Dopamine (DA) is suggested to improve perceptual and cognitive decisions by increasing the signal-to-noise ratio. Somewhat paradoxically, a hyperdopaminergia (arguably more accentuated in the right hemisphere) has also been implied in the genesis of unusual experiences such as hallucinations and paranormal thought. To test these opposing assumptions, we used two lateralized decision tasks, one with lexical (tapping left-hemisphere functions), the other with facial stimuli (tapping right-hemisphere functions). Participants were 40 healthy right-handed men, of whom 20 reported unusual, “paranormal” experiences and beliefs (“believers”), whereas the remaining participants were unexperienced and critical (“skeptics”). In a between-subject design, levodopa (200 mg) or placebo administration was balanced between belief groups (double-blind procedure). For each task and visual field, we calculated sensitivity (d′) and response tendency (criterion) derived from signal detection theory. Results showed the typical right visual field advantage for the lexical decision task and a higher d′ for verbal than facial stimuli. For the skeptics, d′ was lower in the levodopa than in the placebo group. Criterion analyses revealed that believers favored false alarms over misses, whereas skeptics displayed the opposite preference. Unexpectedly, under levodopa, these decision preferences were lower in both groups. We thus infer that levodopa (1) decreases sensitivity in perceptual–cognitive decisions, but only in skeptics, and (2) makes skeptics less and believers slightly more conservative. These results stand at odd to the common view that DA generally improves signal-to-noise ratios. Paranormal ideation seems an important personality dimension and should be assessed in investigations on the detection of signals in noise.

Dopamine (DA) is a neurotransmitter in the extrapyramidal motor system and a neuromodulator involved in motivation, emotion, and cognition (e.g., Gibbs, Naudts, Spencer, & David, 2007; Smith, Li, Becker, & Kapur, 2006; Nieoullon, 2002; Middleton & Strick, 2000; Previc, 1999). It is assumed to improve perceptual and cognitive decisions by increasing the signal-to-noise ratio (SNR), that is, to modulate neuronal activity by enhancing the ability of neurons to transmit signals and reduce distortion by noise (Seamans & Yang, 2004; Spitzer & Walter, 2003; Spitzer, 1997; Cohen & Servan-Schreiber, 1993).

Links between DA and schizophrenia were originally suggested by the observation that DA antagonistic treatment can ameliorate positive symptoms in patients with schizophrenia (Laruelle & Abi-Dargham, 1999; Davidson et al., 1987; Carlsson & Lindqvist, 1963), whereas DA agonistic treatment results in a deterioration of such symptoms (Sekine et al., 2001; Janowsky & Risch, 1979). In healthy populations, DA agonists not only boost learning (Breitenstein et al., 2006; Knecht et al., 2004) but have also the potential to trigger psychotic symptoms (Sekine et al., 2001; Abi-Dargham et al., 1998; Angrist & Gershon, 1970). Furthermore, DA overmedication can produce psychosis in patients with Parkinson's disease (Factor & Molho, 2004; Kuzuhara, 2001). In a nutshell, acute psychosis is generally considered a consequence of a hyperdopaminergic state (Howes & Kapur, 2009; Kapur, 2003; Laruelle & Abi-Dargham, 1999; Davis, Kahn, Ko, & Davidson, 1991; Carlsson & Lindqvist, 1963). In this context, a number of behavioral DA-responsive studies with unmedicated patients have specifically implicated the right (RH) rather than the left hemisphere (LH) (Mohr, Landis, Bracha, Fathi, & Brugger, 2003; Malaspina et al., 2000; Bracha, Livingston, Clothier, Linington, & Karson, 1993).

Against these clinical findings, the notion that DA would increase the SNR in neural networks appears paradoxical. Psychotic symptoms are associated with a poor discrimination between relevant and irrelevant stimuli, suggesting impaired rather than improved perceptual sensitivity. For example, positive psychotic symptoms in patients with schizophrenia are often associated with different kinds of source monitoring impairments (Anselmetti et al., 2007) such as reality-monitoring deficits in terms of a confusion between perception and imagination (Brebion, Smith, Gorman, & Amador, 1997; Frith, 1992), disrupted self–other differentiation (Lee, Kwon, Shin, Lee, & Park, 2007; Kircher & Leube, 2003; Hemsley, 1998), and the failure to distinguish between internal and external stimulation (Brebion, David, Bressan, Ohlsen, & Pilowsky, 2009; Henquet, Krabbendam, Dautzenberg, Jolles, & Merckelbach, 2005; Ford, Mathalon, Heinks, et al., 2001; Ford, Mathalon, Kalba, et al., 2001). Source monitoring deficits have also been observed in healthy individuals with high level of psychotic-like experiences (Laroi, D'Argembeau, Bredart, & van der Linden, 2007; Allen, Freeman, Johns, & McGuire, 2006). Accordingly, signal detection parameter have unequivocally shown a decreased rather than increased perceptual sensitivity in patients with auditory hallucinations (Ishigaki & Tanno, 1999). Furthermore, a lowered sensitivity in detecting a mismatch between expectancy and experience was described. This reduction, accompanied by blood flow changes in right frontal cortex, correlated with delusion severity in psychotic patients (Corlett et al., 2007).

Paranormal belief in healthy participants (e.g., the belief in extrasensory, psychokinetic and prophetic abilities) has been associated with both creative and disordered thought (Claridge, 2009; Leonhard & Brugger, 1998). For instance, individuals with paranormal belief evidence a marked willingness to perceive “patterns in noise” (Reed et al., 2008; Bell, Reddy, Halligan, Kirov, & Ellis, 2007; Brugger & Graves, 1997b) and are more inclined than those skeptical about such abilities to attribute meaning to random associations (Mohr, Landis, & Brugger, 2006; Gianotti, Mohr, Pizzagalli, Lehmann, & Brugger, 2001; Pizzagalli, Lehmann, & Brugger, 2001). Although the creative aspects of paranormal thought have been emphasized by some authors (Folley & Park, 2005; Weinstein & Graves, 2002; Gianotti et al., 2001; Claridge, Pryor, & Watkins, 1990), others have equally stressed its conceptual similarity to psychotic symptoms (Brugger & Graves, 1997b; Kreweras, 1983). Thus, a reduced criterion to acknowledge the presence of a signal not only increases the chances to creatively detect a real stimulus but also bears the risk of Type II errors, that is, hallucinatory perceptions and delusional inferences. These aspects of paranormal belief have been conceptualized by Eckblad and Chapman (1983) as an indicator of the positive phenomenology of schizotypy (i.e., a nonclinical analogue of positive psychotic symptoms).

The link between positive symptoms of psychosis and paranormal/positive schizotypal traits is further evidenced by studies showing that psychometrically assessed positive schizotypal individuals, even if completely healthy, perform similarly to patients with schizophrenia in sensory (Mohr, Rohrenbach, Laska, & Brugger, 2001; Kwapil, Chapman, Chapman, & Miller, 1996), cognitive (Tallent & Gooding, 1999; Duchene, Graves, & Brugger, 1998), and attentional tasks (Mohr, Bracha, & Brugger, 2003; Sarkin, Dionisio, Hillix, & Granholm, 1998; Brugger & Graves, 1997a). Of particular relevance to the present study, these similarities are also evident for lateralized neuropsychological functions (Brugger, 2007; Mohr, Bracha, et al., 2003; Barnett & Corballis, 2002; Pizzagalli et al., 2000; Chapman & Chapman, 1987a). Furthermore, neuropharmacological studies pointed to an enhanced dopaminergic responsivity in both persons with schizotypy (Siever et al., 1993) and schizophrenia (Davidson & Davis, 1988; Pickar et al., 1984).

We here investigated whether DA indeed modulates SNR and/or response bias (Reed et al., 2008; Brugger & Graves, 1997b) in participants with paranormal experiences and beliefs (“believers”) as compared to individuals with a critical attitude toward paranormal phenomena (“skeptics”). In order to account for a potential right hemispheric shift of dopaminergic involvement in paranormal belief, we used a double-blind levodopa/placebo design and two lateralized tachistoscopic tasks, one with a known advantage of the LH (lexical decision task [LDT]) and one with a processing advantage of the RH (facial decision task [FDT]). Using classical signal detection analysis (Gescheider, 1997; Green & Swets, 1966), we determined sensitivity (d′) and response tendencies (criterion C). Because we argued against the notion of DA increasing SNR, we expected (1) an overall reduced detection sensitivity (lower d′) in individuals in the levodopa as compared to the placebo group, especially for the believers. In addition, we predicted (2) a generally looser response criterion (lower criterion C) in individuals in the levodopa as compared to the placebo group, again, especially for the believers. Finally, we hypothesized that (3) this effect would be more prominent for RH compared to LH processing for both sensitivity and criterion measures.

Participants

Forty healthy men were recruited by a classified advertisement in a local newspaper and flyers posted in the Zurich University district. The study was introduced as an “experiment assessing the role of DA on cognitive functions.” We mentioned that blood samples would be taken, and announced an unspecified reimbursement. Critically, the recruitment text for the believers contained the phrase “You do not only consider extrasensory perception a theoretical possibility but you think you are using your own paranormal abilities in everyday situations” (without further justification of why this was a requirement). A contact telephone number and electronic mail address were included. Initially, via standardized telephone interviews (Campbell, 2000), we excluded persons with a personal or first-degree family history of neurological and psychiatric disease, including serious learning disabilities and drug abuse (casual tetrahydrocannabinol consumption was not considered an abuse if the last consumption was more than 2 months ago). Included participants were sent a brief information brochure (by regular mail) about the role of DA in the central nervous system, including potential side effects if it was taken at high doses. It also mentioned the requirement to fill in several questionnaires and stated the approximate duration of the procedure (“1 to 2 hours”) and the financial reimbursement (Swiss Francs/CHF 50). After 20 believers were selected, 20 skeptics were recruited and selected (same inclusion criterion as for believers) who responded to the same recruitment procedure, using an identical advertisement to the first except that the passage referring to belief in the paranormal now stated: “You have a skeptical attitude toward so-called paranormal phenomena and do not generally believe in the existence of extrasensory perceptions like telepathy, clairvoyance and precognition.” Additionally, these skeptics were individually matched to the believers regarding age and educational background (see Table 1). Right-handedness (self-reported at the time of the telephone interview) was later confirmed quantitatively with the 13-item scale by Chapman and Chapman (1987b). Skeptics and believers did not differ from one another in age [t(38) = 0.20, p = .84], education [t(38) = 1.57, p = .13], and the strength of right-handedness [t(38) = 1.23, p = .23].

Table 1. 

Descriptive Data of the Study Sample


Skeptics (n = 20)
Believers (n = 20)
Mean (SD)
Range
Mean (SD)
Range
Age (years) 28.4 (4.5) 21–39 28.7 (4.8) 21–38 
Education (years) 16.3 (2.6) 12–20 15.1 (2.2) 12–20 
ASGS 39.0 (25.4) 1–102 168.9 (30.4) 114–222 
MI 4.1 (2.9) 0–12 20.0 (3.79) 13–26 
Handedness 13.9 (1.2)  14.5 (2.0)  

Skeptics (n = 20)
Believers (n = 20)
Mean (SD)
Range
Mean (SD)
Range
Age (years) 28.4 (4.5) 21–39 28.7 (4.8) 21–38 
Education (years) 16.3 (2.6) 12–20 15.1 (2.2) 12–20 
ASGS 39.0 (25.4) 1–102 168.9 (30.4) 114–222 
MI 4.1 (2.9) 0–12 20.0 (3.79) 13–26 
Handedness 13.9 (1.2)  14.5 (2.0)  

All participants provided written informed consent to participate in the experiment that had been approved by the local Ethics committee.

Questionnaires

Australian Sheep–Goat Scale (ASGS)

This 18-item visual analog scale (Thalbourne & Delin, 1993) assesses both belief in paranormal phenomena (specifically telepathy, clairvoyance, precognition, and psychokinesis) and self-perceived abilities. A sample item is: “I am completely convinced that I have had at least one premonition about the future that came true and which (I believe) was not just a coincidence.” Each item is scored from 0 to 13 reflecting the number of centimeters the participant's answer mark was displaced from the end of the line. Total scores on the ASGS may thus range from 0 to 234, with higher scores indicating stronger experiences and self-perceived abilities.

Magical Ideation (MI) Scale

We assessed participants' MI with a validated true–false 30-item questionnaire that includes items such as “I sometimes have a feeling of gaining or losing energy when people look at me or touch me,” or “Some people can make me aware of them just by thinking about me.” Scores range from 0 to 30, with higher scores indicating more pronounced magical thinking. The scale is published in full in Barnett and Corballis (2002) and in Eckblad and Chapman (1983), and normative data can be found in Garety and Wessely (1994).

Lateralized Tachistoscopic Tasks

Lexical Decision Task

Stimuli were pairs of four-letter strings, one string exposed to the left visual field (LVF), the other simultaneously to the right visual field (RVF) (exposure time = 140 msec, horizontal eccentricity = 1.5° to 3.0° of visual angle; Regard, Landis, & Graves, 1985; Graves, 1983). All letters were written in black Helvetica font (18 points) and presented on a computer screen (gray background). The following LVF/RVF letter string combinations were presented in a pseudorandomized sequence: 12 word/nonword pairs, 12 nonword/word pairs, and 24 nonword/nonword pairs. Only one-syllable or two-syllable German function words were used as targets. These have low pictorial and associative properties and are thus most likely to consistently recruit LH network activation (Sabsevitz, Medler, Seidenberg, & Binder, 2005). Sample stimuli are because (“weil”), since (“seit”), or hardly (“kaum”) (see Figure 1, left).

Figure 1. 

Time course of stimulations in the lexical (A) and facial (B) decision task. After 750 msec (fixation of a central cross), a stimulus was exposed for 140 msec and participants had to press one of two lateral keys depending on the side where a meaningful word (A) or face (B) had been exposed (space bar response for two nonsense words or scrambled faces; 50% of trials). Participants were instructed to respond as quickly and accurately as possible and to fixate their gaze on a cross in the center of the screen.

Figure 1. 

Time course of stimulations in the lexical (A) and facial (B) decision task. After 750 msec (fixation of a central cross), a stimulus was exposed for 140 msec and participants had to press one of two lateral keys depending on the side where a meaningful word (A) or face (B) had been exposed (space bar response for two nonsense words or scrambled faces; 50% of trials). Participants were instructed to respond as quickly and accurately as possible and to fixate their gaze on a cross in the center of the screen.

Close modal

Facial Decision Task

Stimuli were pairs of faces and “nonfaces” (Heider & Groner, 1996). The faces were photographs (black on gray background) of men and women, whereas the “nonfaces” had scrambled eyes–nose–mouth configurations. All stimuli appeared without face outlines or hairlines (“oval faces”). One face or scrambled face was exposed to LVF, the other simultaneously to RVF (exposure time = 140 msec, horizontal eccentricity = 2.6° to 4.3° of visual angle). The following LVF/RVF face/nonface combinations were presented in a pseudorandomized sequence: 12 face/nonface pairs, 12 nonface/face pairs, and 24 nonface/nonface pairs (see Figure 1, right).

During both decision tasks (counterbalanced order), individuals were asked to rest their head on a chin rest, and to fixate their gaze on a cross in the center of the screen (see Figure 1). On seeing a meaningful word (or face, respectively) in LVF (RVF), they had to press a key with their left (right) hand. On not seeing a meaningful word (face) on either side, they had to press the space bar with both thumbs (Figure 1). Participants were instructed to respond as quickly and accurately as possible and to maintain central fixation throughout the task. Participants were tested individually, and light and contrast conditions were kept constant across participants. We assessed accuracy and reaction times (RTs) for correct responses.

Double-blind Procedure

l-Dopa Administration

The study was a randomized, double-blind between-subject levodopa/placebo design. Creation of the randomization code and substance wrapping was performed by a neurologist (H. H.). H. H. was not involved in the behavioral testing, but was available in case of medical emergencies and provided decoding information to the main investigator (P. K.) after all participants had been tested. A dual-release formulation of 200 mg levodopa/50 mg benserazide (Madopar DR; Roche Pharma AG, Reinach, Switzerland) with a fast absorption within the first hour and sustained concentration levels thereafter (Gasser, Jorga, Crevoisier, Hovens, & van Giersbergen, 1999) was administered. Each participant had been strictly instructed to refrain from any food intake between noon and the beginning of the experiment at 3:30 pm. Participants had also been told not to consume alcohol or any other drugs for at least 24 hr before testing. Compliance with these requests were 100% by self-report.

At the end of behavioral testing, participants were asked (questionnaire) which substance they thought they had received, and what they expected the substance effects to be.

l-Dopa Blood Serum Concentrations and Efficacy of Double Blinding

In order to ensure that participants were under significant levodopa concentration throughout the experiment, two blood samples (5–7 ml each) were drawn. The first was collected 30 min after drug administration, just before the two detection tasks. Immediately after behavioral testing, a second sample was drawn. Blood plasma was separated by centrifugation, and the samples were immediately frozen at −80°C for later high-pressure liquid chromatography for electrochemical detection (see Mohr, Landis, Sandor, Fathi, & Brugger, 2004 for further details). No levodopa was detected in the placebo group. In the levodopa group, the mean levodopa serum concentration was 176.95 ± 163.16 ng/ml for the first blood sample and 217.55 ± 106.28 ng/ml for the second blood sample [t(19) = −0.852, p = .405].

Data Analysis

To test for a DA influence on SNR as a function of individuals' paranormal belief, we first determined the signal detection theory parameters (1) d′ (sensitivity) and (2) the observer's response bias (criterion C) (Gescheider, 1997; Green & Swets, 1966) for each visual field (LVF, RVF) and task (LDT, FDT). Responses were scored as hits for trials on which a real stimulus (a word in the LDT and a face in the FDT) had on the side of the participant's keypress. False positives were responses to nonwords or scrambled faces, respectively. Trials on which a stimulus was presented but the response was made on the wrong side (i.e., confusion errors) were planned to be counted as misses (no such error occurred in the present dataset). d′ is a pure index of stimulus detection sensitivity uncontaminated by location of the observer's criterion. Higher d′ values indicate better stimulus detection sensitivity. Lower C values reflect a looser response tendency (“YES answer bias”), and higher C values indicate a stricter response tendency (“NO answer bias”). These two main measures were subjected to two major analyses, (1) and (2), that is, two separate four-way repeated measures ANOVAs with substance group (levodopa vs. placebo) and belief group (believers vs. skeptics) as between-subject factors and task (LDT, FDT) and visual field (LVF, RVF) as repeated within-subject factors.

In addition, to facilitate comparison of the results with those of previous studies (e.g., Mohr, Krummenacher, et al., 2005; Brugger, Gamma, Muri, Schafer, & Taylor, 1993), we also assessed (3) RTs for correct decisions, and calculated laterality indices as previously used (RT correct LVF − RT correct RVF)/(RT correct RVF + RT correct LVF) (Mohr, Krummenacher, et al., 2005; Marshall, Caplan, & Holmes, 1975). A positive value indicates an RVF/LH advantage and a negative value an LVF/RH advantage. These indices were subjected to a three-way repeated measures ANOVA with substance group (levodopa vs. placebo) and belief group (believers vs. skeptics) as between-subject factors and task (LDT, FDT) as repeated within-subject factor. In case of significant interactions, single post hoc comparisons (Newman–Keuls) were performed correcting for multiple comparisons. Homogeneity of variances in the signal detection measures was controlled using Levene's test.

Nonparametric (Spearman) correlation (rs) procedures were used throughout in order (1) to be more conservative when running a large number of correlations on small groups (n = 10) and because an interval level could not be guaranteed for the questionnaire data.

If not stated otherwise, all p values are two-tailed, and the significance level was set to α = .05.

One participant did not obey the LDT instruction (administered before the FDT in his case) and pressed solely the nontarget key in a stereotyped manner. LDT data were thus available from only 39 participants. All other comparisons are based on the originally recruited 40 participants.

Double-blind Procedure

None of the participants reported any remarkable side effects of placebo/levodopa intake. Chi-square analyses of the number of individuals with correct versus incorrect guesses in the postexperimental assessment revealed that neither skeptics (11 correct substance detections consisting of 3 hits and 8 correct rejections and 9 false substance detections consisting of 7 miss and 2 false alarms; χ2 = 0.20, p = .66) nor believers (12 correct substance detections consisting of 5 hits and 7 correct rejections and 8 false substance detections consisting of 5 miss and 3 false alarms; χ2 = 0.80, p = .37) could reliably guess what kind of treatment they had received.

Questionnaire Data

Two separate two-way ANOVAs with substance group (levodopa vs. placebo) and belief group (believers vs. skeptics) as between-subject factors, once on MI scores and once on ASGS scores, revealed significant main effects for belief group in both cases [MI: F(1, 36) = 229.85, p = .000; ASGS: F(1, 36) = 215.30, p = .000]. Believers' ASGS and MI scores were significantly larger than the respective values for the skeptics (see Table 1). As indicated by nonsignificant main effects in both ANOVAs, mean MI scores of the levodopa (11.1 ± 8.5) and placebo (13.0 ± 8.9) groups were comparable [F(1, 36) = 3.32, p = .08] as were ASGS scores [levodopa: 99.6 ± 75.5; placebo: 108.3 ± 68.7; F(1, 36) = 0.98, p = .33]. Interactions were not significant [F(1, 36) < 1.02, p > .32].

A split at the median MI scale score (12.5) into a high and low MI group revealed that all participants with ASGS scores above the median (ASGS = 108) belonged to the high MI group, and all participants with ASGS scores below the median to the low MI group. Despite only moderate item-overlap, raw scores on the MI and the ASGS inventories were positively correlated (rs = .92, p < .000).

Lateralized Stimulus Detection: (1) Sensitivity Data

The four-way ANOVA for d′ measures revealed a significant main effect for substance group [F(1, 35) = 6.13, p = .02]; d′ was lower in the levodopa (1.53 ± 0.49) than placebo (2.03 ± 0.72) group. There was also a main effect for task [F(1, 35) = 6.19, p = .02] with d′ being higher in the LDT (1.97 ± 0.12) than in the FDT (1.54 ± 0.13). Among the two-way interactions, those between visual field and task [F(1, 35) = 4.96, p = .03], and belief and substance group [F(1, 35) = 8.68, p = .006; see Figure 2A] were significant. As to the first interaction (visual field and task), post hoc comparisons revealed a higher d′ for RVF than LVF in the LDT (p = .01), and no difference in d′ between visual fields in the FDT (p = .69). Moreover, for RVF presentations, d′ was higher in the LDT than in the FDT (p = .006), with no task difference being observed for LVF presentations (p = .66). As to the second interaction (belief and substance group), post hoc comparisons showed that d′ was comparable between belief groups in the levodopa group (p = .61), but there was a higher d′ in skeptics as compared to believers in the placebo group (p = .008). Moreover, for believers, d′ was comparable in the substance groups (p = .74), but for skeptics, d′ was lower in the levodopa than in the placebo group (p = .003; see Figure 2A).

Figure 2. 

(A) d′ values of detection sensitivity (mean ± SEM). (B) Values of response criterion C (mean ± SEM). Data are averaged over tasks and hemispheres, but displayed separately for the two belief and substance groups.

Figure 2. 

(A) d′ values of detection sensitivity (mean ± SEM). (B) Values of response criterion C (mean ± SEM). Data are averaged over tasks and hemispheres, but displayed separately for the two belief and substance groups.

Close modal

The three-way interaction between paranormal belief, task, and visual field [F(1, 35) = 3.7, p = .06] just failed to reach conventional significance level. It indicated that for RVF presentations, the believers' sensitivity was lower for face than word detections (p = .042), and skeptics' sensitivity was higher for RVF compared to LVF presentations in the LDT (p < .01; see Supplemental Figure at http://neuro-psychologie.net/SF.pdf). No other main effects or interactions were significant (all post hoc tests revealed that this trend indicates F values ≤ 2.85, corresponding p values ≥ .10).

Correlations analyses between belief and sensitivity revealed that skeptics' LVF/RH d′ in the LDT was significantly positively correlated to raw scores on the ASGS (rs = .78, p < .001; Figure 3A) in both substance groups (placebo group: rs = .72, p = .02; levodopa group: rs = .86, p = .001). For neither substance group, a comparable correlation was found for RVF/LH sensitivity (rs < −.12, p > .73), nor were believers' ASGS raw scores (calculated separately for both substance groups) correlated with d′ measures in the LDT (rs < −.10, p > .78 for LVF/RH stimulations; rs > .08, p > .85 for RVF/LH stimulations). Analogous correlation analyses for the FDT revealed no significant results: rs < −.55, p > .10).

Figure 3. 

(A) Spearman correlation between skeptics' LVF/RH sensitivity for lexical decisions and paranormal belief scores for the two substance groups separately. No comparable correlation pattern was found for RVF/LH stimulations. (B) Analogous correlation as in A, but for the response criterion C.

Figure 3. 

(A) Spearman correlation between skeptics' LVF/RH sensitivity for lexical decisions and paranormal belief scores for the two substance groups separately. No comparable correlation pattern was found for RVF/LH stimulations. (B) Analogous correlation as in A, but for the response criterion C.

Close modal

Lateralized Stimulus Detection: (2) Response Criterion

The four-way ANOVA for criterion C revealed a significant main effect for belief group [F(1, 35) = 11.12, p = .002], indicating that believers' criterion was lower than skeptics' (see Figure 2B). The two-way interaction between belief group and substance group was as predicted, but significant only at the one-tailed level [F(1, 35) = 2,98, p < .05]. In more detail, believers' criterion was lower than the one of skeptics in the placebo group (p = .006), but comparable between belief groups in the levodopa group (p > .10). Moreover, skeptics' (p = .38) and believers' (p = .13) criterions were comparable in the levodopa and placebo groups. No other main effects or interactions were significant (all F values ≤ 2.28, corresponding p values ≥ .14).

Correlation between the C values and raw scores on the ASGS revealed a negative association between these parameters for skeptics in the placebo group and exclusively with respect to LVF/RH stimulations in the LDT (rs = −.68, p = .029; see Figure 3B). For believers in the placebo group, ASGS raw scores were significantly positively correlated to C values regarding RVF/LH stimulations in the FDT (rs = .63, p = .049).

Lateralized Stimulus Detection: (3) Reaction Time Analyses

The three-way ANOVA on RT indices revealed no significant main effects or interactions [F(1, 35) < 0.87, p > .36].

Correlation analyses in the two belief groups between the RT index score in the two decision tasks and participants' raw scores on the ASGS revealed that increasing ASGS scores were negatively correlated to the RT laterality index in skeptics in the levodopa group in both the FDT (rs = −.69, p = .026) and marginally significantly the LDT task (rs = −.62, p = .056), again indicating an association between paranormal belief and attenuated LH superiority.

Healthy participants preselected for their attitudes toward paranormal phenomena were administered two lateralized decision tasks in a double-blind, levodopa, placebo-controlled between-subject design. We aimed to investigate relationships between increased DA availability, the personality trait paranormal belief (notably based on self-perceived own paranormal abilities) and hemisphere-specific signal detection performance. “Signal” was defined here as a meaningful constellation of verbal or facial elements against a “noisy,” meaningless configuration of the same elements. We set out to test two main hypotheses concerning the influence of DA on perceptual decisions, that is, decreased sensitivity (lower SNR) and reduced response criterion. In addition, we hypothesized an accentuation of these predicted effects in the believer group and prominently so for RH processing.

On a methodological note, we emphasize that a genuine double-blind procedure could be realized in the present study; neither believers nor skeptics could reliably guess the type of substance they did ingest nor were there any conspicuous behavioral signs that would have informed the experimenter about the type of pharmacological treatment a particular participant had received.

Perceptual Sensitivity: d

In line with our hypothesis, participants having received levodopa—irrespective of paranormal belief, task, and visual field/hemispheres—had generally lower sensitivities than participants having received placebo. These results indicate that the notion of a general enhancement of the SNR by DA (Seamans & Yang, 2004; Spitzer, 1997; Cohen & Servan-Schreiber, 1993) may have to be revised. At least in our two classical decision tasks, a decrease rather than an increase in sensitivity was apparent. We interpret this reduced perceptual sensitivity caused by levodopa as a consequence of an increase in “internal noise” (Kapur, 2003) that may have interfered with the sensory input.

However, the pharmacological treatment per se was not related to overall decision performance. The lower sensitivity in the levodopa than placebo group was dependent on participants' paranormal belief. Links between the emergence of paranormal belief and the DA system have been suggested (Brugger & Graves, 1997a) and tested (Mohr et al., 2004; Kumari et al., 1999). Here, contrary to our expectation, a reduced sensitivity in the levodopa as compared to the placebo group was only evident in the skeptics. Previous findings (Mohr, Krummenacher, et al., 2005) would have suggested a modulating effect of DA for both believers and skeptics and an improvement of specifically RVF/LH decisions. This could not be replicated in the present study. One reason for this discrepancy may be differences in the study populations. In the previous study, participants had been recruited randomly from the general population and participants with pronounced paranormal beliefs were excluded. Also, Mohr, Krummenacher et al. (2005) reported only hit rates, whereas the SDT analyses in the present study considered both hits and false alarms. Alternatively, one may speculate that the lack of an effect in the group of believers reflected a plateau effect caused by high cerebral baseline DA levels arguably responsible for low d′ values. A single dose of levodopa would then not further have lowered d′ values because of a possibly saturated DA receptor activity.

These results further decrease the plausibility that DA generally improves SNR, and instead propose the opposite, in particular, in individuals with a presumably hypodopaminergic system (i.e., the skeptics). A shift in low MI participants' neuropsychological profile toward the psychotic range has repeatedly been shown subsequent to DA agonistic consumption (Mohr et al., 2006; Mohr, Landis, Bracha, Fathi, & Brugger, 2005; Mohr et al., 2004) and seems to be a consistent pattern. Therefore, the present finding is more in accordance with the hypothesis that enhanced dopaminergic transmission produces less constrained firing patterns in mesolimbic neurons. This would generate more internal noise and promote, on the phenomenal level, loosened associations and superstitious beliefs (Shaner, 1999).

Response Criterion C

Irrespective of substance, task, and visual field/hemispheres, believers had a lower response criterion than skeptics. This means that they evidenced a tendency to say “yes” rather than “no,” or in more general terms, favored a Type I error over a Type II error strategy. Such an inclination is in accordance with previous research with healthy persons endorsing paranormal beliefs (Reed et al., 2008; Tsakanikos & Reed, 2005; Wiseman, Greening, & Smith, 2003; Weinstein & Graves, 2002; Brugger & Graves, 1997b). Although a reduced criterion may be adaptive in detecting real associations and develop novel links between concepts (Folley & Park, 2005; Folley, Doop, & Park, 2003; Weinstein & Graves, 2001, 2002; Gianotti et al., 2001; Claridge et al., 1990), it simultaneously bears the risk of false causal attributions and lowers the threshold for hallucinatory perceptual experiences (Bell et al., 2007; Brugger & Graves, 1997b).

This general belief effect interacted with DA administration. More specifically, the modulation of response criterion by paranormal belief was only significant for participants in the placebo group. This is in contrast to the sensitivity findings and indicates a balancing effect of levodopa; believers' criterion turned more conservative and skeptics' slightly looser. This interaction effect further supports the proposed links between a participant's baseline dopaminergic activity, and the emergence and maintenance of paranormal ideation. It is also compatible with the finding that, in believers, compensation from higher than normal levels of dopaminergic activity may contribute to psychosis protection (Mohr et al., 2004).

There is evidence both from studies with animals (Arnsten, 1997; Arnsten, Cai, Steere, & Goldman-Rakic, 1995; Williams & Goldman-Rakic, 1995; Arnsten, Cai, Murphy, & Goldman-Rakic, 1994) and humans (Mehta et al., 2000) that with respect to a specific task or task demands (e.g., Floresco & Magyar, 2006) dopaminergic actions might follow the trajectory of an inverted U-shape function, with an improvement of cognitive performance from low to medium doses, but a decline from medium to high doses (Goto, Otani, & Grace, 2007). Applying this idea to the present results, it could be speculated that believers already have a high baseline DA activity, which, when further stimulated by an additional levodopa supplementation, results in a reversal of performance.

Although these conjectures may be promising, caution has to be expressed with regard to the possibly weakest point of the present study, that is, use of a between-subject design. Not only did we have to deal with two different baselines in behavioral performance but also with potentially different baselines in DA responsivity (see Mehta (2002) for the importance of initial physiological states or baseline values). No doubt that future research in DA effects on signal detection and their modulation by paranormal belief must employ a within-subject design, even at the cost of running into problems of learning (Gescheider, 1997) and confounding effects of motivational demands. In order to trace participants at a point of the above-mentioned hypothesized inverted U-curve, in future within-subject experiments, baseline assessment (e.g., Muller, von Cramon, & Pollmann, 1998) should not only include cognitive measures but also direct serum or spinal DA metabolic markers such as homovanillin acid (HVA).

Laterality Aspects of Perceptual Sensitivity and Response Criterion

The visual field main effects in the two lateralized tasks confirm the RVF/LH superiority for lexical decisions (Hugdahl, 2000; Regard et al., 1985; Graves, 1983) and a similar (although not statistically significant) superiority of the RH for face detections (Heider & Groner, 1996). This is in line with previous reports showing that RH dominance for face recognition is less pronounced than LH dominance for lexical decisions (e.g., Leehey & Cahn, 1979). This may be due to verbal strategies used by some participants for face recognition leading to activation of LH processes.

With respect to hemispheric processing of verbal material, we found a strikingly different pattern of results for the two belief groups. Interestingly, no lexical decision laterality effects emerged for those participants who claimed to have paranormal experiences and abilities in everyday life. In contrast, in the skeptics, paranormal belief was highly positively correlated in both substance groups to detection sensitivity for words presented to LVF. In other words, as long as paranormal belief remains within a modest range, RH linguistic capacity increases with increasing belief. This is exactly what has been described in numerous previous experiments (e.g., Weinstein & Graves, 2001; Leonhard & Brugger, 1998; Brugger, Regard, et al., 1993) including one recent study that employed an identical LDT paradigm (but no facial decisions, no signal detection analysis, and no preselection with respect to belief groups; Mohr, Krummenacher, et al., 2005).

We have previously argued (Brugger & Mohr, 2008) that the association between paranormal belief and RH linguistic competence may be crucial for an understanding of any laterality aspects of psychotic symptoms. Similar to the situation in acute schizophrenia (Crow, 1997), there seems to be a breakdown of the regular LH dominance for language in extreme paranormal believers whose belief is notably based on apparently paranormal experiences. Up to a certain degree, RH participation in verbal tasks may foster creative thinking, but an overreliance on the less focused associative processing characteristics of the RH may bring about undue claims about perceptual and inferential thoughts. We note that not only skeptics' lexical decision sensitivity in the LVF/RH was correlated to paranormal belief but also their tendency to favor “yes” over “no” responses. The shift in the response criterion was, however, confined to the placebo group. Thus, again within the moderate range of paranormal ideation typical for unselected participants, the higher the belief, the more liberal the response criterion based on verbal information displayed in LVF.

Divided visual field performance did not interact substantially with levodopa administration. This may cast doubt on theories that have inconsistently and even antithetically proposed hemispheric asymmetries in the responsivity to dopaminergic treatment. (e.g., Bracha et al., 1993; Bracha, 1989 for RH > LH, but see Reynolds, 1983 for opposite predictions based on a postmortem investigation). Previous inconsistencies in this admittedly controversial clinical literature may have been due to (1) differences in illness duration (acute vs. chronic), (2) medication effects, and/or (3) methodological differences (e.g., behavioral paradigms vs. receptor density determinations).

The influence of DA may be more marked in the domain of verbal learning (e.g., Breitenstein et al., 2006), and our simple detection tasks may not have been ideal in uncovering differential hemispheric responses to a single dose of levodopa. Rather, indirect evidence for a modulation of laterality patterns by l-dopa comes from the observation that most of the correlations between hemifield performances and paranormal belief were only significant in the placebo group. An exception is the association of belief and the laterality index based on RTs of correct (lexical and facial) decisions (in disfavor of an LH superiority). This was exclusively present for participants (notably skeptics) in the levodopa group. We tentatively conclude that the effects of DA on lateralized performance, if present at all, may target consolidation and motor output processes, rather than the low-level perceptual side of signal detection.

Further Issues

The personality-dependent pharmacological effects described in the present report highlight the need of taking individual personality and capacity differences into account (Mehta, 2002). When ignored, these could obscure meaningful correlations and blur subgroup differences in task performance (White, Lott, & de Wit, 2006; Mohr, Krummenacher, et al., 2005; Mohr, Landis, et al., 2005; Kosslyn et al., 2002; Mattay et al., 2000; Mehta et al., 2000; Kimberg, D'Esposito, & Farah, 1997; Fleming, Bigelow, Weinberger, & Goldberg, 1995). Furthermore, as Hassler and Thadewald (2003) pointed out, pooling data from heterogeneous samples bears the risk to produce misleading results due to “mean value artifacts” (see also the recent illustration by Hadzi-Pavlovic (2007). One source of individual variability in DA responsiveness may be genetically determined (Goldberg et al., 2003; Egan et al., 2001; Weinberger et al., 2001), and potential interactions between such genetic predispositions and an individual's attitude toward the paranormal are currently explored (Raz, Hines, Fossella, & Castro, 2008).

As to the modulation of performance by positive schizotypal personality, we may mention previous findings communicated in the psychiatric literature. It was argued that, contrasted to patients with schizophrenia, individuals with schizotypal personality disorder might have an attenuated risk for psychosis and functional impairments because of a more efficient fronto-cortical buffering of subcortical dopaminergic tone (Koenigsberg et al., 2001; Kirrane et al., 2000; for a similar view with respect to healthy participants' MI and paranormal belief, see Mohr et al., 2004).

General Implications

The main findings of the present study could have implications for the development of novel pharmacotherapeutic treatment strategies for cognitive neuropsychiatric dysfunction. As Floresco and Magyar (2006) pointed out, dopaminergic agents must take into account both the specific types of function that are impaired in patients and the beneficial or deleterious effects that these drugs may have on different cognitive functions.

Further research on the specific DA receptor mechanism that contribute to different types of neuropsychological functions is thus likely to facilitate the development of more selective and effective treatments for specific domains of cognitive dysfunction. To further advance this line of research, it would be challenging to focus more on the specificity of dopaminergic activity by analyzing the effects of (1) dose–response associations, (2) repeated dose administration, and (3) DA antagonists. (4) Monitoring receptor activity in a within-subject design with ligand-PET methodologies may likewise be indispensable.

Apart from their clinical relevance, our findings may have broadest implications for general cognitive psychology and applied psychology. Over the past decade, evidence has been accumulated that paranormal belief is an important personality dimension that should never be left unassessed in the framework of tasks that investigate the detection of signals in noise and the subsequent processes of deductive reasoning. Paranormal ideation may profoundly modulate recognition memory, pain evaluation, social behavior (e.g., reward sensitivity), risk assessments, and general decision making.

We thank Marianne Wackermann for blood serum preparation, Lotti Batschelet and the nurses on floor HAL C of the Department of Neurology for the collection of, and Marc Fathi (Geneva) for the analysis of, the blood samples. This research was funded by a grant from the “Institut für Grenzgebiete der Psychologie und Psychohygiene,” Freiburg i. Br., Germany (#690610) to P. B. and C. M. Preparation of this report was further supported by a grant from the OPO Stiftung to the first author. Parts of this work were presented at the FENS 2002 (Abstr. vol 1, A074.14).

Reprint requests should be sent to Peter Krummenacher, Collegium Helveticum, University of Zurich and ETH Zurich, Schmelzbergstrasse 25, 8092 Zurich, Switzerland, or via e-mail: [email protected].

Abi-Dargham
,
A.
,
Gil
,
R.
,
Krystal
,
J.
,
Baldwin
,
R. M.
,
Seibyl
,
J. P.
,
Bowers
,
M.
,
et al
(
1998
).
Increased striatal dopamine transmission in schizophrenia: Confirmation in a second cohort.
American Journal of Psychiatry
,
155
,
761
767
.
Allen
,
P.
,
Freeman
,
D.
,
Johns
,
L.
, &
McGuire
,
P.
(
2006
).
Misattribution of self-generated speech in relation to hallucinatory proneness and delusional ideation in healthy volunteers.
Schizophrenia
, Research,
84
,
281
288
.
Angrist
,
B. M.
, &
Gershon
,
S.
(
1970
).
The phenomenology of experimentally induced amphetamine psychosis: Preliminary observations.
Biological Psychiatry
,
2
,
95
107
.
Anselmetti
,
S.
,
Cavallaro
,
R.
,
Bechi
,
M.
,
Angelone
,
S. M.
,
Ermoli
,
E.
,
Cocchi
,
F.
,
et al
(
2007
).
Psychopathological and neuropsychological correlates of source monitoring impairment in schizophrenia.
Psychiatry Research
,
150
,
51
59
.
Arnsten
,
A. F.
(
1997
).
Catecholamine regulation of the prefrontal cortex.
Journal of Psychopharmacology (Oxford, England)
,
11
,
151
162
.
Arnsten
,
A. F.
,
Cai
,
J. X.
,
Murphy
,
B. L.
, &
Goldman-Rakic
,
P. S.
(
1994
).
Dopamine D1 receptor mechanisms in the cognitive performance of young adult and aged monkeys.
Psychopharmacology (Berlin)
,
116
,
143
151
.
Arnsten
,
A. F.
,
Cai
,
J. X.
,
Steere
,
J. C.
, &
Goldman-Rakic
,
P. S.
(
1995
).
Dopamine D2 receptor mechanisms contribute to age-related cognitive decline: The effects of quinpirole on memory and motor performance in monkeys.
Journal of Neuroscience
,
15
,
3429
3439
.
Barnett
,
K. J.
, &
Corballis
,
M. C.
(
2002
).
Ambidexterity and magical ideation.
Laterality
,
7
,
75
84
.
Bell
,
V.
,
Reddy
,
V.
,
Halligan
,
P.
,
Kirov
,
G.
, &
Ellis
,
H.
(
2007
).
Relative suppression of magical thinking: A transcranial magnetic stimulation study.
Cortex
,
43
,
551
557
.
Bracha
,
H. S.
(
1989
).
Is there a right hemi-hyper-dopaminergic psychosis?
Schizophrenia Research
,
2
,
317
324
.
Bracha
,
H. S.
,
Livingston
,
R. L.
,
Clothier
,
J.
,
Linington
,
B. B.
, &
Karson
,
C. N.
(
1993
).
Correlation of severity of psychiatric patients' delusions with right hemispatial inattention (left-turning behavior).
American Journal of Psychiatry
,
150
,
330
332
.
Brebion
,
G.
,
David
,
A. S.
,
Bressan
,
R. A.
,
Ohlsen
,
R. I.
, &
Pilowsky
,
L. S.
(
2009
).
Hallucinations and two types of free-recall intrusion in schizophrenia.
Psychological Medicine
,
39
,
917
926
.
Brebion
,
G.
,
Smith
,
M. J.
,
Gorman
,
J. M.
, &
Amador
,
X.
(
1997
).
Discrimination accuracy and decision biases in different types of reality monitoring in schizophrenia.
Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease
,
185
,
247
253
.
Breitenstein
,
C.
,
Korsukewitz
,
C.
,
Floel
,
A.
,
Kretzschmar
,
T.
,
Diederich
,
K.
, &
Knecht
,
S.
(
2006
).
Tonic dopaminergic stimulation impairs associative learning in healthy subjects.
Neuropsychopharmacology
,
31
,
2552
2564
.
Brugger
,
P.
(
2007
).
Tracking a finer madness.
Scientific American Mind
,
10
,
77
79
.
Brugger
,
P.
,
Gamma
,
A.
,
Muri
,
R.
,
Schafer
,
M.
, &
Taylor
,
K. I.
(
1993
).
Functional hemispheric asymmetry and belief in ESP: Towards a “neuropsychology of belief”.
Perceptual and Motor Skills
,
77
,
1299
1308
.
Brugger
,
P.
, &
Graves
,
R. E.
(
1997a
).
Right hemispatial inattention and magical ideation.
European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience
,
247
,
55
57
.
Brugger
,
P.
, &
Graves
,
R. E.
(
1997b
).
Testing vs. believing hypotheses: Magical ideation in the judgement of contingencies.
Cognitive Neuropsychiatry
,
2
,
251
272
.
Brugger
,
P.
, &
Mohr
,
C.
(
2008
).
The paranormal mind: How the study of anomalous experiences and beliefs may inform cognitive neuroscience.
Cortex
,
44
,
1291
1298
.
Brugger
,
P.
,
Regard
,
M.
,
Landis
,
T.
,
Cook
,
N.
,
Krebs
,
D.
, &
Niederberger
,
J.
(
1993
).
“Meaningful” patterns in visual noise: Effects of lateral stimulation and the observer's belief in ESP.
Psychopathology
,
26
,
261
265
.
Campbell
,
J. J.
(
2000
).
Neuropsychiatric assessment.
In C. Coffey & E. Cummings (Eds.),
Textbook of geriatric neuropsychiatry
(2nd ed., pp.
109
124
).
Washington, DC
:
American Psychiatric Press
.
Carlsson
,
A.
, &
Lindqvist
,
M.
(
1963
).
Effect of chlorpromazine or haloperidol on formation of 3methoxytyramine and normetanephrine in mouse brain.
Acta Pharmacologica et Toxicologica (Copenhagen)
,
20
,
140
144
.
Chapman
,
J. P.
, &
Chapman
,
L. J.
(
1987a
).
Handedness of hypothetically psychosis-prone subjects.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology
,
96
,
89
93
.
Chapman
,
L. J.
, &
Chapman
,
J. P.
(
1987b
).
The measurement of handedness.
Brain and Cognition
,
6
,
175
183
.
(
2009
).
Personality, psychopathology, and original minds.
Personality and Individual Differences
,
46
,
749
754
.
Claridge
,
G.
,
Pryor
,
R.
, &
Watkins
,
G.
(
1990
).
Sounds from the bell jar: Ten psychotic authors.
New York
:
St. Martin's Press
.
Cohen
,
J. D.
, &
Servan-Schreiber
,
D.
(
1993
).
A theory of dopamine function and its role in cognitive deficits in schizophrenia.
Schizophrenia Bulletin
,
19
,
85
104
.
Corlett
,
P. R.
,
Murray
,
G. K.
,
Honey
,
G. D.
,
Aitken
,
M. R.
,
Shanks
,
D. R.
,
Robbins
,
T. W.
,
et al
(
2007
).
Disrupted prediction-error signal in psychosis: Evidence for an associative account of delusions.
Brain
,
130
,
2387
2400
.
Crow
,
T. J.
(
1997
).
Schizophrenia as failure of hemispheric dominance for language.
Trends in Neurosciences
,
20
,
339
343
.
Davidson
,
M.
, &
Davis
,
K. L.
(
1988
).
A comparison of plasma homovanillic acid concentrations in schizophrenic patients and normal controls.
Archives of General Psychiatry
,
45
,
561
563
.
Davidson
,
M.
,
Keefe
,
R. S.
,
Mohs
,
R. C.
,
Siever
,
L. J.
,
Losonczy
,
M. F.
,
Horvath
,
T. B.
,
et al
(
1987
).
l-Dopa challenge and relapse in schizophrenia.
American Journal of Psychiatry
,
144
,
934
938
.
Davis
,
K. L.
,
Kahn
,
R. S.
,
Ko
,
G.
, &
Davidson
,
M.
(
1991
).
Dopamine in schizophrenia: A review and reconceptualization.
American Journal of Psychiatry
,
148
,
1474
1486
.
Duchene
,
A.
,
Graves
,
R. E.
, &
Brugger
,
P.
(
1998
).
Schizotypal thinking and associative processing: A response commonality analysis of verbal fluency.
Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience
,
23
,
56
60
.
Eckblad
,
M.
, &
Chapman
,
L. J.
(
1983
).
Magical ideation as an indicator of schizotypy.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology
,
51
,
215
225
.
Egan
,
M. F.
,
Goldberg
,
T. E.
,
Kolachana
,
B. S.
,
Callicott
,
J. H.
,
Mazzanti
,
C. M.
,
Straub
,
R. E.
,
et al
(
2001
).
Effect of COMT Val108/158 Met genotype on frontal lobe function and risk for schizophrenia.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A.
,
98
,
6917
6922
.
Factor
,
S. A.
, &
Molho
,
E. S.
(
2004
).
Threatening auditory hallucinations and Cotard syndrome in Parkinson disease.
Clinical Neuropharmacology
,
27
,
205
207
.
Fleming
,
K.
,
Bigelow
,
L. B.
,
Weinberger
,
D. R.
, &
Goldberg
,
T. E.
(
1995
).
Neuropsychological effects of amphetamine may correlate with personality characteristics.
Psychopharmacology Bulletin
,
31
,
357
362
.
Floresco
,
S. B.
, &
Magyar
,
O.
(
2006
).
Mesocortical dopamine modulation of executive functions: Beyond working memory.
Psychopharmacology (Berlin)
,
188
,
567
585
.
Folley
,
B. S.
,
Doop
,
M. L.
, &
Park
,
S.
(
2003
).
Psychoses and creativity: Is the missing link a biological mechanism related to phospholipids turnover?
Prostaglandins, Leukotrienes and Essential Fatty Acids
,
69
,
467
476
.
Folley
,
B. S.
, &
Park
,
S.
(
2005
).
Verbal creativity and schizotypal personality in relation to prefrontal hemispheric laterality: A behavioral and near-infrared optical imaging study.
Schizophrenia
, Research,
80
,
271
282
.
Ford
,
J. M.
,
Mathalon
,
D. H.
,
Heinks
,
T.
,
Kalba
,
S.
,
Faustman
,
W. O.
, &
Roth
,
W. T.
(
2001
).
Neurophysiological evidence of corollary discharge dysfunction in schizophrenia.
American Journal of Psychiatry
,
158
,
2069
2071
.
Ford
,
J. M.
,
Mathalon
,
D. H.
,
Kalba
,
S.
,
Whitfield
,
S.
,
Faustman
,
W. O.
, &
Roth
,
W. T.
(
2001
).
Cortical responsiveness during talking and listening in schizophrenia: An event-related brain potential study.
Biological Psychiatry
,
50
,
540
549
.
Frith
,
C. D.
(
1992
).
The cognitive neuropsychology of schizophrenia.
Hove, UK
:
Erlbaum
.
Garety
,
P.
, &
Wessely
,
S.
(
1994
).
The assessment of positive symptoms.
In T. R. E. Barnes & H. E. Nelson (Eds.),
The assessment of psychoses. A practical handbook
(pp.
21
39
).
London
:
Chapman & Hall
.
Gasser
,
U. E.
,
Jorga
,
K.
,
Crevoisier
,
C.
,
Hovens
,
S. E.
, &
van Giersbergen
,
P. L.
(
1999
).
COMT inhibition by tolcapone further improves levodopa pharmacokinetics when combined with a dual-release formulation of levodopa/benserazide. A novel principle in the treatment of Parkinson's disease.
European Neurology
,
41
,
206
211
.
Gescheider
,
G. A.
(
1997
).
Psychophysics the fundamentals
(3rd ed.).
Mahwah, NJ
:
Erlbaum
.
Gianotti
,
L. R.
,
Mohr
,
C.
,
Pizzagalli
,
D.
,
Lehmann
,
D.
, &
Brugger
,
P.
(
2001
).
Associative processing and paranormal belief.
Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
,
55
,
595
603
.
Gibbs
,
A. A.
,
Naudts
,
K. H.
,
Spencer
,
E. P.
, &
David
,
A. S.
(
2007
).
The role of dopamine in attentional and memory biases for emotional information.
American Journal of Psychiatry
,
164
,
1603
1609; quiz: 1624
.
Goldberg
,
T. E.
,
Egan
,
M. F.
,
Gscheidle
,
T.
,
Coppola
,
R.
,
Weickert
,
T.
,
Kolachana
,
B. S.
,
et al
(
2003
).
Executive subprocesses in working memory: Relationship to catechol-O-methyltransferase Val158Met genotype and schizophrenia.
Archives of General Psychiatry
,
60
,
889
896
.
Goto
,
Y.
,
Otani
,
S.
, &
Grace
,
A. A.
(
2007
).
The Yin and Yang of dopamine release: A new perspective.
Neuropharmacology
,
53
,
583
587
.
Graves
,
R.
(
1983
).
Mouth asymmetry, dichotic ear advantage and tachistoscopic visual field advantage as measures of language lateralization.
Neuropsychologia
,
21
,
641
649
.
Green
,
D. E.
, &
Swets
,
J. A.
(
1966
).
Signal detection theory and psychophysics.
New York
:
Wiley
.
Hadzi-Pavlovic
,
D.
(
2007
).
Statistically speaking: Correlations III: Heterogeneous data.
Acta Neuropsychiatrica
,
19
,
215
216
.
Hassler
,
U.
, &
Thadewald
,
T.
(
2003
).
Nonsensical and biased correlation due to pooling heterogeneous samples.
Statistician
,
52
,
367
379
.
Heider
,
B.
, &
Groner
,
R.
(
1996
).
Visual field asymmetries in schematic persistence.
Laterality
,
1
,
281
297
.
Hemsley
,
D. R.
(
1998
).
The disruption of the “sense of self” in schizophrenia: Potential links with disturbances of information processing.
British Journal of Medical Psychology
,
71
,
115
124
.
Henquet
,
C.
,
Krabbendam
,
L.
,
Dautzenberg
,
J.
,
Jolles
,
J.
, &
Merckelbach
,
H.
(
2005
).
Confusing thoughts and speech: Source monitoring and psychosis.
Psychiatry Research
,
133
,
57
63
.
Howes
,
O. D.
, &
Kapur
,
S.
(
2009
).
The dopamine hypothesis of schizophrenia: Version III—the final common pathway.
Schizophrenia Bulletin
,
35
,
549
562
.
Hugdahl
,
K.
(
2000
).
Lateralization of cognitive processes in the brain.
Acta Psychologica (Amsterdam)
,
105
,
211
235
.
Ishigaki
,
T.
, &
Tanno
,
Y.
(
1999
).
The signal detection ability of patients with auditory hallucination: Analysis using the continuous performance test.
Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
,
53
,
471
476
.
Janowsky
,
D. S.
, &
Risch
,
C.
(
1979
).
Amphetamine psychosis and psychotic symptoms.
Psychopharmacology (Berlin)
,
65
,
73
77
.
Kapur
,
S.
(
2003
).
Psychosis as a state of aberrant salience: A framework linking biology, phenomenology, and pharmacology in schizophrenia.
American Journal of Psychiatry
,
160
,
13
23
.
Kimberg
,
D. Y.
,
D'Esposito
,
M.
, &
Farah
,
M. J.
(
1997
).
Effects of bromocriptine on human subjects depend on working memory capacity.
NeuroReport
,
8
,
3581
3585
.
Kircher
,
T. T.
, &
Leube
,
D. T.
(
2003
).
Self-consciousness, self-agency, and schizophrenia.
Consciousness and Cognition
,
12
,
656
669
.
Kirrane
,
R. M.
,
Mitropoulou
,
V.
,
Nunn
,
M.
,
New
,
A. S.
,
Harvey
,
P. D.
,
Schopick
,
F.
,
et al
(
2000
).
Effects of amphetamine on visuospatial working memory performance in schizophrenia spectrum personality disorder.
Neuropsychopharmacology
,
22
,
14
18
.
Knecht
,
S.
,
Breitenstein
,
C.
,
Bushuven
,
S.
,
Wailke
,
S.
,
Kamping
,
S.
,
Floel
,
A.
,
et al
(
2004
).
Levodopa: Faster and better word learning in normal humans.
Annals of Neurology
,
56
,
20
26
.
Koenigsberg
,
H. W.
,
Harvey
,
P. D.
,
Mitropoulou
,
V.
,
New
,
A. S.
,
Goodman
,
M.
,
Silverman
,
J.
,
et al
(
2001
).
Are the interpersonal and identity disturbances in the borderline personality disorder criteria linked to the traits of affective instability and impulsivity?
Journal of Personality Disorders
,
15
,
358
370
.
Kosslyn
,
S. M.
,
Cacioppo
,
J. T.
,
Davidson
,
R. J.
,
Hugdahl
,
K.
,
Lovallo
,
W. R.
,
Spiegel
,
D.
,
et al
(
2002
).
Bridging psychology and biology. The analysis of individuals in groups.
American Psychologist
,
57
,
341
351
.
Kreweras
,
G.
(
1983
).
Approche bayesienne des phénomènes «paranormaux».
Mathématiques et Sciences Humaines
,
21
,
59
66
.
Kumari
,
V.
,
Cotter
,
P. A.
,
Mulligan
,
O. F.
,
Checkley
,
S. A.
,
Gray
,
N. S.
,
Hemsley
,
D. R.
,
et al
(
1999
).
Effects of d-amphetamine and haloperidol on latent inhibition in healthy male volunteers.
Journal of Psychopharmacology (Oxford, England)
,
13
,
398
405
.
Kuzuhara
,
S.
(
2001
).
Drug-induced psychotic symptoms in Parkinson's disease. Problems, management and dilemma.
Journal of Neurology
,
248(Suppl. 3)
,
III28
III31
.
Kwapil
,
T. R.
,
Chapman
,
J. P.
,
Chapman
,
L. J.
, &
Miller
,
M. B.
(
1996
).
Deviant olfactory experiences as indicators of risk for psychosis.
Schizophrenia Bulletin
,
22
,
371
382
.
Laroi
,
F.
,
D'Argembeau
,
A.
,
Bredart
,
S.
, &
van der Linden
,
M.
(
2007
).
Face recognition failures in schizotypy.
Cognitive Neuropsychiatry
,
12
,
554
571
.
Laruelle
,
M.
, &
Abi-Dargham
,
A.
(
1999
).
Dopamine as the wind of the psychotic fire: New evidence from brain imaging studies.
Journal of Psychopharmacology (Oxford, England)
,
13
,
358
371
.
Lee
,
J.
,
Kwon
,
J. S.
,
Shin
,
Y. W.
,
Lee
,
K. J.
, &
Park
,
S.
(
2007
).
Visual self-recognition in patients with schizophrenia.
Schizophrenia Research
,
94
,
215
220
.
Leehey
,
S. C.
, &
Cahn
,
A.
(
1979
).
Lateral asymmetries in the recognition of words, familiar faces and unfamiliar faces.
Neuropsychologia
,
17
,
619
628
.
Leonhard
,
D.
, &
Brugger
,
P.
(
1998
).
Creative, paranormal, and delusional thought: A consequence of right hemisphere semantic activation?
Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and Behavioral Neurology
,
11
,
177
183
.
Malaspina
,
D.
,
Bruder
,
G.
,
Furman
,
V.
,
Gorman
,
J. M.
,
Berman
,
A.
, &
Van Heertum
,
R.
(
2000
).
Schizophrenia subgroups differing in dichotic listening laterality also differ in neurometabolism and symptomatology.
Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
,
12
,
485
492
.
Marshall
,
J. C.
,
Caplan
,
D.
, &
Holmes
,
J. M.
(
1975
).
The measure of laterality.
Neuropsychologia
,
13
,
315
321
.
Mattay
,
V. S.
,
Callicott
,
J. H.
,
Bertolino
,
A.
,
Heaton
,
I.
,
Frank
,
J. A.
,
Coppola
,
R.
,
et al
(
2000
).
Effects of dextroamphetamine on cognitive performance and cortical activation.
Neuroimage
,
12
,
268
275
.
Mehta
,
M. A.
(
2002
).
Where do we go from here? The importance of initial values.
Neuropsychopharmacology
,
27
,
879
880; author reply: 881
.
Mehta
,
M. A.
,
Owen
,
A. M.
,
Sahakian
,
B. J.
,
Mavaddat
,
N.
,
Pickard
,
J. D.
, &
Robbins
,
T. W.
(
2000
).
Methylphenidate enhances working memory by modulating discrete frontal and parietal lobe regions in the human brain.
Journal of Neuroscience
,
20
,
RC65
.
Middleton
,
F. A.
, &
Strick
,
P. L.
(
2000
).
Basal ganglia output and cognition: Evidence from anatomical, behavioral, and clinical studies.
Brain and Cognition
,
42
,
183
200
.
Mohr
,
C.
,
Bracha
,
H. S.
, &
Brugger
,
P.
(
2003
).
Magical ideation modulates spatial behavior.
Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
,
15
,
168
174
.
Mohr
,
C.
,
Krummenacher
,
P.
,
Landis
,
T.
,
Sandor
,
P. S.
,
Fathi
,
M.
, &
Brugger
,
P.
(
2005
).
Psychometric schizotypy modulates levodopa effects on lateralized lexical decision performance.
Journal of Psychiatric Research
,
39
,
241
250
.
Mohr
,
C.
,
Landis
,
T.
,
Bracha
,
H. S.
,
Fathi
,
M.
, &
Brugger
,
P.
(
2003
).
Human locomotion: Levodopa keeps you straight.
Neuroscience Letters
,
339
,
115
118
.
Mohr
,
C.
,
Landis
,
T.
,
Bracha
,
H. S.
,
Fathi
,
M.
, &
Brugger
,
P.
(
2005
).
Levodopa reverses gait asymmetries related to anhedonia and magical ideation.
European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience
,
255
,
33
39
.
Mohr
,
C.
,
Landis
,
T.
, &
Brugger
,
P.
(
2006
).
Lateralized semantic priming: Modulation by levodopa, semantic distance, and participants' magical beliefs.
Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment
,
2
,
71
84
.
Mohr
,
C.
,
Landis
,
T.
,
Sandor
,
P. S.
,
Fathi
,
M.
, &
Brugger
,
P.
(
2004
).
Nonstereotyped responding in positive schizotypy after a single dose of levodopa.
Neuropsychopharmacology
,
29
,
1741
1751
.
Mohr
,
C.
,
Rohrenbach
,
C. M.
,
Laska
,
M.
, &
Brugger
,
P.
(
2001
).
Unilateral olfactory perception and magical ideation.
Schizophrenia
, Research,
47
,
255
264
.
Muller
,
U.
,
von Cramon
,
D. Y.
, &
Pollmann
,
S.
(
1998
).
D1- versus D2-receptor modulation of visuospatial working memory in humans.
Journal of Neuroscience
,
18
,
2720
2728
.
Nieoullon
,
A.
(
2002
).
Dopamine and the regulation of cognition and attention.
Progress in Neurobiology
,
67
,
53
83
.
Pickar
,
D.
,
Labarca
,
R.
,
Linnoila
,
M.
,
Roy
,
A.
,
Hommer
,
D.
,
Everett
,
D.
,
et al
(
1984
).
Neuroleptic-induced decrease in plasma homovanillic acid and antipsychotic activity in schizophrenic patients.
Science
,
225
,
954
957
.
Pizzagalli
,
D.
,
Lehmann
,
D.
, &
Brugger
,
P.
(
2001
).
Lateralized direct and indirect semantic priming effects in subjects with paranormal experiences and beliefs.
Psychopathology
,
34
,
75
80
.
Pizzagalli
,
D.
,
Lehmann
,
D.
,
Gianotti
,
L.
,
Koenig
,
T.
,
Tanaka
,
H.
,
Wackermann
,
J.
,
et al
(
2000
).
Brain electric correlates of strong belief in paranormal phenomena: Intracerebral EEG source and regional Omega complexity analyses.
Psychiatry Research
,
100
,
139
154
.
Previc
,
F. H.
(
1999
).
Dopamine and the origins of human intelligence.
Brain and Cognition
,
41
,
299
350
.
Raz
,
A.
,
Hines
,
T.
,
Fossella
,
J.
, &
Castro
,
D.
(
2008
).
Paranormal experience and the COMT dopaminergic gene: A preliminary attempt to associate phenotype with genotype using an underlying brain theory.
Cortex
,
44
,
1336
1341
.
Reed
,
P.
,
Wakefield
,
D.
,
Harris
,
J.
,
Parry
,
J.
,
Cella
,
M.
, &
Tsakanikos
,
E.
(
2008
).
Seeing non-existent events: Effects of environmental conditions, schizotypal symptoms, and sub-clinical characteristics.
Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry
,
39
,
276
291
.
Regard
,
M.
,
Landis
,
T.
, &
Graves
,
R.
(
1985
).
Dissociated hemispheric superiorities for reading stenography vs. print.
Neuropsychologia
,
23
,
431
435
.
Reynolds
,
G. P.
(
1983
).
Increased concentrations and lateral asymmetry of amygdala dopamine in schizophrenia.
Nature
,
305
,
527
529
.
Sabsevitz
,
D. S.
,
Medler
,
D. A.
,
Seidenberg
,
M.
, &
Binder
,
J. R.
(
2005
).
Modulation of the semantic system by word imageability.
Neuroimage
,
27
,
188
200
.
Sarkin
,
A. J.
,
Dionisio
,
D. P.
,
Hillix
,
W. A.
, &
Granholm
,
E.
(
1998
).
Positive and negative schizotypal symptoms relate to different aspects of crossover reaction time task performance.
Psychiatry Research
,
81
,
241
249
.
Seamans
,
J. K.
, &
Yang
,
C. R.
(
2004
).
The principal features and mechanisms of dopamine modulation in the prefrontal cortex.
Progress in Neurobiology
,
74
,
1
58
.
Sekine
,
Y.
,
Iyo
,
M.
,
Ouchi
,
Y.
,
Matsunaga
,
T.
,
Tsukada
,
H.
,
Okada
,
H.
,
et al
(
2001
).
Methamphetamine-related psychiatric symptoms and reduced brain dopamine transporters studied with PET.
American Journal of Psychiatry
,
158
,
1206
1214
.
Shaner
,
A.
(
1999
).
Delusions, superstitious conditioning and chaotic dopamine neurodynamics.
Medical Hypotheses
,
52
,
119
123
.
Siever
,
L. J.
,
Amin
,
F.
,
Coccaro
,
E. F.
,
Trestman
,
R.
,
Silverman
,
J.
,
Horvath
,
T. B.
,
et al
(
1993
).
CSF homovanillic acid in schizotypal personality disorder.
American Journal of Psychiatry
,
150
,
149
151
.
Smith
,
A.
,
Li
,
M.
,
Becker
,
S.
, &
Kapur
,
S.
(
2006
).
Dopamine, prediction error and associative learning: A model-based account.
Network
,
17
,
61
84
.
Spitzer
,
M.
(
1997
).
A cognitive neuroscience view of schizophrenic thought disorder.
Schizophrenia Bulletin
,
23
,
29
50
.
Spitzer
,
M.
, &
Walter
,
H.
(
2003
).
In A. David & T. T. Kircher (Eds.)
,
The self in neuroscience and psychiatry
(pp.
436
444
).
Cambridge, MA
:
Cambridge University Press
.
Tallent
,
K. A.
, &
Gooding
,
D. C.
(
1999
).
Working memory and Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance in schizotypic individuals: A replication and extension.
Psychiatry Research
,
89
,
161
170
.
Thalbourne
,
M. A.
, &
Delin
,
P. S.
(
1993
).
A new instrument for measuring the sheep–goat variable: Its psychometric properties and factor structure.
Journal of the Society for Psychical Research
,
59
,
172
186
.
Tsakanikos
,
E.
, &
Reed
,
P.
(
2005
).
Seeing words that are not there: Detection biases in schizotypy.
British Journal of Clinical Psychology
,
44
,
295
299
.
Weinberger
,
D. R.
,
Egan
,
M. F.
,
Bertolino
,
A.
,
Callicott
,
J. H.
,
Mattay
,
V. S.
,
Lipska
,
B. K.
,
et al
(
2001
).
Prefrontal neurons and the genetics of schizophrenia.
Biological Psychiatry
,
50
,
825
844
.
Weinstein
,
S.
, &
Graves
,
R. E.
(
2001
).
Creativity, schizotypy, and laterality.
Cognitive Neuropsychiatry
,
6
,
131
146
.
Weinstein
,
S.
, &
Graves
,
R. E.
(
2002
).
Are creativity and schizotypy products of a right hemisphere bias?
Brain and Cognition
,
49
,
138
151
.
White
,
T. L.
,
Lott
,
D. C.
, &
de Wit
,
H.
(
2006
).
Personality and the subjective effects of acute amphetamine in healthy volunteers.
Neuropsychopharmacology
,
31
,
1064
1074
.
Williams
,
G. V.
, &
Goldman-Rakic
,
P. S.
(
1995
).
Modulation of memory fields by dopamine D1 receptors in prefrontal cortex.
Nature
,
376
,
572
575
.
Wiseman
,
R.
,
Greening
,
E.
, &
Smith
,
M.
(
2003
).
Belief in the paranormal and suggestion in the seance room.
British Journal of Psychology
,
94
,
285
297
.