Schlosser
,
L.
,
Kessler
,
N.
,
Feldmann
,
M.
,
Wehrle
,
F.
,
Rometsch
,
S.
,
Greutmann
,
M.
,
Oxenius
,
A.
,
Brugger
,
P.
, &
Latal
,
B.
(
2022
).
Neurocognitive functioning in young adults with congenital heart disease: Insights from a case-control study
.
Cardiology in the Young
,
32
(
5
),
694
701
. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1047951121002705
Schroeter
,
M. L.
,
Godulla
,
J.
,
Thiel
,
F.
,
Taskin
,
B.
,
Beutner
,
F.
,
Dubovoy
,
V. K.
,
Teren
,
A.
,
Camilleri
,
J.
,
Eickhoff
,
S.
,
Villringer
,
A.
, &
Mueller
,
K.
(
2023
).
Heart failure decouples the precuneus in interaction with social cognition and executive functions
.
Scientific Reports
,
13
(
1
), Article 1. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-28338-0
Selemon
,
L. D.
(
2013
).
A role for synaptic plasticity in the adolescent development of executive function
.
Translational Psychiatry
,
3
(
3
), Article 3. https://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2013.7
Sherif
,
T.
,
Rioux
,
P.
,
Rousseau
,
M.-E.
,
Kassis
,
N.
,
Beck
,
N.
,
Adalat
,
R.
,
Das
,
S.
,
Glatard
,
T.
, &
Evans
,
A. C.
(
2014
).
CBRAIN: A web-based, distributed computing platform for collaborative neuroimaging research
.
Frontiers in Neuroinformatics
,
8
, 54. https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fninf.2014.00054
Skotting
,
M. B.
,
Eskildsen
,
S. F.
,
Ovesen
,
A. S.
,
Fonov
,
V. S.
,
Ringgaard
,
S.
,
Hjortdal
,
V. E.
, &
Lauridsen
,
M. H.
(
2021
).
Infants with congenital heart defects have reduced brain volumes
.
Scientific Reports
,
11
(
1
), Article 1. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-83690-3
Sotiras
,
A.
,
Resnick
,
S. M.
, &
Davatzikos
,
C.
(
2015
).
Finding imaging patterns of structural covariance via non-negative matrix factorization
.
NeuroImage
,
108
,
1
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.11.045
Squeglia
,
L. M.
,
Jacobus
,
J.
,
Sorg
,
S. F.
,
Jernigan
,
T. L.
, &
Tapert
,
S. F.
(
2013
).
Early adolescent cortical thinning is related to better neuropsychological performance
.
Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society: JINS
,
19
(
9
),
962
970
. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617713000878
Sridharan
,
D.
,
Levitin
,
D. J.
, &
Menon
,
V.
(
2008
).
A critical role for the right fronto-insular cortex in switching between central-executive and default-mode networks
.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
,
105
(
34
),
12569
12574
. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0800005105
Tamnes
,
C. K.
,
Østby
,
Y.
,
Walhovd
,
K. B.
,
Westlye
,
L. T.
,
Due-Tønnessen
,
P.
, &
Fjell
,
A. M.
(
2010
).
Neuroanatomical correlates of executive functions in children and adolescents: A magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) study of cortical thickness
.
Neuropsychologia
,
48
(
9
),
2496
2508
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.04.024
Toro
,
R.
,
Perron
,
M.
,
Pike
,
B.
,
Richer
,
L.
,
Veillette
,
S.
,
Pausova
,
Z.
, &
Paus
,
T.
(
2008
).
Brain size and folding of the human cerebral cortex
.
Cerebral Cortex
,
18
(
10
),
2352
2357
. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhm261
Tyagi
,
M.
,
Fteropoulli
,
T.
,
Hurt
,
C. S.
,
Hirani
,
S. P.
,
Rixon
,
L.
,
Davies
,
A.
,
Picaut
,
N.
,
Kennedy
,
F.
,
Deanfield
,
J.
,
Cullen
,
S.
, &
Newman
,
S. P.
(
2017
).
Cognitive dysfunction in adult CHD with different structural complexity
.
Cardiology in the Young
,
27
(
5
),
851
859
. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1047951116001396
Uddin
,
L. Q.
,
Nomi
,
J. S.
,
Hebert-Seropian
,
B.
,
Ghaziri
,
J.
, &
Boucher
,
O.
(
2017
).
Structure and function of the human insula
.
Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology: Official Publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society
,
34
(
4
),
300
306
. https://doi.org/10.1097/WNP.0000000000000377
von Rhein
,
M.
,
Buchmann
,
A.
,
Hagmann
,
C.
,
Huber
,
R.
,
Klaver
,
P.
,
Knirsch
,
W.
, &
Latal
,
B.
(
2014
).
Brain volumes predict neurodevelopment in adolescents after surgery for congenital heart disease
.
Brain
,
137
(
1
),
268
276
. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awt322
Watson
,
C. G.
,
Asaro
,
L. A.
,
Wypij
,
D.
,
Robertson
,
R. L.
,
Newburger
,
J. W.
, &
Rivkin
,
M. J.
(
2016
).
Altered gray matter in adolescents with D-transposition of the great arteries
.
The Journal of Pediatrics
,
169
,
36.e1
43.e1
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2015.09.084
Yeager
,
B. E.
,
Bruss
,
J.
,
Duffau
,
H.
,
Herbet
,
G.
,
Hwang
,
K.
,
Tranel
,
D.
, &
Boes
,
A. D.
(
2022
).
Central precuneus lesions are associated with impaired executive function
.
Brain Structure & Function
,
227
(
9
),
3099
3108
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-022-02556-0
Zijdenbos
,
A. P.
,
Forghani
,
R.
, &
Evans
,
A. C.
(
2002
).
Automatic “pipeline” analysis of 3-D MRI data for clinical trials: Application to multiple sclerosis
.
IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging
,
21
(
10
),
1280
1291
. https://doi.org/10.1109/TMI.2002.806283
Zimmerman
,
M. S.
,
Smith
,
A. G. C.
,
Sable
,
C. A.
,
Echko
,
M. M.
,
Wilner
,
L. B.
,
Olsen
,
H. E.
,
Atalay
,
H. T.
,
Awasthi
,
A.
,
Bhutta
,
Z. A.
,
Boucher
,
J. L.
,
Castro
,
F.
,
Cortesi
,
P. A.
,
Dubey
,
M.
,
Fischer
,
F.
,
Hamidi
,
S.
,
Hay
,
S. I.
,
Hoang
,
C. L.
,
Hugo-Hamman
,
C.
,
Jenkins
,
K. J.
, …
Kassebaum
,
N. J.
(
2020
).
Global, regional, and national burden of congenital heart disease, 1990–2017: A systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2017
.
The Lancet Child & Adolescent Health
,
4
(
3
),
185
200
. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2352-4642(19)30402-X
Ziolkowski
,
J.
(
2022
).
Investigating individual variability of morphometric covariance patterns in Autism Spectrum Disorder
.
MSc theses, McGill University
,
Canada.
https://www.proquest.com/openview/c72f14476564d2cb27087dc104532b08/1?pq-origsite=gscholar&cbl=18750&diss=y
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International (CC BY 4.0) license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. For a full description of the license, please visit https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/legalcode.

Supplementary data