Studies of semantic dementia, imaging, and repetitive TMS have suggested that the bilateral anterior temporal lobes (ATLs) underpin a modality-invariant representational hub within the semantic system. Questions remain, however, regarding functional specialization across a variety of knowledge domains within the ATL region. We investigated direct evidence for the functional relevance of the superior ATL in processing social concepts. Using converging evidence from noninvasive brain stimulation and neuropsychology, we demonstrate graded differentiation of right and left superior anterior temporal areas in social cognition. Whereas the left superior ATL is necessary for processing both social and nonsocial abstract concepts, social conceptual processing predominates in the right superior ATL. This graded hemispheric specialization is mirrored in the patient results. Our data shed new light on the classic debate about hemispheric differences in semantic and social cognition. These results are considered in the context of models of semantic representation and the emerging data on connectivity for left and right ATL regions.

Human behavior occurs in social contexts. Human neuroscience needs to understand which brain regions support social behavior and how they function. Multiple lines of evidence suggest that the anterior temporal lobes (ATLs) are part of the network supporting semantic (Lambon Ralph, 2013) and social cognition (Zahn et al., 2007; Amodio & Frith, 2006; Moll, Zahn, de Oliveira-Souza, Krueger, & Grafman, 2005).

The role of the ATL in semantic cognition has been supported by patient investigations (Patterson, Nestor, & Rogers, 2007; Snowden, Goulding, & Neary, 1989), neuroimaging studies (Visser, Embleton, Jefferies, Parker, & Lambon Ralph, 2010; Vandenberghe, Price, Wise, Josephs, & Frackowiak, 1996), and neurostimulation studies (Pobric, Jefferies, & Lambon Ralph, 2007, 2010; Lambon Ralph, Pobric, & Jefferies, 2009). Neuropsychological evidence comes from patients with semantic dementia (SD), who have bilateral atrophy and hypometabolism in the ATLs. In addition to a generalized yet selective semantic impairment, right ATL (rATL) neurodegeneration has been associated with impairments of social behavior (Chan et al., 2009), such as lack of empathy (Rankin et al., 2006), socially inappropriate behavior (Zahn et al., 2009), and theory of mind tasks (Irish, Hodges, & Piguet, 2014), although with careful assessment, similar neuropsychiatric features are observed in cases with predominantly left ATL (lATL) atrophy, only less frequently (Chan et al., 2009).

Studies of patients with SD have given rise to the “hub-and-spoke” hypothesis in which the ATLs are a key node in the semantic network where different inputs converge to form modality-invariant conceptual representations (Lambon Ralph, Sage, Jones, & Mayberry, 2010; Patterson et al., 2007; Rogers & McClelland, 2004). Given that SD is a neurodegenerative condition, there is always the possibility that semantic dysfunction follows from pathology extending beyond the ATL, and it is this more subtle, widespread damage that is the cause of the patients' semantic impairment (Martin, 2007). In addition, because SD is characterized by bilateral atrophy, albeit sometimes very asymmetric in the early phase, it is not possible to investigate the contribution of lATL and rATL in isolation. Given these facts, the contributions of the ATL to semantic processing cannot be uniquely defined on the basis of this neuropsychological evidence alone.

Neuroimaging studies of semantic cognition have highlighted the role of the left hemisphere in the comprehension and production of spoken language (Wise, 2003). Furthermore, the importance of left anterior temporal cortex for language processing and semantic memory has been highlighted with PET (Devlin et al., 2000; Vandenberghe et al., 1996), intracranial recordings (Halgren, Baudena, Heit, Clarke, & Marinkovic, 1994), and MEG (Marinkovic et al., 2003). Bilateral ATL activations have been observed in semantic tasks for words (Binney, Embleton, Jefferies, Parke, & Lambon Ralph, 2010; Zahn et al., 2007), sounds, spoken names, and pictures (Visser & Lambon Ralph, 2011; Sharp, Scott, & Wise, 2004). The neuroimaging literature of social cognition has reported a right hemispheric bias for social stimuli (Skipper, Ross, & Olson, 2011; Zahn et al., 2007). Zahn and colleagues (2007) have suggested that the right superior ATL (sATL) selectively represents social concepts. When participants were making semantic judgments about social concepts (e.g., “honor–brave”) versus animal function concepts (e.g., “trainable–ridden”), right sATL regions were activated. This was independently corroborated in a recent study (Skipper et al., 2011)—although the peak activations were found in the lATL rather than rATL region in another study (Ross & Olson, 2010), calling into question the strength of ATL laterality for social conceptual processing. In addition, ATL activations have been reported for other high-level social verbal and nonverbal tasks: moral judgments (Moll et al., 2005), social versus nonsocial gestures (Straube, Green, Jansen, Chatterjee, & Kircher, 2010), social attribution stimuli such as Heider and Simmel animations (Ross & Olson, 2010), and socioemotional stories (Ferstl & von Cramon, 2002). This consistency of ATL activations to high-level social tasks irrespective of the modality of the task (verbal or nonverbal) has led to its inclusion in the “social brain network” (Frith & Frith, 2012; Amodio & Frith, 2006).

It is clear that diverse social stimuli and tasks evoke activations in the ATLs. However, the precise role of the ATLs in the network remains controversial (Simmons, Reddish, Bellgowan, & Martin, 2010). By combining neurostimulation with patient investigation, we addressed three key limitations in the literature: (1) Most patients with neurodegeneration show widespread damage that is not only confined to subregions of the ATL unilaterally; (2) fMRI does not provide causal evidence for the necessity of activated regions in the task of interest; and (3) to our knowledge, no study has directly compared processing of social concepts in left and right sATL.

First, we established the necessary role of sATL in processing social versus nonsocial abstract concepts via TMS. We predicted that, if the rATL is involved in processing social concepts, as observed in patients with rATL neurodegeneration, then repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) should generate a greater effect on social than nonsocial concepts. If the lATL or rATL is implicated in general semantic processing, but not general cognition (as per patients with SD), then stimulation should impact on performance with both social and nonsocial abstract concepts but not on difficulty-matched nonsemantic tasks. In the neuropsychological part of the study, we predicted the lATL case would be impaired on processing both social and nonsocial concepts, in keeping with general degradation of semantic knowledge observed in patients with SD. On the basis of previous patient (Zahn et al., 2009) and neuroimaging evidence (Skipper et al., 2011; Zahn et al., 2007), we predicted that the rATL case would exhibit a greater impairment on processing social versus nonsocial concepts.

To establish a necessary role of the ATLs in representing semantic knowledge of social and nonsocial concepts, we investigated convergent evidence from TMS and neuropsychology. TMS is a well-established, noninvasive technique that induces electrical activation in the underlying cortex. A long train of low-frequency stimulation temporarily suppresses neural processing and disrupts behavioral tasks that rely on that cortical region (Walsh & Cowey, 2000). Cortical sites for TMS were left and right sATL and the occipital pole as a control site. Sites were identified in each individual participant through coregistration with their MRI scan. In a semantic synonym judgment task, participants were required to pick which of two concepts (e.g., “honest” vs. “eager”) is more closely related to the probe (“sincere”). Our control task was a difficulty-matched, number magnitude judgment assessment. Performance was assessed before and after TMS stimulation. In the neuropsychological part of the study, we used a semantic discrimination task, which probed knowledge of social and animal function concepts in two rare cases of focal neurodegeneration of the lATL and rATL, respectively.

Design

A 3 × 3 × 2 repeated-measures design was used, with site (right sATL vs. left sATL vs. occipital pole), task (nonsocial synonym judgment vs. social synonym judgment vs. magnitude judgment), and TMS (no TMS vs. rTMS stimulation) as the three within-participant factors. The study utilized rTMS, using the “virtual lesion” method in which the train of rTMS is delivered offline (without a concurrent behavioral task), and then behavioral performance is probed during the temporary refractory period and compared with performance on the same task outside this refractory window.

Participants

Twelve right-handed participants took part in the experiment (seven women; mean age = 25.8 years, SD = 3.8 years). All participants were strongly right-handed, with a laterality quotient above 75 (mean = 88.33, SD = 6.24) on the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield, 1971) and had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. All participants provided written consent for participation after being screened for adverse effects of TMS. The experiment was approved by the local ethics committee.

Stimuli

One hundred sixty concept trials were used in the synonym judgment task, and 80 number trials were used in the number judgment task. Each trial contained three concepts: a probe concept (e.g., “sincere”), a target choice (e.g., “honest”), and an unrelated foil (e.g., “eager”). All words within a triad were matched for frequency (mean = 28.49, SD = 11.23; Kučera & Francis, 1967) and imageability (mean = 275.71, SD = 43.72; Bird, Franklin, & Howard, 2001). All triads were divided into social and nonsocial trials based on ratings from independent evaluators. Item ratings were obtained for 450 words by asking 40 undergraduate students (8 men and 32 women) whose ages ranged from 18 to 32 (mean = 25) years to rate the words based on how social they are. Students were given a 7-point scale (1 = very nonsocial to 7 = very social) and asked to indicate the rating of each word. Students were given examples of possible ratings for words not included in the set. From that set, we created 80 social and 80 nonsocial trials. For nonsocial trial, the mean rating was 2.34, with SD = 0.60 and range = 1.73–2.94. For social trial, the mean rating was 5.77, with SD = 0.79 and range = 5.02–6.56. Consequently, social and nonsocial trials were matched on psycholinguistic variables as well as behavioral measures: RTs and accuracy in pilot experiments. The format for the number task was the same as for the synonym judgment task: A three-digit probe number was presented at the top of the screen, and underneath, 2 three-digit number choices were given. Participants were required to pick which of the two numbers was closest in value to the probe. The pilot studies revealed that making magnitude judgments about three-digit numbers was the best behavioral match (in terms of RTs and accuracy) for our synonym task.

Task and Procedure

A PC running E-Prime software (Psychology Software Tools, Inc., Pittsburgh, PA) allowed the presentation of stimuli and recording of the responses. In a single experimental session, participants performed a synonym judgment task and a number magnitude judgment task. In a semantic synonym judgment task, participants were required to pick which of two words (e.g., “honest” vs. “eager”) is more closely related to the probe (“sincere”). The probe is presented at the top of the screen, and the two word options are presented underneath. The participants indicated their choice by using both hands for pressing a single key (M or Z). Half of the targets were associated with each key, so they used each hand for 50% of the answers. The occurrence of target location was randomized for each participant and each session. The experiment began with a practice block of 10 trials for each stimulus set. Experimental trials were presented in a random order in two blocks (80 synonyms and 40 numbers). After 10 min of offline rTMS, another two blocks (80 synonyms and 40 numbers) followed. This yielded 240 trials per experimental session. The blocks were randomized across participants. Stimuli were presented until the response was given (maximum duration = 3000 msec) and followed by a blank screen (duration = 500 msec). Computer key presses were recorded using a computer keyboard that was placed in front of each participant. Response latencies were recorded by the computer, and the accuracy was checked in the analyses.

TMS

A MagStim Rapid2 (Magstim Co., Whitland, UK) stimulator with two external boosters was used (maximum output = approx. 2.2 T). Magnetic stimulation was applied using a 50-mm figure-of-eight coil.

Selection of TMS Site

The structural T1-weighted MRI scans were coregistered with the participant's scalp using MRIreg (www.mccauslandcenter.sc.edu/mricro/mricron/index.html). Immediately before the TMS session, scalp coordinates were measured using an Ascension Minibird (www.ascension-tech.com) magnetic tracking system. The right Montreal Neurological Institute coordinates for the sATL (53, 8, −13) were obtained by averaging peak coordinates from previous fMRI studies associating the right sATL with knowledge of social concepts (Zahn et al., 2007: 51, 15, −12; (Zahn et al., 2009: 54, 0, −3) and peak coordinates of resting state glucose hypometabolism from a study of social concepts in frontotemporal lobar degeneration (FTLD; 54, 9, −24: Zahn et al., 2009). The coordinates for the left sATL corresponded to the homologue region (−53, 8, −13). A middle occipital stimulation site (Oz) was also employed as a site to control for possible nonspecific TMS effects.

Stimulation Parameters

Individual active motor threshold was determined for every participant. rTMS was delivered offline for 10 min at 1 Hz (600 sec at 120% motor threshold level) applied to the right sATL, left sATL, and Oz. The coil was securely held by experimenter, centered over the site to be stimulated. The average stimulation intensity during rTMS was 64% of the machine output.

Patients

Two female (age = 60 and 61 years at the time of testing, education = 13 and 14 years) right-handed patients with FTLD according to Lund–Manchester Consensus criteria (Neary et al., 1998) were selected from a larger case series because they exhibited the rare picture of predominant atrophy of the lATL versus rATL, respectively. The Lund–Manchester Consensus criteria were previously shown to correlate highly with postmortem neuropathological diagnoses (Snowden et al., 2011). The study was approved by the South Manchester NHS Research Ethics Committee, and the patients gave written informed consent. In addition to extensive background neuropsychological testing, a neurological examination, and an extensive neuropsychiatric interview with their caregivers, they received the social concept discrimination task (Zahn et al., 2009). Both patients showed features of SD as classified by a senior neuropsychiatrist (R. Z.) and senior neuropsychologist (M. A. L. R.), but the rATL case presented primarily with behavioral features. Data for healthy older adults are taken from published test norms and, for the same test, from Zahn et al. (2009).

Materials and Procedure

The semantic task consisted of two carefully matched conditions to compare concepts describing social behavior (i.e., social concepts) and concepts describing nonsocial behavior or properties of animals (i.e., animal function concepts) of equal familiarity and descriptiveness (see previous description in Zahn et al., 2009). A prime word describing a concept (e.g., “adventurous”) was presented at the top of a screen, and participants had to decide which of two concepts underneath was more related in meaning to the prime concept at the top. Target (e.g., “courage”) and distracter (e.g., “controlled”) concepts were chosen from the same category as the prime concepts (e.g., animal function concept prime: “trainable,” target: “ridden,” distracter: “bites”) and had been used in our previous fMRI studies (Ross & Olson, 2010; Zahn et al., 2007). Seventy-three items with 80% response agreement in aged controls were presented randomly (25 positive social concept triads, 24 negative social concept triads, and 24 animal function concept triads).

RT Analyses

The RTs for all participants and all conditions were submitted to a repeated-measures ANOVA with three within-participant factors: Task (nonsocial synonym judgment, social synonym judgment, and magnitude judgment), Site (right sATL, left sATL, and Oz), and TMS (no TMS vs. rTMS). We removed 16 items from the synonym task (11 nonsocial and 5 social) and two items from the number task that yielded over 50% errors in the no-TMS condition. This resulted in removal of 7.9% of items. The main effects of Task, Site, and TMS were not significant. We observed a significant interaction between Task and TMS (F = 36.108, df = 2, 22, p < .001). We also report a significant interaction between Task and Site (F = 4.196, df = 2, 22, p = .029). Crucially, we observed a significant three-way interaction between Task, Site, and TMS (F = 5.259, df = 4, 44, p = .001). To explore the three-way interaction further, we split the original analysis by task and carried out three repeated-measures ANOVA with two within-participant factors Site (right sATL, left sATL, and Oz) and TMS (no TMS vs. rTMS). For the nonsocial task, main effects of Site and TMS were not significant. However, we observed a significant interaction between Site and TMS (F = 7.279, df = 2, 22, p = .004). For the social task, we observed a main effect of TMS (F = 14.994, df = 1, 11, p = .003) and a significant interaction between TMS and Site (F = 6.005, df = 2, 22, p = .008). For the number task, we only observed a main effect of the TMS (F = 10.698, df = 1, 11, p = .007). Planned t tests on the RTs were used to compare performance in rATL and lATL for social and nonsocial concepts. After controlling for false discovery rate (Benjamini & Hochberg, 1995), there was a difference for social and nonsocial concepts between rATL and lATL. The rATL was more involved in processing social than nonsocial concepts, with a significant TMS effect for social concepts, t(11) = 3.713, p < .01, but not for nonsocial concepts, t(11) = 1.67, p > .05, whereas the lATL was involved in both social and nonsocial concepts (t(11) = 3.63, p < .004, and t(11) = 2.285, p < .05, respectively; see Figure 1A).

Figure 1. 

Effect of rTMS on synonym judgment and magnitude judgment tasks for RTs (A) and error rates (B). Each bar represents the mean decision time alongside the corresponding standard error adjusted for within-participant comparisons (Loftus & Masson, 1994) for each condition. Occipital = occipital pole.

Figure 1. 

Effect of rTMS on synonym judgment and magnitude judgment tasks for RTs (A) and error rates (B). Each bar represents the mean decision time alongside the corresponding standard error adjusted for within-participant comparisons (Loftus & Masson, 1994) for each condition. Occipital = occipital pole.

Close modal

Error Analyses

The error rate was examined in a repeated-measures ANOVA with Task (synonym judgment, number judgment), Site (right sATL, left sATL, and Oz), and TMS (no TMS vs. rTMS) as factors. There was a main effect of Task (F = 6.341, df = 2, 22, p = .007) as well as a main effect of TMS (F = 20.982, df = 1, 11, p = .001). We also observed a significant interaction between Task and TMS (F = 6.613, df = 2, 22, p = .011). All other main effects or interactions were not significant (p > .1). To explore the task-and-TMS interaction further, we split the original analysis by task and carried out three repeated-measures ANOVA with two within-participant factors Site (right sATL, left sATL, and Oz) and TMS (no TMS vs. rTMS). For the nonsocial task, the main effect of TMS was significant (F = 14.88, df = 1, 11, p = .003). All other main effects or interactions were not significant (p > .1). For the social task, we observed the main effect of TMS (F = 15.604, df = 1, 11, p = .002), whereas the main effect of Site and Site x-TMS interaction were not significant (p > .1). For the number task, all main effects or interactions were not significant (p > .1; see Figure 1B).

Neuropsychological Results

Neuropsychological data were analyzed using an established method for directly comparing two single cases with each other (C_CTC; Crawford, Garthwaite, & Wood, 2010). We report one-tailed p values (lower performance on social concepts relative to animal function concepts in patients with rATL damage compared with patients with no damage to this region). We also compared the performance of patients with the control group by using an established method for individual patient-based analysis (Singlims: Crawford & Garthwaite, 2002). Both the rATL and lATL cases were impaired relative to the controls on social and nonsocial concepts (all t(29)s > 2.35, all ps < .013, one-tailed). However, the difference in impairment between social and nonsocial concepts was significantly larger for the rATL case than for the lATL case, t(29) = 3.0, p < .005 (see Figure 2).

Figure 2. 

Overall performance of the lATL patient and rATL patient compared with the control group on social and nonsocial concepts. Error bars indicate SEM on the control group (n = 30) performance.

Figure 2. 

Overall performance of the lATL patient and rATL patient compared with the control group on social and nonsocial concepts. Error bars indicate SEM on the control group (n = 30) performance.

Close modal

The similarities between our TMS data and neuropsychological investigation can be observed in Figure 3.

Figure 3. 

Stimulation sites, TMS results, lesion sites, and patient performance data on social and nonsocial concept task conditions. (A) ATL regions that were disrupted with TMS, shown for one participant. (B) The TMS effect on semantic judgments of social and nonsocial concepts and magnitude judgment of numbers. Each bar represents the TMS effect as RT change caused by TMS. Error bars indicate SEM adjusted to reflect the between-condition variance used in repeated-measure designs (Loftus & Masson, 1994). (C) MRI images for patients with lATL- and rATL-predominant SD. (D) Overall impairment in percentage points (% correct control − % correct patient) of the lATL patient and rATL patient compared with the control group on social and nonsocial concepts. Error bars indicate SEM on the control group (n = 30) performance. L-sATL = left sATL; R-sATL = right sATL; OCC = occipital pole; LATL atrophy = patient with predominantly lATL atrophy; RATL atrophy = patient with predominantly rATL atrophy. Asterisk denotes significantly slower RTs after rTMS than at baseline. Asterisk denotes significantly worse performance on a task.

Figure 3. 

Stimulation sites, TMS results, lesion sites, and patient performance data on social and nonsocial concept task conditions. (A) ATL regions that were disrupted with TMS, shown for one participant. (B) The TMS effect on semantic judgments of social and nonsocial concepts and magnitude judgment of numbers. Each bar represents the TMS effect as RT change caused by TMS. Error bars indicate SEM adjusted to reflect the between-condition variance used in repeated-measure designs (Loftus & Masson, 1994). (C) MRI images for patients with lATL- and rATL-predominant SD. (D) Overall impairment in percentage points (% correct control − % correct patient) of the lATL patient and rATL patient compared with the control group on social and nonsocial concepts. Error bars indicate SEM on the control group (n = 30) performance. L-sATL = left sATL; R-sATL = right sATL; OCC = occipital pole; LATL atrophy = patient with predominantly lATL atrophy; RATL atrophy = patient with predominantly rATL atrophy. Asterisk denotes significantly slower RTs after rTMS than at baseline. Asterisk denotes significantly worse performance on a task.

Close modal

In this study, we were able to ascertain differential contributions of left and right sATL regions to social cognition and general semantic processing. The left sATL region was implicated in both social and nonsocial concepts. In contrast, the right sATL made a greater contribution to social than nonsocial concepts. Our results are in agreement with the central role of the ATL in representing conceptual knowledge (Lambon Ralph, 2013; Patterson et al., 2007; Spitsyna, Warren, Scott, Turkheimer, & Wise, 2006; Bozeat, Lambon Ralph, Patterson, Garrard, & Hodges, 2000) and the importance of the right temporal lobe for social cognitive impairments in rATL neurodegeneration (Chan et al., 2009; Liu et al., 2004).

In a review of the role of superior temporal areas (STS), Hein and Knight (2008) proposed that the same brain region can support different cognitive functions determined by a task-dependent network of connections. Indeed, this hypothesis has been formally explored previously for the ATLs and semantic processing, using computational models (Schapiro, McClelland, Welbourne, Rogers, & Lambon Ralph, 2013; Lambon Ralph, McClelland, Patterson, Galton, & Hodges, 2001). Most relevant to the current findings, Lambon Ralph et al. (2001) demonstrated that a bilateral ATL semantic system exhibits a left-sided sensitivity to language (speech production) as a consequence of greater connectivity of the lATL to left-hemisphere language centers. Following the same line of argument, the differential involvement of left and right sATL regions in social and nonsocial conceptual processing could be explained by the specific pattern of connections between the ATL regions with the rest of the brain. Indeed, Skipper et al. (2011) have already proposed that a greater role for the right temporal region in social processing might reflect a right-sided pattern of connectivity to social-processing regions.

These proposals are consistent with the emerging story from recent white matter connectivity studies. Specifically, the temporal pole has dense connections with fusiform gyrus, limbic structures, and pFC (Pascual et al., 2014; Gloor, 1997). These brain areas are involved in linguistic, social, and emotional processing as well as decision-making (Simmons et al., 2010; Gloor, 1997). In an in vivo probabilistic tractography study, Binney, Parker, and Lambon Ralph (2012) showed that there is strong intratemporal lobe ATL white matter connectivity, conducive to the information integration required for the extraction of modality-invariant, general semantic representations. Furthermore, there are more regionally specific connectivity patterns to nontemporal lobe areas. Relevant to the formation of social concepts, the sATL and temporal pole are connected to the frontal lobe and OFC via the uncinate fasciculus (UF), a bidirectional white matter pathway. In a recent study, Catani and colleagues (2013) used tractography to show that damage to UF correlated with deficits in semantic processing in patients with primary progressive aphasia. It is quite possible, therefore, that the sATL-rTMS effect on social concepts observed in this study reflects either the convergence and representation of socially relevant information at the sATL site and/or the perturbation of information transmission between ATL and orbitofrontal regions (Quentin, Chanes, Migliaccio, Valabrègue, & Valero-Cabré, 2013; Neubert, Mars, Buch, Olivier, & Rushworth, 2010).

The graded, differential roles of lATL and rATL in social and general concepts may well result from the pattern of connectivity. As noted above, the role of sATL in social concepts (left and right) probably reflects its connectivity between temporal cortex and OFC (Pascual et al., 2014; Catani et al., 2013; Binney et al., 2012; Moran, Mufson, & Mesulam, 1987). After previous proposals and computational demonstrations (Lambon Ralph et al., 2001), the graded functional differences between left and right probably reflect (i) a right > left bias in UF density (Highley, Walker, Esiri, Crow, & Harrison, 2002)—which will encourage a rightward division of labor for social conceptual processing—and (ii) greater left-sided language connectivity (both “dorsal” arcuate fasciculus and “ventral” medial longitudinal fasciculus/extreme capsule complex language pathways are left hemisphere biased; Parker et al., 2005).

The bilateral ATLs play a crucial role in semantic memory (Lambon Ralph et al., 2009; Patterson et al., 2007). It is undisputed that the left hemisphere has a central role in the comprehension and production of spoken language (Wise, 2003). Furthermore, imaging studies show that abstract nouns produce greater activation almost exclusively in the left hemisphere in superior temporal and inferior frontal cortex (Binder, Desai, Graves, & Conant, 2009; Spitsyna et al., 2006; Sabsevitz, Medler, Seidenberg, & Binder, 2005). This finding has been confirmed by neurostimulation (Hoffman, Jefferies, & Lambon Ralph, 2010) and patient studies (Hoffman, Pobric, & Lambon Ralph, 2012). Moreover, patients with lATL neurodegeneration also exhibit behavioral problems such as behavioral rigidity, disinhibition, apathy, and obsessional behavior as well as mood changes such as depression (Chan et al., 2009). However, these problems are often masked and underreported given the predominant language problems associated with lATL neurodegeneration. Although the left UF connects lATL with pFC, it has been shown that direct stimulation of the left uncinate does not result in general language impairments (Duffau, Gatignol, Moritz-Gasser, & Mandonnet, 2009). Therefore, one possibility is that the lATL is specialized for verbal semantic processing because language representations are left lateralized: For example, using a connectionist model, Lambon Ralph et al. (2001) accounted for left–right asymmetries in picture naming in terms of stronger connections between lATL and speech output processes in the left hemisphere.

Extending this hypothesis, it is possible that the right temporal lobe might make a greater contribution to social semantic processing if the socially relevant information that interacts with semantic representations comes more strongly from right than left posterior regions or if the preferential left-hemisphere language connectivity induces a division of labor across the ATLs such that the rATL defaults to social processing because of connections to right prefrontal and orbitofrontal regions. This is in accordance with a recent meta-analysis of functional imaging studies of conceptual knowledge, which argues for a graded version of the hub-and-spoke account, whereby the representation of conceptual knowledge is supported by bilateral yet graded connectivity between the ATLs and modality-specific sensory, motor, and limbic cortices (Rice, Lambon Ralph, & Hoffman, 2015).

Although we used SD as a test case for patient performance, these results have broader implications and are consistent with social processing deficits in other neurological and psychiatric syndromes. Specifically, it is well established that individuals with psychopathy have marked difficulties in processing abstract concepts (Kiehl et al., 2004). Subsequently, in an fMRI study, decreased rATL activations were reported when psychopaths were making lexical decisions about abstract concepts. In addition, in this group, ATLs are characterized by structural abnormalities such as reduced gray matter volume, including weaker connections from rATL to orbitofrontal regions (Craig et al., 2009).

As well as giving insights about the role of lATL and rATL regions in social and general semantic concepts, the current results provide a new perspective on the rich set of clinical results. Detailed studies of left and right temporal lobes, FTLD (SD) cases have tended to emphasize the fact that left-sided cases have a predominantly language-focussed presentation; and the right-sided patients, a social deficit (Chan et al., 2009; Zahn et al., 2009; Rosen et al., 2005; Miller, Darby, Benson, Cummings, & Miller, 1997). Consistent with the results from the current study and implications of the white matter neuroanatomy reviewed above, Hornberger, Geng, and Hodges (2011) found that patients with SD had decreased fractional anisotropy values in the left and right UF, compared with the healthy controls. Yet, only in patients with rATL neurodegeneration did decreased fractional anisotropy values correlate with behavioral symptoms. The clinical literature does not support, however, an absolute dissociation between general versus social conceptual representations in lATL versus rATL regions, respectively. Indeed, in a more formal assessment of neuropsychiatric symptoms, Chan et al. (2009) found that both patients with predominantly left- and right-sided ATL were reported to have social and behavioral impairments to different degrees. Our study validates these clinical studies (Chan et al., 2009; Zahn et al., 2009) by combining approaches from classical neuropsychology and cognitive neuroscience; we demonstrate graded hemispheric specialization of ATL regions to social cognition and general semantic knowledge.

We thank Helen Beaumont, Dr. Sophie Green, and Dr. Paul Hoffman for administering neuropsychological tests. We thank the patients and controls for their continued support of our research. This research was supported by MRC program grants (G0501632 and MR/J004146/1) to M. A. L. R. and an MRC Clinician Scientist Fellowship (G0902304) to R. Z.

Reprint requests should be sent to Dr. Gorana Pobric, Neuroscience and Aphasia Research Unit, School of Psychological Sciences, University of Manchester, Oxford Road, Manchester, M13 9PL, United Kingdom, or via e-mail: [email protected].

Amodio
,
D. M.
, &
Frith
,
C. D.
(
2006
).
Meeting of minds: The medial frontal cortex and social cognition
.
Nature Reviews Neuroscience
,
4
,
268
277
.
Benjamini
,
Y.
, &
Hochberg
,
Y.
(
1995
).
Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing
.
Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, Series B
,
57
,
289
300
.
Binder
,
J. R.
,
Desai
,
R. H.
,
Graves
,
W. W.
, &
Conant
,
L. L.
(
2009
).
Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies
.
Cerebral Cortex
,
19
,
2767
2796
.
Binney
,
R. J.
,
Embleton
,
K. V.
,
Jefferies
,
E.
,
Parke
,
G. J. M.
, &
Lambon Ralph
,
M. A.
(
2010
).
Ventral and inferolateral aspects of the anterior temporal lobe are crucial in semantic memory: Evidence from a novel direct comparison of distortion-corrected fMRI, rTMS, and semantic dementia
.
Cerebral Cortex
,
20
,
2728
2738
.
Binney
,
R. J.
,
Parker
,
G. J.
, &
Lambon Ralph
,
M. A.
(
2012
).
Convergent connectivity and graded specialization in the rostral human temporal lobe as revealed by diffusion-weighted imaging probabilistic tractography
.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
,
10
,
1998
2014
.
Bird
,
H.
,
Franklin
,
S.
, &
Howard
,
D.
(
2001
).
Age of acquisition and imageability ratings for a large set of words, including verbs and function words
.
Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers
,
33
,
73
79
.
Bozeat
,
S.
,
Lambon Ralph
,
M. A.
,
Patterson
,
K.
,
Garrard
,
P.
, &
Hodges
,
J. R.
(
2000
).
Non-verbal semantic impairment in semantic dementia
.
Neuropsychologia
,
38
,
1207
1215
.
Catani
,
M.
,
Mesulam
,
M. M.
,
Jakobsenm
,
E.
,
Malik
,
F.
,
Martersteck
,
A.
,
Wieneke
,
C.
, et al
(
2013
).
A novel frontal pathway underlies verbal fluency in primary progressive aphasia
.
Brain
,
136
,
2619
2628
.
Chan
,
D.
,
Anderson
,
V.
,
Pijnenburg
,
Y.
,
Whitwell
,
J.
,
Barnes
,
J.
,
Scahill
,
R.
, et al
(
2009
).
The clinical profile of right temporal lobe atrophy
.
Brain
,
132
,
1287
1298
.
Craig
,
M. C.
,
Catani
,
M.
,
Deeley
,
Q.
,
Latham
,
R.
,
Daly
,
E.
,
Kanaan
,
R.
, et al
(
2009
).
Altered connections on the road to psychopathy
.
Molecular Psychiatry
,
14
,
946
953
.
Crawford
,
J. R.
, &
Garthwaite
,
P. H.
(
2002
).
Investigation of the single case in neuropsychology: Confidence limits on the abnormality of test scores and test score differences
.
Neuropsychologia
,
40
,
1196
1208
.
Crawford
,
J. R.
,
Garthwaite
,
P. H.
, &
Wood
,
L. T.
(
2010
).
Inferential methods for comparing two single cases
.
Cognitive Neuropsychology
,
27
,
377
400
.
Devlin
,
J. T.
,
Russell
,
R. P.
,
Davis
,
M. H.
,
Price
,
C. J.
,
Wilson
,
J.
,
Moss
,
H. E.
, et al
(
2000
).
Susceptibility-induced loss of signal: Comparing PET and fMRI on a semantic task
.
Neuroimage
,
11
,
589
600
.
Duffau
,
H.
,
Gatignol
,
P.
,
Moritz-Gasser
,
S.
, &
Mandonnet
,
E.
(
2009
).
Is the left uncinate fasciculus essential for language? A cerebral stimulation study
.
Journal of Neurology
,
256
,
382
389
.
Ferstl
,
E. C.
, &
von Cramon
,
D. Y.
(
2002
).
What does the frontomedian cortex contribute to language processing: Coherence or theory of mind?
Neuroimage
,
17
,
1599
1612
.
Frith
,
C. D.
, &
Frith
,
U.
(
2012
).
Mechanisms of social cognition
.
Annual Review of Psychology
,
63
,
287
313
.
Gloor
,
P.
(
1997
).
The temporal lobe and the limbic system
.
Oxford
:
Oxford University Press
.
Halgren
,
E.
,
Baudena
,
P.
,
Heit
,
G.
,
Clarke
,
M.
, &
Marinkovic
,
K.
(
1994
).
Spatiotemporal stages in face and word-processing: 1. Depth recorded potentials in the human occipital and parietal lobes
.
Journal of Physiology-Paris
,
88
,
1
50
.
Hein
,
G.
, &
Knight
,
R. T.
(
2008
).
Superior temporal sulcus—It's my area: Or is it?
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
,
20
,
2125
2136
.
Highley
,
J. R.
,
Walker
,
M. A.
,
Esiri
,
M. M.
,
Crow
,
T. J.
, &
Harrison
,
P. J.
(
2002
).
Asymmetry of the uncinate fasciculus: A post-mortem study of normal subjects and patients with schizophrenia
.
Cerebral Cortex
,
11
,
1218
1224
.
Hoffman
,
P.
,
Jefferies
,
E.
, &
Lambon Ralph
,
M. A.
(
2010
).
Ventrolateral prefrontal cortex plays an executive regulation role in comprehension of abstract words: Convergent neuropsychological and rTMS evidence
.
Journal of Neuroscience
,
46
,
15450
15456
.
Hoffman
,
P.
,
Pobric
,
G.
, &
Lambon Ralph
,
M. A.
(
2012
).
Brain networks in semantic association tasks
.
Aphasiology
,
26
,
1119
1130
.
Hornberger
,
M.
,
Geng
,
J.
, &
Hodges
,
J. R.
(
2011
).
Convergent grey and white matter evidence of orbitofrontal cortex changes related to disinhibition in behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia
.
Brain
,
134
,
2502
2512
.
Irish
,
M.
,
Hodges
,
J. R.
, &
Piguet
,
O.
(
2014
).
Right anterior temporal lobe dysfunction underlies theory of mind impairments in semantic dementia
.
Brain
,
137
,
1241
1253
.
Kiehl
,
K. A.
,
Smith
,
A. M.
,
Mendrek
,
A.
,
Forster
,
B. B.
,
Hare
,
R. D.
, &
Liddle
,
P. F.
(
2004
).
Temporal lobe abnormalities in semantic processing by criminal psychopaths as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging
.
Psychiatry Research
,
130
,
297
312
.
Kučera
,
H.
, &
Francis
,
W.
(
1967
).
Computational analysis of present day American English
.
Providence, RI
:
Brown University Press
.
Lambon Ralph
,
M. A.
(
2013
).
Neurocognitive insights on conceptual knowledge and its breakdown
.
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B, Biological Sciences
,
369
,
20120392
.
Lambon Ralph
,
M. A.
,
McClelland
,
J. L.
,
Patterson
,
K.
,
Galton
,
C. J.
, &
Hodges
,
J. R.
(
2001
).
No right to speak? The relationship between object naming and semantic impairment: Neuropsychological evidence and a computational model
.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
,
13
,
341
356
.
Lambon Ralph
,
M. A.
,
Pobric
,
G.
, &
Jefferies
,
E.
(
2009
).
Conceptual knowledge is underpinned by the temporal pole bilaterally: Convergent evidence from rTMS
.
Cerebral Cortex
,
19
,
832
838
.
Lambon Ralph
,
M. A.
,
Sage
,
K.
,
Jones
,
R. W.
, &
Mayberry
,
E. J.
(
2010
).
Coherent concepts are computed in the anterior temporal lobes
.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A.
,
107
,
2717
2722
.
Liu
,
W.
,
Miller
,
B. L.
,
Kramer
,
J. H.
,
Rankin
,
K.
,
Wyss-Coray
,
C.
,
Gearhart
,
R.
, et al
(
2004
).
Behavioral disorders in the frontal and temporal variants of frontotemporal dementia
.
Neurology
,
62
,
742
748
.
Loftus
,
G. R.
, &
Masson
,
M. E. J.
(
1994
).
Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs
.
Psychological Bulletin & Review
,
1
,
476
490
.
Marinkovic
,
K.
,
Dhond
,
R. P.
,
Dale
,
A. M.
,
Glessner
,
M.
,
Carr
,
V.
, &
Halgren
,
E.
(
2003
).
Spatiotemporal dynamics of modality-specific and supramodal word processing
.
Neuron
,
38
,
487
497
.
Martin
,
A.
(
2007
).
The representation of object concepts in the brain
.
Annual Review of Psychology
,
58
,
25
45
.
Miller
,
B. L.
,
Darby
,
A.
,
Benson
,
D. F.
,
Cummings
,
J. L.
, &
Miller
,
M. H.
(
1997
).
Aggressive, socially disruptive and antisocial behaviour associated with fronto-temporal dementia
.
British Journal of Psychiatry
,
170
,
150
154
.
Moll
,
J.
,
Zahn
,
R.
,
de Oliveira-Souza
,
R.
,
Krueger
,
F.
, &
Grafman
,
J.
(
2005
).
The neural basis of human moral cognition
.
Nature Reviews Neuroscience
,
6
,
799
809
.
Moran
,
M. A.
,
Mufson
,
E. J.
, &
Mesulam
,
M. M.
(
1987
).
Neural inputs into the temporopolar cortex of the rhesus monkey
.
Journal of Comparative Neurology
,
256
,
88
103
.
Neary
,
D.
,
Snowden
,
J. S.
,
Gustafson
,
L.
,
Passant
,
U.
,
Stuss
,
D.
,
Black
,
S.
, et al
(
1998
).
Frontotemporal lobar degeneration: A consensus on clinical diagnostic criteria
.
Neurology
,
51
,
1546
1554
.
Neubert
,
F. X.
,
Mars
,
R. B.
,
Buch
,
E. R.
,
Olivier
,
E.
, &
Rushworth
,
M. F.
(
2010
).
Cortical and subcortical interactions during action reprogramming and their related white matter pathways
.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A.
,
107
,
13240
13245
.
Oldfield
,
R. C.
(
1971
).
The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory
.
Neuropsycholgia
,
9
,
97
113
.
Parker
,
G. J.
,
Luzzi
,
S.
,
Alexander
,
D. C.
,
Wheeler-Kingshott
,
C. A.
,
Ciccarelli
,
O.
, &
Lambon Ralph
,
M. A.
(
2005
).
Lateralization of ventral and dorsal auditory-language pathways in the human brain
.
Neuroimage
,
24
,
656
666
.
Pascual
,
B.
,
Masdeu
,
J. C.
,
Hollenbeck
,
M.
,
Makris
,
N.
,
Insausti
,
R.
,
Ding
,
S. L.
, et al
(
2014
).
Large-scale brain networks of the human left temporal pole: A functional connectivity MRI study
.
Cerebral Cortex
,
34
,
14864
14873
.
Patterson
,
K.
,
Nestor
,
P. J.
, &
Rogers
,
T. T.
(
2007
).
Where do you know what you know? The representation of semantic knowledge in the human brain
.
Nature Reviews Neuroscience
,
8
,
976
987
.
Pobric
,
G.
,
Jefferies
,
E.
, &
Lambon Ralph
,
M. A.
(
2007
).
Anterior temporal lobes mediate semantic representation: Mimicking semantic dementia by using rTMS in normal participants
.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A.
,
104
,
20137
20141
.
Pobric
,
G.
,
Jefferies
,
E.
, &
Lambon Ralph
,
M. A.
(
2010
).
Category-specific vs. category-general semantic impairment induced by transcranial magnetic stimulation
.
Current Biology
,
20
,
964
968
.
Quentin
,
R.
,
Chanes
,
L.
,
Migliaccio
,
R.
,
Valabrègue
,
R.
, &
Valero-Cabré
,
A.
(
2013
).
Fronto-tectal white matter connectivity mediates facilitatory effects of non-invasive neurostimulation on visual detection
.
Neuroimage
,
82
,
344
354
.
Rankin
,
K. P.
,
Gorno-Tempini
,
M. L.
,
Allison
,
S. C.
,
Stanley
,
C. M.
,
Glenn
,
S.
,
Weiner
,
M. W.
, et al
(
2006
).
Structural anatomy of empathy in neurodegenerative disease
.
Brain
,
129
,
2945
2956
.
Rice
,
G. E.
,
Lambon Ralph
,
M. A.
, &
Hoffman
,
P.
(
2015
).
The roles of left versus right anterior temporal lobes in conceptual knowledge: An ALE meta-analysis of 97 functional neuroimaging studies
.
Cerebral Cortex
,
25
,
4374
4391
.
Rogers
,
T. T.
, &
McClelland
,
J. L.
(
2004
).
Semantic cognition: A parallel distributed processing approach
.
Cambridge, MA
:
MIT Press
.
Rosen
,
H. J.
,
Allison
,
S. C.
,
Schauer
,
G. F.
,
Gorno-Tempini
,
M. L.
,
Weiner
,
M. W.
, &
Miller
,
B. L.
(
2005
).
Neuroanatomical correlates of behavioural disorders in dementia
.
Brain
,
128
,
2612
2625
.
Ross
,
L. A.
, &
Olson
,
I. R.
(
2010
).
Social cognition and anterior temporal poles
.
Neuroimage
,
49
,
3452
3462
.
Sabsevitz
,
D. S.
,
Medler
,
D. A.
,
Seidenberg
,
M.
, &
Binder
,
J. R.
(
2005
).
Modulation of the semantic system by word imageability
.
Neuroimage
,
27
,
188
200
.
Schapiro
,
A. C.
,
McClelland
,
J. L.
,
Welbourne
,
S. R.
,
Rogers
,
T. T.
, &
Lambon Ralph
,
M. A.
(
2013
).
Why bilateral damage is worse than unilateral damage to the brain
.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
,
12
,
107
123
.
Sharp
,
D. J.
,
Scott
,
S. K.
, &
Wise
,
R. J.
(
2004
).
Retrieving meaning after temporal lobe infarction: The role of the basal language area
.
Annals of Neurology
,
56
,
836
846
.
Simmons
,
W. K.
,
Reddish
,
M.
,
Bellgowan
,
P. S.
, &
Martin
,
A.
(
2010
).
The selectivity and functional connectivity of the anterior temporal lobes
.
Cerebral Cortex
,
20
,
813
825
.
Skipper
,
L. M.
,
Ross
,
L. A.
, &
Olson
,
I. R.
(
2011
).
The sensory and categorical topography of the anterior temporal lobe
.
Neuropsychologia
,
49
,
3419
3429
.
Snowden
,
J. S.
,
Goulding
,
P. J.
, &
Neary
,
D.
(
1989
).
Semantic dementia: A form of circumscribed cerebral atrophy
.
Behavioural Neurology
,
2
,
167
182
.
Snowden
,
J. S.
,
Thompson
,
J. C.
,
Stopford
,
C. L.
,
Richardson
,
A. M.
,
Gerhard
,
A.
,
Neary
,
D.
, et al
(
2011
).
The clinical diagnosis of early-onset dementias: Diagnostic accuracy and clinicopathological relationships
.
Brain
,
134
,
2478
2492
.
Spitsyna
,
G.
,
Warren
,
J. E.
,
Scott
,
S. K.
,
Turkheimer
,
F. E.
, &
Wise
,
R. J. S.
(
2006
).
Converging language streams in the human temporal lobe
.
Journal of Neuroscience
,
26
,
7328
7336
.
Straube
,
B.
,
Green
,
A.
,
Jansen
,
A.
,
Chatterjee
,
A.
, &
Kircher
,
T.
(
2010
).
Social cues, mentalizing and the neural processing of speech accompanied by gestures
.
Neuropsychologia
,
48
,
382
393
.
Vandenberghe
,
R.
,
Price
,
C.
,
Wise
,
R.
,
Josephs
,
O.
, &
Frackowiak
,
R. S. J.
(
1996
).
Functional anatomy of a common semantic system for words and pictures
.
Nature
,
383
,
254
256
.
Visser
,
M.
,
Embleton
,
K. V.
,
Jefferies
,
E.
,
Parker
,
G. J.
, &
Lambon Ralph
,
M. A.
(
2010
).
The inferior, anterior temporal lobes and semantic memory clarified: Novel evidence from distortion-corrected fMRI
.
Neuropsychologia
,
48
,
1689
1696
.
Visser
,
M.
, &
Lambon Ralph
,
M. A.
(
2011
).
Differential contributions of bilateral ventral anterior temporal lobe and left anterior superior temporal gyrus to semantic processes
.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
,
23
,
3121
3131
.
Walsh
,
V.
, &
Cowey
,
A.
(
2000
).
Transcranial magnetic stimulation and cognitive neuroscience
.
Nature Reviews Neuroscience
,
1
,
73
79
.
Wise
,
R. J.
(
2003
).
Language systems in normal and aphasic human subjects: Functional imaging studies and inferences from animal studies
.
British Medical Bulletin
,
65
,
95
119
.
Zahn
,
R.
,
Moll
,
J.
,
Iyengar
,
V.
,
Huey
,
E. D.
,
Tierney
,
M.
,
Krueger
,
F.
, et al
(
2009
).
Social conceptual impairments in frontotemporal lobar degeneration with right anterior temporal hypometabolism
.
Brain
,
132
,
604
616
.
Zahn
,
R.
,
Moll
,
J.
,
Krueger
,
F.
,
Huey
,
E. D.
,
Garrido
,
G.
, &
Grafman
,
J.
(
2007
).
Social concepts are represented in the superior anterior temporal cortex
.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A.
,
104
,
6430
6435
.
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. For a full description of the license, please visit https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/legalcode.