Abstract
The differential contribution of medial-temporal lobe regions to verbal declarative memory is debated within the neuroscience, neuropsychology, and cognitive psychology communities. We evaluate whether the extent of surgical resection within medial-temporal regions predicts longitudinal verbal learning and memory outcomes. This single-center retrospective observational study involved patients with refractory temporal lobe epilepsy undergoing unilateral anterior temporal lobe resection from 2007 to 2015. Thirty-two participants with Engel Class 1 and 2 outcomes were included (14 left, 18 right) and followed for a mean of 2.3 years after surgery (±1.5 years). Participants had baseline and postsurgical neuropsychological testing and high-resolution T1-weighted MRI scans. Postsurgical lesions were manually traced and coregistered to presurgical scans to precisely quantify resection extent of medial-temporal regions. Verbal learning and memory change scores were regressed on hippocampal, entorhinal, and parahippocampal resection volume after accounting for baseline performance. Overall, there were no significant differences in learning and memory change between patients who received left and right anterior temporal lobe resection. After controlling for baseline performance, the extent of left parahippocampal resection accounted for 27% (p = .021) of the variance in verbal short delay free recall. The extent of left entorhinal resection accounted for 37% (p = .004) of the variance in verbal short delay free recall. Our findings highlight the critical role that the left parahippocampal and entorhinal regions play in recall for verbal material.
INTRODUCTION
The differential role of medial-temporal lobe regions in verbal declarative learning and memory is the subject of ongoing debate. Declarative memory refers to the conscious learning of facts and events (Squire & Zola, 1996; Squire, 1992). It has been clear since the early days of anteromesial temporal lobe epilepsy surgery in humans that the hippocampus is critical for initial formation of declarative memories (Scoville & Milner, 1957). Primate studies reveal that lesions involving the hippocampal, parahippocampal, and perirhinal cortex produce worse memory impairment than lesions involving the hippocampal formation alone (Zola-Morgan, Squire, Clower, & Rempel, 1993; Zola-Morgan, Squire, Amaral, & Suzuki, 1989). Likewise, larger medial-temporal lesions, including entorhinal and parahippocampal cortex, more consistently lead to impairment in delayed nonmatching to sample tasks compared with hippocampal-only lesions (Malkova & Mishkin, 2003; Gaffan, 1994; Murray, Gaffan, & Mishkin, 1993; Zola-Morgan et al., 1993).
Patients undergoing epilepsy surgery provide a means to investigate the mnemonic contributions of different medial-temporal subregions, as these regions may be surgically removed to a varying degree if implicated in the seizure network. For patients with medication-refractory epilepsy, the best option for achieving seizure freedom is surgical resection, with 30–80% of patients attaining this outcome (Tellez-Zenteno, Dhar, & Wiebe, 2005). For patients with temporal lobe epilepsy, rates of seizure freedom following unilateral anteromesial temporal lobe resection (ATLR) are higher, between 66% and 70% (Spencer & Huh, 2008). In a recent meta-analysis, seizure outcomes following standard ATLR are superior to selective amygdalohippocampectomy (Josephson et al., 2013).
However, negative consequences such as memory impairment are also common (Sherman et al., 2011). Memory subtypes appear highly lateralized, as impairment across a variety of episodic memory tasks has been demonstrated with left ATLR, but not right ATLR (Barr, Goldberg, Wasserstein, & Novelly, 1990). Between 22% and 63% of individuals who undergo unilateral left ATLR experience decline in verbal memory (Sherman et al., 2011), though there is a considerable amount of variability in degree of verbal memory change (Hermann, Seidenberg, Haltiner, & Wyler, 1995).
Patients who undergo larger unilateral temporal lobe resections perform worse on an verbal associative learning and recall tasks than patients who had smaller surgeries (Helmstaedter, Petzold, & Bien, 2011; Helmstaedter, Roeske, Kaaden, Elger, & Schramm, 2011), suggesting that medial-temporal regions other than the hippocampus support verbal declarative memory formation. There is a strong correlation between 18F-fluorodeoxyglucose uptake in the left perirhinal cortex and learning of arbitrary word pairs in patients with left temporal lobe epilepsy (Weintrob, Saling, Berkovic, Berlangieri, & Reutens, 2002). Similarly, the structural integrity of the left perirhinal region, as measured by MRI T2 relaxation time, is associated with learning of arbitrary relational verbal material, whereas consolidation of the material is related to hippocampal integrity (Lillywhite et al., 2007). Furthermore, regional blood flow measured by PET [15O]H2O during a task involving unrelated verbal word pairs implicates a predominantly left-sided network including dorsolateral pFC, fusiform, parahippocampal, perirhinal cortices, and posterior cingulate structures (Weintrob, 2004). Saling has proposed a two-part model for verbal memory involving the left temporal lobe: (1) a mesial protosemantic component responsible for arbitrary paired associated learning and (2) a lateral semantic component when semantic meaning is required (e.g., during related paired associates or passage learning; Saling, 2009). Furthermore, Fernández and Tendolkar (2006) propose that rhinal cortex acts as a “gatekeeper” to the hippocampal formation, with the semantic status of the incoming information determining probability of transfer to the hippocampus (Fernández & Tendolkar, 2006).
Variance in neurosurgical practice and cognitive outcomes in the epilepsy surgical population provides a unique opportunity to understand the respective contributions of the entorhinal and parahippocampal regions to the formation and retention of new verbal memory traces in humans. Here, we introduce a novel approach for measuring the extent of entorhinal and parahippocampal resection in a case series of patients who underwent ATLR. We segment the medial-temporal neocortical architecture of each individual participant on his or her high-resolution presurgical MRI scan and then coregister his or her postsurgical lesion mask to this native neuroanatomical map. This method allows for precise quantification of resection extent in medial-temporal lobe subregions and investigation of their particular contributions to longitudinal verbal declarative memory outcomes.
METHODS
This was a retrospective observational study conducted at New York University (NYU) Comprehensive Epilepsy Center at the NYU School of Medicine using data collected from 2007 to 2015. This study was approved by the NYU School of Medicine institutional review board. All patients consented to participate in research MRI scanning and neuropsychological testing and to have their clinical records reviewed. All patients were adults diagnosed with medication-refractory focal epilepsy (defined as failure of at least two appropriately chosen and dosed antiepileptic medications) and were considered appropriate candidates for temporal lobe resection.
Clinical Variables
Participants' clinical data such as seizure type and frequency, age of seizure onset, duration of epilepsy, estimated lifetime number complex partial and secondarily generalized tonic-clonic seizures, and number of current antiepileptic medications were extracted from clinical records. The presence or absence of mesial temporal sclerosis was diagnosed using MRI criteria (i.e., hippocampal volume loss and increased T2 signal on T2-weighted imaging) and histopathological evaluation of resected surgical tissue. The intracarotid sodium amobarbital procedure (also known as the “Wada test”) variables included language lateralization (left/right/bilateral) and memory scores for the right and left hemisphere (discrimination of 12 target objects and pictures from 24 foils). Patients were followed clinically for at least 1 year to assess postsurgical clinical outcomes. Seizure type and frequency were used to determine their Engel class outcome (Engel 1 = seizure free, Engel 2 = rare disabling seizures, Engel 3 = worthwhile reduction in seizure frequency, Engel 4 = no worthwhile improvement; 1992 Palm Desert Conference on Epilepsy Surgery).
Participant Population
There were 34 participants initially enrolled in this study, including 15 who underwent a left ATLR and 19 who underwent a right ATLR. Two participants were excluded from analyses because of poor postsurgical seizure outcomes (Engel class 3–4). The remaining group of 32 participants had a mean age of 35.5 years (range = 16–63 years) at time of surgery and mean postoperative follow-up of 2.41 years (SD = 1.45 years; Table 1). Mean age of seizure onset was 15.1 years (range = 1–47 years), with a mean duration of seizures of 18.9 years (SD = 14.2). About 41% of the cohort had mesial temporal sclerosis identified by pathology. Mean educational level was 15.0 years (SD = 2.8). At presurgical baseline testing, participants had an average IQ of 99.0 (SD = 13.8). As a group, patients had low depression (mean BDI = 9.3, SD = 5.7) and anxiety (mean = 11.3, SD = 11.4) scores. There were no differences in baseline or postsurgical demographic, clinical, or neuropsychological characteristics of patients who underwent a left- or right-sided ATLR (Table 1), except that patients who received a left ATLR showed a trend toward a lower left-sided Wada memory score at baseline (7.6 vs. 10.2, p = .055), as well as lower postsurgical verbal memory and naming scores, compared with patients who received a right-sided resection.
. | All Participants . | Left ATLR . | Right ATLR . | p . |
---|---|---|---|---|
Demographic and Clinical Characteristics | ||||
Number of participants | N = 32 | N = 14 | N = 18 | |
Age at surgery (years, range) | 35.53 (16–63) | 36.57 ± 9.35 | 34.72 ± 13.54 | .666 |
Sex (male/female) | 17/15 | 8/6 | 9/9 | .688 |
Education (years) | 15.0 ± 2.77 | 15.00 ± 2.08 | 15.00 ± 3.12 | 1.00 |
Ethnicity | ||||
White | 23 (71.9%) | 10 | 13 | .735 (Fisher's exact) |
Black | 4 (12.5%) | 2 | 2 | |
Hispanic | 4 (12.5%) | 2 | 2 | |
Asian | 1 (3.1%) | 0 | 1 | |
Handedness (right/left/ambidextrous) | 23/8/1 | 10/4/0 | 13/4/1 | .636 |
Age at seizure onset (years) | 15.06 (1–47) | 15.71 ± 10.36 | 14.56 ± 11.24 | .767 |
Duration of seizures (years) | 18.88 ± 14.18 | 18.93 ± 13.34 | 18.83 ± 15.18 | .985 |
Years of follow-up after surgery (years) | 2.41 ± 1.45 | 2.51 ± 1.46 | 2.34 ± 1.50 | .763 |
Mesial temporal sclerosis | 13/17 (40.6%/53.1%) | 4/9 | 9/8 | .283 |
Engel class outcome | ||||
Class 1 | 27 (84.4%) | 11 | 16 | .631 |
Class 2 | 5 (15.6%) | 3 | 2 | |
Baseline Neuropsychological Testing | ||||
FSIQ | 99.04 ± 13.76 | 98.83 ± 13.99 | 99.19 ± 14.05 | .948 |
Verbal Comprehension Index | 100.5 ± 19.82 | 94.36 ± 18.98 | 105.28 ± 19.64 | .124 |
Perceptual Reasoning Index | 98.84 ± 15.39 | 101.14 ± 17.77 | 97.06 ± 13.51 | .465 |
Working Memory Index | 93.78 ± 18.64 | 99.50 ± 10.06 | 89.50 ± 22.49 | .164 |
Processing Speed Index | 93.72 ± 19.71 | 97.75 ± 10.91 | 90.88 ± 24.02 | .365 |
Verbal Learning | 46.81 ± 12.89 | 45.14 ± 14.69 | 46.33 ± 9.94 | .404 |
Verbal SDFR | 8.85 ± 3.53 | 8.21 ± 3.42 | 8.58 ± 3.58 | .190 |
Verbal LDFR | 9.42 ± 3.67 | 8.71 ± 3.47 | 9.58 ± 3.42 | .346 |
Visual Confrontation Naming (BNT) | 50.20 ± 7.30 | 48.93 ± 6.96 | 51.31 ± 7.64 | .382 |
Visual Memory (RCFT delay trial) | 12.90 ± 6.48 | 14.00 ± 7.47 | 12.17 ± 5.84 | .458 |
BDI | 9.26 ± 5.73 | 9.15 ± 5.90 | 9.36 ± 5.79 | .929 |
BAI | 11.32 ± 11.35 | 12.07 ± 12.69 | 10.57 ± 10.26 | .734 |
Left Wada memory score | 9.03 ± 3.52 | 7.64 ± 3.97 | 10.18± 2.72 | .055 |
Right Wada memory score | 7.32 ± 3.32 | 8.29 ± 2.64 | 6.47 ± 3.70 | .138 |
Wada language (left/right/bilateral) | 30/0/1 | 13/0/1 | 17/0/0 | .452 (Fisher's exact) |
Postsurgical Neuropsychological Testing | ||||
FSIQ | 99.61 ± 14.61 | 99.25 ± 11.35 | 99.88 ± 17.02 | .913 |
Verbal Comprehension Index | 103.93 ± 15.02 | 101.00 ± 9.83 | 106.00 ± 17.82 | .387 |
Perceptual Reasoning Index | 99.45 ± 13.58 | 101.33 ± 11.74 | 98.12 ± 14.95 | .540 |
Working Memory Index | 100.56 ± 14.76 | 103.45 ± 13.78 | 98.56 ± 15.50 | .408 |
Processing Speed Index | 97.18 ± 14.01 | 96.42 ± 14.79 | 97.75 ± 13.87 | .809 |
Verbal Learning | 42.56 ± 10.81 | 39.08 ± 10.77 | 46.33 ± 9.94 | .094 |
Verbal SDFR | 6.92 ± 3.67 | 5.38 ± 3.15 | 8.58 ± 3.58 | .026 |
Verbal LDFR | 7.60 ±4.26 | 5.77 ± 4.30 | 9.58 ± 3.34 | .022 |
Visual Confrontation Naming (BNT) | 48.30 ± 8.81 | 44.36 ± 9.52 | 52.54 ± 5.70 | .012 |
Visual Memory (RCFT delay trial) | 10.97 ± 5.17 | 12.69 ± 5.66 | 9.56 ± 4.43 | .106 |
BDI | 8.13 ± 7.32 | 9.36 ± 8.23 | 7.12 ± 6.56 | .406 |
BAI | 6.84 ± 6.40 | 8.29 ± 7.97 | 5.64 ± 4.67 | .260 |
. | All Participants . | Left ATLR . | Right ATLR . | p . |
---|---|---|---|---|
Demographic and Clinical Characteristics | ||||
Number of participants | N = 32 | N = 14 | N = 18 | |
Age at surgery (years, range) | 35.53 (16–63) | 36.57 ± 9.35 | 34.72 ± 13.54 | .666 |
Sex (male/female) | 17/15 | 8/6 | 9/9 | .688 |
Education (years) | 15.0 ± 2.77 | 15.00 ± 2.08 | 15.00 ± 3.12 | 1.00 |
Ethnicity | ||||
White | 23 (71.9%) | 10 | 13 | .735 (Fisher's exact) |
Black | 4 (12.5%) | 2 | 2 | |
Hispanic | 4 (12.5%) | 2 | 2 | |
Asian | 1 (3.1%) | 0 | 1 | |
Handedness (right/left/ambidextrous) | 23/8/1 | 10/4/0 | 13/4/1 | .636 |
Age at seizure onset (years) | 15.06 (1–47) | 15.71 ± 10.36 | 14.56 ± 11.24 | .767 |
Duration of seizures (years) | 18.88 ± 14.18 | 18.93 ± 13.34 | 18.83 ± 15.18 | .985 |
Years of follow-up after surgery (years) | 2.41 ± 1.45 | 2.51 ± 1.46 | 2.34 ± 1.50 | .763 |
Mesial temporal sclerosis | 13/17 (40.6%/53.1%) | 4/9 | 9/8 | .283 |
Engel class outcome | ||||
Class 1 | 27 (84.4%) | 11 | 16 | .631 |
Class 2 | 5 (15.6%) | 3 | 2 | |
Baseline Neuropsychological Testing | ||||
FSIQ | 99.04 ± 13.76 | 98.83 ± 13.99 | 99.19 ± 14.05 | .948 |
Verbal Comprehension Index | 100.5 ± 19.82 | 94.36 ± 18.98 | 105.28 ± 19.64 | .124 |
Perceptual Reasoning Index | 98.84 ± 15.39 | 101.14 ± 17.77 | 97.06 ± 13.51 | .465 |
Working Memory Index | 93.78 ± 18.64 | 99.50 ± 10.06 | 89.50 ± 22.49 | .164 |
Processing Speed Index | 93.72 ± 19.71 | 97.75 ± 10.91 | 90.88 ± 24.02 | .365 |
Verbal Learning | 46.81 ± 12.89 | 45.14 ± 14.69 | 46.33 ± 9.94 | .404 |
Verbal SDFR | 8.85 ± 3.53 | 8.21 ± 3.42 | 8.58 ± 3.58 | .190 |
Verbal LDFR | 9.42 ± 3.67 | 8.71 ± 3.47 | 9.58 ± 3.42 | .346 |
Visual Confrontation Naming (BNT) | 50.20 ± 7.30 | 48.93 ± 6.96 | 51.31 ± 7.64 | .382 |
Visual Memory (RCFT delay trial) | 12.90 ± 6.48 | 14.00 ± 7.47 | 12.17 ± 5.84 | .458 |
BDI | 9.26 ± 5.73 | 9.15 ± 5.90 | 9.36 ± 5.79 | .929 |
BAI | 11.32 ± 11.35 | 12.07 ± 12.69 | 10.57 ± 10.26 | .734 |
Left Wada memory score | 9.03 ± 3.52 | 7.64 ± 3.97 | 10.18± 2.72 | .055 |
Right Wada memory score | 7.32 ± 3.32 | 8.29 ± 2.64 | 6.47 ± 3.70 | .138 |
Wada language (left/right/bilateral) | 30/0/1 | 13/0/1 | 17/0/0 | .452 (Fisher's exact) |
Postsurgical Neuropsychological Testing | ||||
FSIQ | 99.61 ± 14.61 | 99.25 ± 11.35 | 99.88 ± 17.02 | .913 |
Verbal Comprehension Index | 103.93 ± 15.02 | 101.00 ± 9.83 | 106.00 ± 17.82 | .387 |
Perceptual Reasoning Index | 99.45 ± 13.58 | 101.33 ± 11.74 | 98.12 ± 14.95 | .540 |
Working Memory Index | 100.56 ± 14.76 | 103.45 ± 13.78 | 98.56 ± 15.50 | .408 |
Processing Speed Index | 97.18 ± 14.01 | 96.42 ± 14.79 | 97.75 ± 13.87 | .809 |
Verbal Learning | 42.56 ± 10.81 | 39.08 ± 10.77 | 46.33 ± 9.94 | .094 |
Verbal SDFR | 6.92 ± 3.67 | 5.38 ± 3.15 | 8.58 ± 3.58 | .026 |
Verbal LDFR | 7.60 ±4.26 | 5.77 ± 4.30 | 9.58 ± 3.34 | .022 |
Visual Confrontation Naming (BNT) | 48.30 ± 8.81 | 44.36 ± 9.52 | 52.54 ± 5.70 | .012 |
Visual Memory (RCFT delay trial) | 10.97 ± 5.17 | 12.69 ± 5.66 | 9.56 ± 4.43 | .106 |
BDI | 8.13 ± 7.32 | 9.36 ± 8.23 | 7.12 ± 6.56 | .406 |
BAI | 6.84 ± 6.40 | 8.29 ± 7.97 | 5.64 ± 4.67 | .260 |
Independent samples t test comparison performed with p values listed.
Significant after correction for multiple comparisons.
Neuropsychological Testing
Presurgical comprehensive neuropsychological testing was conducted as part of routine clinical practice. Postsurgical neuropsychological testing was acquired for research (at least 6 months following surgery) and included the same test battery administered presurgically. The neuropsychological test battery adhered to the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke common data elements protocol for epilepsy (Loring et al., 2011), with the exception of the California Verbal Learning Test-2nd Edition (CVLT-II), which was administered to patients before 2011. The Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Third or Fourth Edition (WAIS-III or WAIS-IV) was administered to obtain the Full-Scale Intellectual Quotient (FSIQ). Participants completed standard questionnaires of mood functioning, including the Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI; Beck & Steer, 1993) and Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II; Beck, Steer, & Brown, 1996). Measurements of global cognitive functioning (FSIQ) and mood (BAI, BDI-II) were used to determine whether changes in these variables contributed to memory decline between pre- and postsurgical time points.
Verbal declarative memory was assessed with serial word list-learning tasks (Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test or the CVLT-II; Taylor, 1959). On both of these list-learning tasks, examinees are asked to learn a list of words (List A) across a series of five learning trials, given a second distraction word list (List B), and then they are asked to freely recall words from List A. Free recall of the List A words are tested after a brief (2 min) and long (20 min) delay. The total score across all five learning trials was used as a measure of verbal learning; the short delay free recall (SDFR) score represents verbal free recall after 2 min and the long delay free recall (LDFR) score represents verbal free recall after 20 min (Taylor, 1959). Raw scores across these two list-learning tasks are highly correlated in patients with closed head injuries whereas normalized scores differ (Stallings, Boake, & Sherer, 1995); therefore, raw scores were utilized for this study. Within an individual, the same list-learning task was administered pre- and postsurgically. Longitudinal change in verbal declarative learning and memory performance was assessed by subtracting the postsurgical test score from the presurgical test score. Negative scores indicate decline in performance, and positive scores indicate improvement. The following dependent variables were used in subsequent analyses of longitudinal change in neuropsychological performance: Change in Verbal Learning, Verbal SDFR (SDFR-change), and Verbal LDFR (LDFR-change).
To test the specificity of findings to verbal declarative memory, we calculated change scores for the Boston Naming Test (BNT), a measure of visual confrontation naming, and the Rey Complex Figure Test (RCFT)-Delayed Recall Trial, a measure of memory for visual material. Finally, we calculated change scores for the BAI and BDI to ensure that changes in verbal declarative memory were not associated with changes in mood or anxiety.
MRI Scanning Protocol
Before resective epilepsy surgery, all patients completed a high-resolution T1-weighted MPRAGE (echo time = 3.25 msec, repetition time = 2530 msec, inversion time = 1100 msec, flip angle = 7°, field of view = 256 mm, matrix = 2566256, voxel size = 1 × 1 × 1.3 mm) on a Siemens (Erlangen, Germany) Allegra 3T research-dedicated MR scanner. Immediately following resective surgery (i.e., within 24 hr), a clinical MRI scanning protocol was acquired at the NYU Department of Radiology on 1.5-T and 3-T Siemens scanners. In many patients, an additional postsurgical MRI scan was acquired at a second time point more remote to surgery (i.e., more than 6 months to 1 year after surgery); however, in these remote MRI scans there was evidence of brain shift, which tended to increase error in the coregistration of postsurgical to presurgical MRI scans. Error associated with brain shift over time was greater than error associated with immediate postsurgical brain swelling; therefore, in all cases the MRI scan that was acquired proximal to surgery was utilized. Postsurgical image acquisitions were used for manual tracing of the surgical lesion and included a high-resolution T1-weighted volume (MPRAGE) and a T2-weighted FLAIR volume, which was used to improve visualization of postsurgical lesion extent.
Quantifying Surgical Lesion Extent
Surgical lesion extent for specific temporal lobe subregions was quantified by (1) anatomical labeling of the high-resolution presurgical MRI scan (see below), (2) slice-by-slice manual tracing of lesion extent on the postsurgical MRI, (3) rigid body coregistration of the postsurgical MRI lesion mask to the native presurgical MRI scan, and (4) quantification of the percentage of each labeled subregion that was surgically resected. By coregistering the postsurgical lesion mask to an individual's presurgical MRI rather than a standard neuroanatomical atlas, this procedure preserves native architectural features and allows for precise delineation of subregion lesion extent. These steps are outlined in more detail below.
Neuroanatomical Labeling of Subcortical Structures and Cortical Regions on Presurgical Scans
Subcortical Segmentation and Labeling
Presurgical images were processed with FreeSurfer 5.1 software package (surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu). Segmentation and labeling of subcortical structures (see Figure 1) was performed by rigid body alignment of the participant's brain to a probabilistic atlas, followed by nonlinear morphing to the atlas. Labels were generated based on the prior probability of a given tissue class occurring at a specific atlas location, the likelihood of the image intensity given that tissue class, and the probability of the local spatial configuration of labels given the tissue class (Fischl et al., 2002). Labels were manually inspected and, in rare cases, corrected if necessary. ROIs for the current study include the hippocampus, entorhinal, and parahippocampal regions.
Cortical Surface Reconstruction, Segmentation, and Labeling
Freesurfer 5.1 was used to reconstruct, segment, and label the cortical sheet. These procedures are described in detail in prior publications (Fischl, Sereno, & Dale, 1999; Fischl et al., 2002; Dale, Fischl, & Sereno, 1999). In brief, processing steps included segmentation of gray matter and white matter, inflation and flattening of the cortical surface, and automatic correction of topological defects. First, intensity variations due to magnetic field inhomogeneities are corrected; a normalized intensity image is created; and the skull is removed from the high-resolution, T1-weighted, anatomical MR image. The cortical gray matter ribbon is segmented, and the resulting surface is covered with a polygonal tessellation and smoothed to reduce metric distortions. After constructing the initial surface model, a refinement procedure delineates the gray/white interface. This surface is subsequently deformed outwards to obtain an explicit representation of the pial surface. Then, an automated method for subdividing the human cerebral cortex into standard gyral-based neuroanatomical regions is applied (Desikan et al., 2006). This method results in the neuroanatomic labeling of 34 cortical regions (see Figure 1B) and corresponding white matter regions. The following ROIs were analyzed given that they are typically included (to varying degrees) in a standard ATLR: entorhinal, fusiform gray matter, parahippocampal, inferior temporal lobe, middle temporal lobe, superior temporal lobe, temporal pole.
Manual Lesion Tracing and Estimation of Resection Percentages
Before lesion tracing, the postoperative T1-weighted and FLAIR images were coregistered to the preoperative T1-weighted image using rigid body linear transformation algorithms. Surgical lesion masks were manually traced on the postsurgical T1-weighted scan, with the T2-weighted FLAIR image used to verify lesion boundaries (see Figure 1C–D). Lesion masks were crosschecked across the coronal, axial, and sagittal planes of the T1-weighted image. Each postoperative slice was visually reviewed against the presurgical segmentations, and if there was evidence of brain shift, then the masks were manually adjusted accordingly. These resection masks were then overlaid onto the neuroanatomical division maps of the individual patient to calculate the percentage of the subcortical and cortical regions that were removed (see Figure 1E–F). Total resection extent was calculated as a percentage of the total supratentorial brain volume. The hippocampus and amygdala resection extent was quantified as a percentage of total preoperative hippocampus and amygdala volume removed for each patient. Resection extent for entorhinal, parahippocampal, fusiform, temporal pole, superior temporal, middle temporal, and inferior temporal gray and white matter regions was quantified as a percentage of total preoperative area removed for each patient.
Statistical Analysis
Statistical analysis was performed with SPSS Version 22.0 (SPSS, Inc., Chicago, IL). Descriptive statistics, including frequencies, means, and standard deviations, were calculated. An independent samples t test with a p value of .05 was used to determine differences for all continuous variables such as clinical, demographic, and baseline neuropsychological testing by side of surgery. For comparison between categorical variables such as Engel class outcome and side of surgery, a Fisher's exact test was used. For analysis of longitudinal neuropsychological outcomes, only Engel Classes 1 and 2 outcomes were included to avoid confounding poor seizure control or impact of medication changes with cognitive performance. A repeated-measures ANOVA with Lesion group (right/left) as the between-subject factor and Time point (pre/post) as the within-subject factor was conducted, with performance on verbal memory tests as the dependent variables. Regression analyses were performed to examine the relationship between resection extent of the medial-temporal subregions (hippocampus, entorhinal cortex, and parahippocampal regions) and change in verbal learning and memory scores, after accounting for baseline performance. Given the strong established relationship between verbal memory decline and left-sided resection described in the literature, we performed separate regression analyses in the left and right ATLR groups. To account for multiple comparisons, Benjamini–Hochberg correction for false discovery rate was applied.
RESULTS
Surgical Resection
The extent of surgical resection was quantified for total resection volume, percentage of total brain volume resected, as well as for each temporal lobe substructure, and described in more detail in Table 2. There was a significant difference in the total resection volume and the percentage of total brain volume resected between patients who received left ATLR versus right ATLR (p < .001). There was a wide degree of variance of medial-temporal structures resected (see Table 2). Patients who underwent a left ATLR had significantly decreased extent of inferior, mid, and superior temporal neocortical and subcortical tissue resected compared with right ATLR (p < .001). Patients who underwent a right ATLR had slightly less hippocampus resected (left 0.84, SD = 0.07 vs. right 0.91, SD = 0.09; p = .014). Other resection extent percentages were statistically similar.
Region . | Range . | Left ATLR (n = 14)a . | Right ATLR (n = 18)a . | pb . |
---|---|---|---|---|
Total resection volume (mm3) | 17319–66955 | 29139 ± 9103 | 46165 ± 11938 | <.001* |
Percentage of total brain volume resected | 0.02–0.07 | 0.03 ± 0.01 | 0.04 ± 0.01 | <.001* |
Hippocampal (gray matter) | 0.64–1.00 | 0.84 ± 0.07 | 0.91 ± 0.09 | .014* |
Entorhinal (gray and white matter) | 0.32–0.97 | 0.84 ± 0.20 | 0.89 ± 0.10 | .394 |
Parahippocampal (gray and white matter) | 0–0.97 | 0.62 ± 0.28 | 0.64 ± 0.24 | .507 |
Fusiform (gray and white matter) | 0.08–0.46 | 0.19 ± 0.06 | 0.27 ± 0.09 | .009* |
Temporal pole (gray and white matter) | 0.49–1.00 | 0.89 ± 0.13 | 0.94 ± 0.06 | .166 |
Inferior temporal (gray and white matter) | 0.08–0.67 | 0.20 ± 0.11 | 0.40 ± 0.13 | <.001* |
Mid temporal (gray and white matter) | 0.04–0.61 | 0.14 ± 0.10 | 0.35 ± 0.15 | <.001* |
Superior temporal (gray and white matter) | 0.02–0.79 | 0.08 ± 0.07 | 0.37 ± 0.15 | <.001* |
Region . | Range . | Left ATLR (n = 14)a . | Right ATLR (n = 18)a . | pb . |
---|---|---|---|---|
Total resection volume (mm3) | 17319–66955 | 29139 ± 9103 | 46165 ± 11938 | <.001* |
Percentage of total brain volume resected | 0.02–0.07 | 0.03 ± 0.01 | 0.04 ± 0.01 | <.001* |
Hippocampal (gray matter) | 0.64–1.00 | 0.84 ± 0.07 | 0.91 ± 0.09 | .014* |
Entorhinal (gray and white matter) | 0.32–0.97 | 0.84 ± 0.20 | 0.89 ± 0.10 | .394 |
Parahippocampal (gray and white matter) | 0–0.97 | 0.62 ± 0.28 | 0.64 ± 0.24 | .507 |
Fusiform (gray and white matter) | 0.08–0.46 | 0.19 ± 0.06 | 0.27 ± 0.09 | .009* |
Temporal pole (gray and white matter) | 0.49–1.00 | 0.89 ± 0.13 | 0.94 ± 0.06 | .166 |
Inferior temporal (gray and white matter) | 0.08–0.67 | 0.20 ± 0.11 | 0.40 ± 0.13 | <.001* |
Mid temporal (gray and white matter) | 0.04–0.61 | 0.14 ± 0.10 | 0.35 ± 0.15 | <.001* |
Superior temporal (gray and white matter) | 0.02–0.79 | 0.08 ± 0.07 | 0.37 ± 0.15 | <.001* |
Means and SD percentage resection of each temporal lobe substructure given for left and right ATLR.
Independent samples t test comparison performed with p values listed.
Significant after correction for multiple comparisons.
Cognitive Outcomes
Among participants who had an Engel Class 1 or Engel Class 2 outcome (n = 32), there was an average decrease in all measures of verbal memory (Figure 2; Table 3). After correction for multiple comparisons, there were no significant differences in memory performance change by side of resection, although there was a trend toward differing BNT and BDI changes, with patients receiving a left ATLR exhibiting a greater decline in naming and patients receiving a right ATLR reporting less depression after surgery.
Neuropsychological Test . | All Participants . | pa . | Left ATLR . | Right ATLR . | pb . |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Verbal Learning (L = 14, R = 13)c | −5.60 ± 10.56 | .014* | −8.31 ± 11.50 | −2.67 ± 8.39 | .188 |
Verbal SDFR (L = 14, R = 13)c | −2.00 ± 3.38 | .007* | −2.92 ± 3.55 | −1.00 ± 3.01 | .159 |
Verbal LDFR (L = 14, R = 13)c | −1.92 ± 3.57 | .011* | −3.07 ± 3.55 | −0.67 ± 3.08 | .084 |
RCFT (L = 11, R = 16) | −2.30 ± 5.67 | .045* | −0.73 ± 6.76 | −3.38 ± 4.72 | .241 |
Visual Confrontation Naming (BNT) (L = 14, R = 13) | −1.62 ± 7.10 | .244 | −4.43 ± 8.40 | 1.53 ± 3.71 | .022 |
BDI (L = 13, R 13) | −2.85 ± 7.54 | .066 | 0.08 ± 6.78 | −5.77 ± 7.34 | .045 |
BAI (L = 14, R = 13) | −5.11 ± 11.32 | .027* | −3.79 ± 11.84 | −6.54 ± 10.95 | .524 |
Neuropsychological Test . | All Participants . | pa . | Left ATLR . | Right ATLR . | pb . |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Verbal Learning (L = 14, R = 13)c | −5.60 ± 10.56 | .014* | −8.31 ± 11.50 | −2.67 ± 8.39 | .188 |
Verbal SDFR (L = 14, R = 13)c | −2.00 ± 3.38 | .007* | −2.92 ± 3.55 | −1.00 ± 3.01 | .159 |
Verbal LDFR (L = 14, R = 13)c | −1.92 ± 3.57 | .011* | −3.07 ± 3.55 | −0.67 ± 3.08 | .084 |
RCFT (L = 11, R = 16) | −2.30 ± 5.67 | .045* | −0.73 ± 6.76 | −3.38 ± 4.72 | .241 |
Visual Confrontation Naming (BNT) (L = 14, R = 13) | −1.62 ± 7.10 | .244 | −4.43 ± 8.40 | 1.53 ± 3.71 | .022 |
BDI (L = 13, R 13) | −2.85 ± 7.54 | .066 | 0.08 ± 6.78 | −5.77 ± 7.34 | .045 |
BAI (L = 14, R = 13) | −5.11 ± 11.32 | .027* | −3.79 ± 11.84 | −6.54 ± 10.95 | .524 |
Group level differences were calculated with a paired t test (post–pre), with mean raw scores and SD given for all participants.
Independent samples t test comparing change in neuropsychological test scores. After correction for multiple comparisons, there were no differences in the change in neuropyschological test outcomes for left- versus right-sided resections.
Calculated for all participants with same memory test given pre- and postsurgery (CVLT and Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test) for Engel Classes 1 and 2 only (n = 25).
Significant change from presurgical to postsurgical testing.
Predictors of Verbal Declarative Memory Outcomes
Clinical characteristics including age of seizure onset, duration of seizures before surgery, educational level, verbal IQ, and ipsilateral Wada memory score were not correlated with change in verbal learning or memory (Table 4); however, there was a positive correlation between baseline verbal learning and verbal learning change (r = .54, p = .006) and baseline SDFR and SDFR-change (r = .44, p = .026). There was no correlation between baseline LDFR and LDFR change (r = .286, p = .17). Neither total resection volume or percentage of total brain volume resected was significantly correlated with change in verbal learning or memory.
Participant Characteristics . | Verbal Learningb . | Verbal SDFRb . | Verbal LDFRb . | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
R . | p . | R . | p . | R . | p . | |
Age of seizure onset | .067a | .750 | .085 | .687 | .063 | .763 |
Duration of seizures before surgery | −.045 | .830 | <.001 | 1.000 | .029 | .892 |
Education | .246 | .236 | .073 | .729 | .292 | .157 |
Verbal IQ | .079 | .720 | −.011 | .961 | .189 | .387 |
Ipsilateral WADA Memory Score | −.167 | .436 | −.198 | .354 | −.295 | .162 |
Participant Characteristics . | Verbal Learningb . | Verbal SDFRb . | Verbal LDFRb . | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
R . | p . | R . | p . | R . | p . | |
Age of seizure onset | .067a | .750 | .085 | .687 | .063 | .763 |
Duration of seizures before surgery | −.045 | .830 | <.001 | 1.000 | .029 | .892 |
Education | .246 | .236 | .073 | .729 | .292 | .157 |
Verbal IQ | .079 | .720 | −.011 | .961 | .189 | .387 |
Ipsilateral WADA Memory Score | −.167 | .436 | −.198 | .354 | −.295 | .162 |
Values provided are Pearson correlations (bivariate), with two-tailed p values.
For verbal change scores, only participants who took the same memory test pre and post were included (n = 25).
Among participants who received a left ATLR, we found that baseline verbal learning performance accounted for 37% of the variance in verbal learning decline (Table 5). After accounting for baseline performance, the extent of parahippocampal resection (gray and white matter) contributed an additional 27% of the variance in verbal learning decline, although this did not reach statistical significance after correction for multiple comparisons (p = .02; Table 5A). Baseline verbal SDFR performance accounted for 37% of the variance in verbal SDFR-change. The extent of entorhinal region (gray and white matter) resection accounted for an additional 37% of the variance (p = .004), after controlling for the contribution of baseline SDFR performance. In a separate model, the extent of parahippocampal resection accounted for 27% of the variance in verbal SDFR-change, after controlling for baseline performance (p = .021; Table 5B). For LDFR-change, none of the regional resection extent values predicted performance decline (Table 5C). In summary, after correction for multiple comparisons, resection extent in parahippocampal and entorhinal regions remained significant predictors of decline in verbal SDFR.
A. Linear Regression with Verbal Learning Change as the Dependent Variable, for Left ATLR, Engel Classes 1–2 Only (n = 13) . | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
. | Baseline VL R2 . | R2-Change . | Total R2 . | Standardized β . | t . | p . |
Total volume resected | .37 | .05 | .41 | −0.21 | −0.862 | .409 |
Hippocampus | .41 | .09 | .50 | −0.32 | −1.30 | .23 |
Entorhinal region (GM + WM) | .37 | .06 | .43 | −0.25 | −1.03 | .33 |
Parahippocampal region (GM + WM) | 0.37 | 0.27 | 0.64 | −0.52 | −2.76 | .02 |
B. Linear Regression with SDFR Change as the Dependent Variable, for Left ATLR and Engel Classes 1–2 Only (n = 13) | ||||||
Baseline SDFR R2 | R2-Change | Total R2 | Standardized β | t | p | |
Total volume resected | .37 | .02 | .39 | −0.14 | −0.56 | .590 |
Hippocampus | .63 | .05 | .68 | −0.22 | −1.14 | .29 |
Entorhinal region (GM + WM) | .37 | .37 | .74 | −0.614 | −3.72 | .004* |
Parahippocampal region (GM + WM) | .37 | .27 | .637 | −0.520 | −2.73 | .021* |
C. Linear Regression with LDFR Change as the Dependent Variable, for Left ATLR, Engel Classes 1–2 Only (n = 13) | ||||||
Total R2 | Standardized β | t | p | |||
Total volume resected | .037 | −0.192 | −0.649 | .530 | ||
Hippocampus | .188 | −0.433 | −1.521 | .159 | ||
Entorhinal Region (GM + WM) (n = 13) | .275 | −0.524 | −2.042 | .066 | ||
Parahippocampal region (GM + WM) | .144 | −0.338 | −1.192 | .258 |
A. Linear Regression with Verbal Learning Change as the Dependent Variable, for Left ATLR, Engel Classes 1–2 Only (n = 13) . | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
. | Baseline VL R2 . | R2-Change . | Total R2 . | Standardized β . | t . | p . |
Total volume resected | .37 | .05 | .41 | −0.21 | −0.862 | .409 |
Hippocampus | .41 | .09 | .50 | −0.32 | −1.30 | .23 |
Entorhinal region (GM + WM) | .37 | .06 | .43 | −0.25 | −1.03 | .33 |
Parahippocampal region (GM + WM) | 0.37 | 0.27 | 0.64 | −0.52 | −2.76 | .02 |
B. Linear Regression with SDFR Change as the Dependent Variable, for Left ATLR and Engel Classes 1–2 Only (n = 13) | ||||||
Baseline SDFR R2 | R2-Change | Total R2 | Standardized β | t | p | |
Total volume resected | .37 | .02 | .39 | −0.14 | −0.56 | .590 |
Hippocampus | .63 | .05 | .68 | −0.22 | −1.14 | .29 |
Entorhinal region (GM + WM) | .37 | .37 | .74 | −0.614 | −3.72 | .004* |
Parahippocampal region (GM + WM) | .37 | .27 | .637 | −0.520 | −2.73 | .021* |
C. Linear Regression with LDFR Change as the Dependent Variable, for Left ATLR, Engel Classes 1–2 Only (n = 13) | ||||||
Total R2 | Standardized β | t | p | |||
Total volume resected | .037 | −0.192 | −0.649 | .530 | ||
Hippocampus | .188 | −0.433 | −1.521 | .159 | ||
Entorhinal Region (GM + WM) (n = 13) | .275 | −0.524 | −2.042 | .066 | ||
Parahippocampal region (GM + WM) | .144 | −0.338 | −1.192 | .258 |
Significant after correction for multiple comparisons (p < .05).
Discriminant Validity of Left Parahippocampal and Entorhinal Findings
To determine whether decline in verbal SDFR-change was being driven by resection extent in left entorhinal and parahippocampal regions and not other left temporal lobe subregions, we entered the remaining left temporal lobe subregions (i.e., fusiform, temporal pole, inferior temporal, middle temporal, and superior temporal gray and white matter) as predictors of verbal SDFR-change in separate regression models. There was no relationship between resection extent in any of these left temporal subregions and verbal SDFR-change. Furthermore, we examined the relationship between verbal SDFR-change and resection extent in all of the right temporal lobe subregions of patients who underwent right ATLR, and there were no significant findings. To establish the specificity of findings to verbal declarative memory, we examined the relationship between resection extent in left entorhinal regions and BNT-change (p = .252), RCFT-change (p = .262), BDI-change (p = .296), and BAI change (p = .857), and there were no significant findings. Likewise, there was no significant correlation between left parahippocampal resection extent and BNT-change (p = .368), RCFT change (p = .692), BDI-change (p = .688), or BAI change (p = .851). In addition, there was no relationship between change in verbal SDFR and improvement in depression (p = .514) or anxiety (p = .821) symptoms. Taken together, these results demonstrate that greater resection extent of left entorhinal and parahippocampal regions predicts decline in verbal declarative memory specifically and not neuropsychological functions in general.
DISCUSSION
In this retrospective longitudinal study, we demonstrate that the degree of left parahippocampal and entorhinal resection predicted decline in verbal declarative memory performance, after correction for multiple comparisons. This finding was present after accounting for the contribution of baseline verbal memory performance, which has been reported as a critical variable in prior literature (Dulay & Busch, 2012; Baxendale, Thompson, Harkness, & Duncan, 2006). It is important to note that the total resection volume and resection extent of other left and right temporal subregions were not related to verbal memory outcomes, suggesting that the extent of resection of left parahippocampal and entorhinal regions made a specific contribution to verbal memory decline. However, we cannot draw conclusions about the contribution of hippocampal resection, as this structure was nearly completely resected in most cases (0.84 ± 0.07).
Declines in verbal memory after left temporal lobectomy have been described in other retrospective neurosurgical series (Helmstaedter, Petzold, et al., 2011; Helmstaedter, Roeske, et al., 2011; Helmstaedter, Grunwald, Lehnertz, Gleissner, & Elger, 1997) and selective amygdalo-hippocampectomy (Wendling et al., 2013; Morino et al., 2006). However, there are mixed findings in verbal memory outcomes between the more selective mesial temporal and complete temporal lobe resection (Kuang, Yang, Gu, Kong, & Cheng, 2014; Wendling et al., 2013; Helmstaedter et al., 2008; Morino et al., 2006). Our findings extend previous reports by identifying key temporal lobe subregions that appear to drive verbal memory decline. These findings are consistent with a small case series of patients who had hippocampal-sparing temporal lobe resections; only patients who had perirhinal and entorhinal cortex resections had an immediate and lasting effect on associative learning of unrelated word pairs (Weintrob, Saling, Berkovic, & Reutens, 2007).
Our findings highlight the critical role that the perforant pathway plays in encoding and STM for verbal material. The entorhinal cortex plays a privileged role because it receives input from the perirhinal cortex and parahippocampal cortex and communicates directly to the dentate gyrus and CA1–C3 hippocampal subfields (Eichenbaum, Sauvage, Fortin, Komorowski, & Lipton, 2012). Entorhinal cortex serves as a critical gateway facilitating transfer and convergence of unimodal and polymodal sensory input from the dorsal and ventral streams to the hippocampus (Eichenbaum et al., 2012). In particular, entorhinal cortex may be particularly activated by novel information, increasing the probability of transfer to the hippocampus for encoding (Fernández & Tendolkar, 2006). Communication between entorhinal cortex and hippocampus is facilitated by phase-coupled theta and gamma frequency activity (Fell, Klaver, Elger, & Fernandez, 2002). MRI volumetric studies of patients with intractable temporal lobe epilepsy show hippocampal and entorhinal atrophy ipsilateral to the seizure focus compared with healthy controls, suggesting ongoing cell loss from disruption of a critical hippocampal–entorhinal connection (Bernasconi et al., 2003).
Direct electrical stimulation studies also support the critical role of entorhinal cortex in encoding. In rodents, stimulation of the perforant pathway results in long-term potentiation, including the resetting of the theta phase, which has been associated with improved memory outcomes (Vertes, 2005; Williams & Givens, 2003; Ehret et al., 2001; Bliss & Gardner-Medwin, 1973; Bliss & Lomo, 1973). In epilepsy patients undergoing surgical evaluation, stimulation applied to the EC, but not the hippocampus, enhanced learning on a spatial navigation task (Suthana et al., 2012). Likewise, previous fMRI studies also support the importance of the parahippocampal structures to verbal associative learning (word–word or word–face), demonstrating increased activation during these tasks (Vilberg & Davachi, 2013; Kirwan & Stark, 2004; Weintrob et al., 2002; Bernard, Desgranges, Platel, Baron, & Eustache, 2001).
To our knowledge, this is the first study to separately measure resection extent in the parahippocampal and entorhinal regions in human participants after epilepsy surgery and examine their respective contributions to verbal declarative memory decline. The strengths of this study include the use of an MRI volumetric tracing method that precisely quantifies the extent of resection in each of these medial-temporal subregions. Since memory stabilizes approximately 2 years after surgery (Andersson-Roswall, Engman, Samuelsson, & Malmgren, 2010; Alpherts et al., 2006), our length of follow-up has likely captured patients' new cognitive baseline.
A limitation of our method for determining resection extent was that brain shift from pre- to postoperative images was evident in some participants. This necessitated visual review and manual correction of lesion masks, which increased the processing time demands. We utilized MRI scans acquired proximal to surgery to minimize such error. Thus, it is important to point out that the lesion masks reflect the extent of tissue resected but not additional damage that might occur from Wallerian degeneration (Winston, Stretton, Sidhu, Symms, & Duncan, 2014; Liu, Gross, Wheatley, Concha, & Beaulieu, 2013; McDonald et al., 2010). Postsurgical white matter changes, including both degeneration and reorganization, might impact postsurgical neuropsychological performance (Yogarajah et al., 2010). However, more sensitive measures of tissue microstructure, such as diffusion tensor imaging, are needed to assess this. In summary, although ongoing postsurgical changes in brain microstructure might also play an important role in postsurgical memory performance, our study is limited to assessing the role of surgical resection extent. In addition, we chose a cortical parcellation method (Desikan et al., 2006) that achieves regional specificity while minimizing the multiple comparison problem and the impact of coregistration error. Finer-grained parcellations improve spatial specificity; however, costs include a higher number of statistical tests and potentially increased risk of coregistration error in smaller regions. Future prospective studies would benefit from higher-resolution pre- and postsurgical imaging of the mesial temporal region and finer grained parcellation of the entorhinal region into perirhinal, medial, and lateral subregions. Although we did not find a relationship between hippocampal resection extent and learning and memory outcomes, this may have been due to the near complete resection of the hippocampus in most patients and the limited range of hippocampal resection extent values. A final limitation of our study is the limited number of patients included, although our numbers fall within the range of published studies on surgical cohorts.
These findings may ultimately inform neurosurgical approaches to reduce cognitive morbidity after epilepsy surgery. At the very least, these numbers may be helpful in presurgical counseling to advise patients as to the degree of memory impairment to be expected after surgery. Our investigation suggests that there is a measurable decline, on the order of 10–20% decline on a verbal list learning task, in the majority of patients who undergo a left temporal lobe resection. Ideally, a larger, multicenter sample with different surgical approaches could confirm or refute our findings. In particular, larger participant numbers are needed to determine if the presence of mesial temporal sclerosis influences the relationship between resection extent and memory outcomes. Finally, our findings support further investigation of stimulation of perforant pathways to potentially remediate memory dysfunction.
In summary, we use a precise method of quantifying surgical resection volumes to discover that degree of left parahippocampal and entorhinal resection was strongly correlated with decline in recall or verbal material. These findings support the growing body of lesion literature in animals and humans that highlights the critical role of the perforant structures in declarative memory function.
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by NYU Finding a Cure for Epilepsy and Seizures (FACES) and the Morris and Alma Schapiro Fund.
Reprint requests should be sent to Anli Liu, NYU Comprehensive Epilepsy Center, New York University School of Medicine, 223 East 34th Street, New York, NY 10016-6402, or via e-mail: [email protected].