Abstract
We present an effective model for timing-dependent synaptic plasticity (STDP) in terms of two interacting traces, corresponding to the fraction of activated NMDA receptors and the concentration in the dendritic spine of the postsynaptic neuron. This model intends to bridge the worlds of existing simplistic phenomenological rules and highly detailed models, thus constituting a practical tool for the study of the interplay of neural activity and synaptic plasticity in extended spiking neural networks. For isolated pairs of pre- and postsynaptic spikes, the standard pairwise STDP rule is reproduced, with appropriate parameters determining the respective weights and timescales for the causal and the anticausal contributions. The model contains otherwise only three free parameters, which can be adjusted to reproduce triplet nonlinearities in hippocampal culture and cortical slices. We also investigate the transition from time-dependent to rate-dependent plasticity occurring for both correlated and uncorrelated spike patterns.
1 Introduction
The fact that synaptic plasticity can depend on the precise timing of pre- and postsynaptic spikes (Bi & Rubin, 2005; Rubin, Gerkin, Bi, & Chow, 2005) indicates that time has to be coded somehow in individual neurons. If the concentration of a certain ion or molecule, which we will refer to as a trace, decays in time after a given event in a regular fashion, then the level of that trace could serve as a time coder, in the same way as the concentration of a radioactive isotope can be used to date a fossil.
A range of models has been proposed that formulate long-term potentiation (LTP) and long-term depression (LTD) in terms of traces in the postsynaptic neurons (Karmarkar & Buonomano, 2002; Badoual et al., 2006; Shouval, Bear, & Cooper, 2002; Rubin et al., 2005; Graupner & Brunel, 2012; Uramoto & Torikai, 2013). Several of these models successfully reproduce a wide range of experimental results, including pairwise STDP, triplet, and even quadruplet nonlinearities. Most models, however, require fitting of a large number of parameters individually for each experimental setup and involve heavily nonlinear functions of the trace concentrations. While possibly realistic in nature, the study of neural systems modeled under these rules from a dynamical point of view becomes a highly nontrivial task. At the other end, the connection between predictions of simplified models, constructed as phenomenological rules (Badoual et al., 2006; Froemke & Dan, 2002), and the biological underpinnings is normally hard to establish, as they usually aim to reproduce only the synaptic change and do away with the information stored in the the traces themselves.
In this letter, we propose a straightforward model formulating synaptic potentiation and depression in terms of two interacting traces representing the fraction of activated N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptors and the concentration of intracellular at the postsynaptic spine, with the intention of bridging these two worlds. Having a low number of parameters and being composed of only polynomial differential equations, the model is able nonetheless to reproduce key features of LTP and LTD. Moreover, since the parameters of the model are easily related to the dynamical properties of the system, it permits making a connection between the observed synaptic weight change and the behavior of the underlying traces.
2 The Model
Plasticity in our model will be expressed in terms of two interacting traces on the postsynaptic site, which we denote x and y, representing the fraction of open-state NMDA receptors (or NMDARs) and the concentration in the dendritic spine of the postsynaptic neuron, respectively. For clarification, we recall the overall mechanism of the synaptic transmission process in a glutamatergic synapse, as illustrated in Figure 1.
A presynaptic spike results in the release of glutamate molecules across the synaptic cleft, which will activate a series of receptors on the postsynaptic spine, including the NMDA receptors and -amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid (AMPA) receptors (AMPAR) (Meldrum, 2000). ions will then flow through the AMPAR channels into the dendritic spine of the postsynaptic cell, triggering a cascade of events that may eventually lead to the activation of an axonal spike at the soma of the postsynaptic cell and of an action potential backpropagating down the dendritic tree. This action potential has two effects captured within our model: the first is the activation of voltage-gated channels (VGCC), allowing an influx of ions, resulting in an increase of the concentration y; the second is the unblocking of NMDAR channels, as we detail in what follows.
ions may flow into the postsynaptic spine also through the NMDAR channels (Meldrum, 2000), but for this to happen, two conditions need to be fulfilled. NMDARs are activated when glutamate binds to them, which opens the receptor’s permeable channel. The channels are said to be open when the protein’s conformational state permits ions to flow through them, and closed otherwise. At resting membrane potential, however, ions are present in the channel’s pore, blocking the channel and preventing ions from permeating the neuron (Mayer, Westbrook, & Guthrie, 1984). This block is temporarily removed by a backpropagating action potential. For to flow into the postsynaptic spine, two conditions need to be fulfilled: the presence of glutamate in the synaptic cleft, triggered by a presynaptic spike, and a back propagating action potential, signaling a postsynaptic spike. The NMDA receptors are hence the primary agents within our model for the interaction of pre- and postsynaptic neural activities in terms of axonal spikes. They are also the primary agents for causality within the STDP rule.
2.1 Trace Dynamics
Two forces therefore compete to drive nonlinear plasticity in our model: trace accumulation and spike suppression, the latter formulated in the present effective model via a saturation term.
The update rules, equation 2.1, for the traces are reduced in the sense that all superfluous constants have been rescaled away, as discussed in the appendix.
2.2 Update Rules for the Synaptic Weight
The second term in equation 2.3 leads to a decrease of the synaptic weight and needs a finite level for both the calcium concentration y and the fraction of open NMDA receptors x (which can be taken as a measure of the glutamate concentration in the synaptic cleft), in addition to the presence of a presynaptic spike, which acts as a second coincidence detector as proposed by Karmarkar and Buonomano (2002). The parameters and represent the relative strengths of these two contributions.
The pre- and postsynaptic spikes mark the timing of the synaptic update in our model, equation 2.3, for the synaptic plasticity. Here we considered -like spikes, and one needs, for numerical simulations using small but finite time steps, to update the traces using equation 2.1 before updating the weights using equation 2.3.
2.3 The Pairwise STDP Rule
In the limit of low frequencies, the traces decay to zero in between the occurrence of two pairs of spikes, which may hence be considered as isolated.
2.4 Spike Triplets
The effect of a pair of pre- and postsynaptic spikes has been experimentally shown to depend, in a nonlinear fashion, not only on its interspike interval but also on the presence of additional spikes temporally proximal to the pair. The contribution of spike triplets, the simplest case of spike-pair interactions, cannot be described as a linear sum of two individual contributions of spike pairs (Froemke & Dan, 2002; Wang, Gerkin, Nauen, & Bi, 2005).
We see that the first term in equations 2.9 and 2.10 corresponding to the first pair remains in both cases unchanged, by construction, with nonlinearities appearing in the second contribution. In the second term of equation 2.9, we find a first factor (the first parenthesis) corresponding to a correction produced by the interaction between the two traces (the calcium inflow through NMDAR channels), and a second factor corresponding to the balance between trace accumulation and spike suppression. In equation 2.10, we also find a term balancing trace accumulation and spike suppression. The multiplicative factor inside the brackets comes from the way we have decided to factorize the equation, since .
2.5 Interpretation of the Variables and Parameters in the Model
Our proposed model contains a relatively small number of variables and parameters and can be considered an effective approach with the biological underpinnings of STDP being governed by a substantially larger number of variables and parameters whose functional interdependences are naturally far more complex than the polynomial description here proposed. Any effective model will, however, pool together within each effective variable or parameter several effects that might depend on a variety of different factors in the biological neuron.
In section 2.1, we defined x as the fraction of open but unblocked NMDAR channels. When paired with a postsynaptic spike and under the simplifications assumed in the model, the value of x can be then associated with a transient calcium current, and a comparison with experimental results of the parameters related to x would reflect this role. The time window for LTP, for instance, results in our model from the value of (as we showed in section 2.3). can then be interpreted in this context as the decay time of the transient calcium current. Hao and Oertner (2012) have argued that the narrow window for LTP results from AMPA-EPSP in the postsynaptic spine. In fact, as reviewed in the same article, the whole spine seems to work as an electrical amplifier, locally prolonging the depolarization time at the spine. It is therefore not surprising to find different values of the time constants in different neurons or even within different synapses within the same neuron. In our model, we do not compute AMPA currents directly and reduce the overall effect of the spine to the effective value of . Similarly, represents the timescale for decay of the effective calcium concentration at the spine.
We have included in this work saturation terms for both variables x and y. As it has been proposed in the past (Froemke & Dan, 2002), triplet nonlinearities in visual cortical neurons indicate strong suppression effects on future spikes by previous spikes of the train. The saturation terms included in the model provide one possible effective way of dealing with spike suppression, reducing a biological complex phenomenon further down the cascade of processes, leading eventually to LTP and to LTD, respectively.
3 Results for the Hippocampus
Three parameters entering equations 2.1 and 2.6, namely, yc, xb, and yb, are to be selected. In a continuous time evolution scenario, xb and yb determine strict maximal concentrations for the traces. In the discrete time scenario, overshoots are, however, possible due to the finite increase in the traces after every spike. In this context and in a low-frequency situation, the first spike in the stimulation pattern is unaffected by the limiting factor, and only the efficacy of the following spike is reduced. Since xb and yb then do not affect pairwise STDP, they need to be selected from higher-order contributions to the weight change. In this case, we selected the values of yc, xb, and yb from triplet results, as presented in what follows.
In Figure 5 we now compare our results for triplets, as described in section 2.4, with experiments for cultured rat hippocampal neurons (Wang et al., 2005). The triplet stimulation experimental protocol consists of a regular train of 60 triplets with a repetition frequency of , and we use the identical protocol for the theory simulations. We also keep the pairwise STDP parameters, equation 3.1, valid for cultured rat hippocampal neurons and adjust the remaining three free parameters yc, xb, and yb by minimizing the standard deviation (SD) between the numerical and the experimental results, obtaining , , and (with an SD of ).
We found that the SD varies smoothly, and relatively weakly, with the exact choice of the three free parameters, as can be expected from the analytical expressions, and that this freedom can be used to obtain a range of functional dependencies of the synaptic plasticity on spiking frequencies, as discussed in section 5.
We have also included in Figure 5 the expected synaptic weight changes for the case of a linear superposition of the two respective interspike contributions via equation 2.5. One observes that the discrepancy between the nonlinear and the linear interactions is much stronger for pre-post-pre than for post-pre-post triplets, with the former leading to an overall reduced synaptic weight change and the latter configuration to a substantial potentiation. It is interesting to observe here that spike suppression, as proposed in Froemke and Dan (2002), from cortical neurons cannot explain nonlinearities in hippocampus. Suppression of the second presynaptic spike in the triplet would reduce depression, and the overall result would be supralinear potentiation, contrary to the experimental observation. Trace accumulation is the dominant effect driving nonlinearities in hippocampal neurons.
4 Results for the Cortex
To select yc, xb, and yb we once again resort to triplet results. In Froemke and Dan (2002), the change produced by triplets of either two pre- and one postsynaptic spikes or one pre- and two postsynaptic spikes was also measured. The data consist in this case, however, of a large set of specific triplet timing configurations, with every individual triplet configuration measured once. We decided to treat all measurements on an equal footing, fitting the complete set by minimizing the mean square error without introducing any further bias.
We obtain in this case , , and . The obtained SD of is, in this case, much larger than the one found for hippocampus, though that is partly due to the variance in the experimental data themselves, corresponding to individual data points and not to averaged results. Another consequence of the large variance in the data is that the minimum in the SD is relatively broad. We discuss these points in detail in what follows.
In order to compare the results for cortical neurons with the previous section on hippocampal neurons, as presented in Figure 5, we have performed a smooth interpolation of the set of individual experimental results for cortical triplets by means of gaussian filters. In Figure 7 we compare the theory results with the interpolated experimental data.
Contrary to the hippocampal triplet results presented in Figure 5, experiments in cortical slices show that post-pre-post triplets lead to strong depression and pre-post-pre triplets to potentiation. Post-pre-post triplets deviate, in addition, somewhat more from a linear superposition of the contribution of the two inherent spike pairs than the pre-post-pre configuration. While the predictions of the model presented in Figure 7 are clearly not as good as the ones obtained for hippocampal culture, they are still qualitatively in agreement with the experimental results, successfully capturing the asymmetry between post-pre-post and pre-post-pre triplets. While there is still room for improvement in this regard, we believe it is important that the model can switch from the hippocampal to the cortical regime in terms of triplet nonlinearities.
As we previously mentioned, the data have a much larger variance in this case. To have an idea of of the variability of the data, we computed the standard deviation of the data to the smooth gaussian interpolation of width 5 ms that we used for the visual comparison of Figure 7, which yields an SD of (as compared to the SD of between model and experiment). For this reason, we believe that a reasonable goal in this case is to reproduce the distinct qualitative feature of the triplet nonlinearities more than an accurate quantitative approximation.
The optimal value of obtained when fitting the experimental triplet results (see Figure 7), seems to be too large, in particular when compared to the one obtained for hippocampal neurons. This result can be traced back to the occurrence of a broad minimum for the least-square fit together with a relative high variability of the experimental data. We have hence also examined parameter configurations with lower values for yc. Also included in Figure 7 is an example with , also representing the observed experimental features qualitatively. We find that the particular cortical structure of triplets arises from strong saturation, being a consequence of .
5 Frequency Dependence
So far we have considered only pairs or triplets of pre- and postsynaptic spikes coming at low frequencies and with very precise timings. This will not necessarily be the case in a natural train of spikes. It is therefore interesting to examine the model’s prediction for spike trains with different degrees of correlation between pre- and postsynaptic spikes. A neuron usually receives input from about 10,000 other neurons. While the correlation of the postsynaptic neuron will be higher for a strong synapse driving the neuron, the postsynaptic neuron in general will not be correlated with all of its inputs. We therefore study both types of connections.
We begin in section 5.1 by studying the case of uncorrelated trains of pre- and postsynaptic spikes and then analyze in section 5.2 the case of a driving synapse with different degrees of correlation. In these sections, we numerically evaluate the synaptic strength change as a function of the pre- and postsynaptic neuronal firing rates.
5.1 Plasticity Induced by Uncorrelated Spikes
We begin by evaluating the synaptic change produced by uncorrelated trains of Poisson pre- and postsynaptic spikes. In these simulations, we use the same parameters as fitted from pairwise and triplet experiments in hippocampus and cortex, referring to hippocampal and cortical neurons, respectively.
The results of the simulations for hippocampal neurons are shown in Figure 8. We present two kinds of plots in the diagram: a plot where the pre- and postsynaptic firing rates are equal and plots of constant presynaptic frequency for varying postsynaptic firing rates. We observe in this last type that the sign of the weight changes as a function of the postsynaptic activity for a constant presynaptic frequency, generically switching from negative to positive at a certain threshold . This threshold increases with rising presynaptic frequency, resulting in a sliding threshold. In other rate-based learning rules like BCM (Bienenstock, Cooper, & Munro, 1982), similar thresholds for potentiation are determined by appropriate long-term averages of the postsynaptic activity. In our model, is set by the level of the presynaptic activity, as measured on timescales of the respective traces. This feature would allow the neuron to adjust the threshold of each synapse independently, setting in each case the level of what constitutes significant activity.
The overall synaptic change becomes Hebbian for large pre- and post-firing rates fpre and fpost, in the sense that it is then proportional to the product . This weight change is influenced in a substantial way by the value of yc, and we have presented in Figure 8 two sets of parameters, one with (left panel) and one with (right panel), yielding otherwise similar SDs when fitting the experimental triplet data ( and , respectively).
Potentiation dominates for larger values of yc, as seen in Figure 8(b). These results seem, at first sight, counterintuitive given the role of yc as a threshold for LTP. Note, however, that yc contributes to the increase in y through equation 2.1, and both LTP and LTD are dependent on y in the plasticity rule, equation 2.6, with the LTD contribution being proportional to .
Comparing Figures 8a and 8b, we observe that yc can be used to regulate the threshold for potentiation in the rate-encoding limit without changing the behavior of isolated spike triplets substantially. yc is hence a vehicle for also adapting the overall postsynaptic activity level, and it would be interesting, for future research, to study how this regulative mechanism would interact with other known ways to regulate the overall level of the postsynaptic neural activity, such as intrinsic plasticity rules (Triesch, 2007; Marković & Gros, 2012; Linkerhand & Gros, 2013).
It has to be remarked that the solid lines in Figure 8, representing weight changes as a function of the postsynaptic frequency for a constant presynaptic firing rate, while of theoretical interest to understand the behavior of , will not correspond to a usual physiological functional relationship between the rates, at least for a driving synapse. If the presynaptic synapse drives the postsynaptic neuron, the postsynaptic activity will in general be an increasing function of the presynaptic rate. Here we have chosen (the dashed lines in Figures 8 and 9) as an illustration, but a more detailed transfer function should be selected for accurate and quantitative comparisons with experimental results. In this sense, the parameter configuration of Figure 8(b) shows better agreement with experimental procedures, such as that of Sjöström, Turrigiano, and Nelson (2001), where potentiation is shown to become stronger with higher frequencies.
No complete set of experimental results has hitherto been published, unfortunately, where all pairwise, triplet, and frequency-dependent plasticity have been measured for the same type of synapse and with the same experimental stimulation procedure. A full consistency check between model and experiment is hence not possible to date.
In Figure 9 the results of numerical simulations for L2/3 cortical neurons for the same protocol of Figure 8 are presented. In this case, depression is found for all combinations of pre- and postsynaptic frequencies, a robust prediction of the model. Different values of yc were selected to test this behavior, and in each case the rest of the parameters were fitted to the triplet results. In each case, the value of yb obtained by this fitting turned out to be lower than yc. The y-trace has hence a hard time overcoming the threshold yc for LTP as calcium increase by further spikes is prevented. As a test, if yb was artificially set to values larger than yc, potentiation for larger frequencies was recovered, but the fit of the experimental triplet data deteriorated substantially, obtaining potentiation for PostPrePost triplets, contrary to the experimental results. This indicates that triplet nonlinearities found in L2/3 cortical neurons result from spike suppression, contrary to the predominant trace accumulation effect present in hippocampal neurons.
These results, predicted for L2/3 neurons as fitted from Froemke and Dan (2002), would then be in stark contrast to those of Sjöström et al. (2001) for L5 neurons in visual cortex where LTP dominates for large frequencies. It should be pointed out, however, that the pairwise STDP plot presented in Sjöström et al. (2001) is already different from that of L2/3 neurons, raising the question of to what extent results coming from different neurons or obtained using different stimulation procedures, should be alike.
On the other hand, the prediction of overall depression dominating for uncorrelated spike trains in certain cortical neurons seems to be in line with, or at least does not contradict, experimental findings for deprivation experiments. In cortical areas, where topological maps are usually found, deprivation of sensory input has been shown to result in depression of the respective synaptic connections (Trachtenberg, Trepel, & Stryker, 2000; Feldman, 2000). At the same time, correlation has been found to substantially decrease after these procedures in areas projecting to cortex (Linden, Heynen, Haslinger, & Bear, 2009), suggesting that decorrelation of spike trains could be responsible for the observed depression in cortical neurons.
A possible reason behind the observed differences in these studies might be the stimulation protocol employed. While in Bi and Poo (1998) and Wang et al. (2005), plasticity is triggered by eliciting the firing of the pre- and postsynaptic neurons by dual whole-cell clamp, in the cortical results from Froemke and Dan (2002), extracellular presynaptic stimulation is performed, clamping only the postsynaptic neurons. This creates an asymmetry between pre-post-pre and post-pre-post triplets. Moreover, in the case of extracellular stimulation, the question remains to what extent other synapses are being affected, potentially triggering, in turn, other forms of plasticity such as local synaptic scaling.
It is important to stress that the robust depression found here for higher frequencies is a direct consequence of the triplet results, and indeed it vanishes if one uses hippocampal-like triplet results. The same suppression effect present for triplets also affects higher-frequency trains, resulting in depression.
For lower frequencies, the pairwise contribution dominates when determining the balance between potentiation and depression. Izhikevich and Desai (2003) show how a straightforward application of the pairwise rule to Poisson uncorrelated spike trains (as in our simulation), adding up linearly the effect of every pair in the train according to the pairwise STDP rule with cortical parameters, always leads to depression, since the pairs simply sample the STDP curve, which has an overall negative area (the opposite is true in hippocampal neurons, as we show below). Our model is, by construction, equivalent in the low-frequency limit to the linear pairwise model since isolated spikes produce no synaptic change in our model and triplets and higher-order configurations become very infrequent if the frequency is low. For low pre- and postsynaptic frequencies, the trains of Poisson spikes can be considered as pairs of random duration that sample the pairwise STDP curve.
While for hippocampal neurons , we find in cortex . This means that in the absence of higher-order contributions (which is true if both the pre- and the postsynaptic frequencies are low), uncorrelated spikes will on average lead to depression in cortical neurons and to potentiation in hippocampal neurons. If the frequencies tend to zero, the average interspike interval will be long compared to the STDP window duration, and the net amount of synaptic change, whether positive or negative, will be low. In the following section, this fact will become clear when the synaptic changes of correlated and uncorrelated spikes are compared.
As the frequencies of pre- and postsynaptic spikes increase, the interspike period decreases, and when this becomes comparable to the timescale of the STDP window (which is related to the trace timescale), the pairwise approximation will break down since interactions can no longer be neglected. When both the pre- and postsynaptic frequencies are on the order of , the average time between a pre- and a postsynaptic spike is on the order of and interactions are to be expected. This is where the particular models for the underlying dynamics will differ. Izhikevich and Desai (2003) also show with their nearest-neighbor implementation that synaptic change goes from general depression (in the all-to-all implementation) to BCM-like when they consider only the closest previous and posterior postsynaptic spike to each presynaptic spike to compute the linear sum of pairs.
This choice, which at first glance would seem an approximation independent of any underlying dynamics, has strong implications for the biological underpinnings that could implement this algorithm. A first-neighbor approximation requires hard resetting any traces possibly present, forgetting completely anything that happened outside that window. The nearest-neighor implementation does not aim to explain triplet nonlinearities such as those evidenced by the PrePostPre protocol.
The interspike interaction in our model is driven by the undelying traces. We have chosen in our simulation to use for the frequency-dependence protocol the same parameters obtained from pairwise and triplet fits. As observed in cortical PostPrePost triplets, strong suppression severely limits the potentiation of further spikes (compare PostPrePost to linear superposition results). At high frequencies, triplet interactions become relevant, and the same suppression should then be evidenced for frequency-dependent plasticity. We believe, then, that any model aiming to reproduce time-dependent plasticity up to triplet order as measured by Froemke and Dan (2002) should show depression also for high frequencies in cortical neurons.
5.2 Plasticity Induced by Correlated Spikes
So far we have analyzed the effect of uncorrelated spikes on the synaptic weight change in both hippocampal and cortical neurons (see Figures 8 and 9). It is, however, interesting to see the predictions of the model for a strong synapse driving the postsynaptic neuron. In this case, pre- and postsynaptic spikes should be correlated, at least partially, together with a certain positive delay.
To reproduce this effect with our model, we simulated trains of correlated spikes where, with each presynaptic spike, a postsynaptic spike can occur with probability p after a certain delay d. As an example, if every presynaptic spike triggers a postsynaptic spike, then p = 1. This would mean, however, that the postsynaptic frequency fpost changes with p (fpre). In order to compare our results for different values of p and keep fpost independent of p, we complete the train of postsynaptic spikes with Poisson spikes of frequency . In this way, fpost is independent of p, which now regulates the degree of correlation between pre- and postsynaptic spikes: represents the fully decorrelated case, since all the postsynaptic spikes are drawn from the Poisson distribution, and represents the fully correlated case already mentioned.
In Figure 10 we present the synaptic changes produced by spike trains for different values of p. In this case, results for a delay of are presented. The same tests were performed with delays from to with only quantitative, not qualitative, differences.
We observe in Figure 10, for both hippocampal and cortical neurons, that correlated spike trains induce an increasing amount of potentiation for low to intermediate frequencies (). In the correlated scenario, and since in this case we are simulating a driving synapse, postsynaptic spikes follow presynaptic spikes in a causal order. When the frequency is higher than , the interspike period becomes comparable to the STDP time window, and each postsynaptic spike will also “see” the following presynaptic spike, thus triggering the LTD term. Depending on the trace saturation constants, LTD or LTP will eventually dominate for large frequencies. If LTD dominates, depression results, and after a certain reversal frequency, the behavior is switched from Hebbian to anti-Hebbian. This is the case for the triplet fitted values presented in Figure 10.
It is important to note that the model is also able to produce Hebbian behavior within the entire physiological range of activities by changing yc. The smaller the saturation effects are, the larger this reversal frequency becomes. In fact, with the second set of parameters used in Figure 8b, no such reversal is found within physiological frequencies (not shown here).
If observed, such a reversal, yet another side of the suppression effect, would have the benefit of being self-stabilizing, tuning synaptic strength to help keep neural activities bound.
The kink observable for the fully correlated curve () of Figure 10b results from the particularly strong suppression effects in cortical neurons, captured in our model by . For frequencies above a certain threshold (about ) the trace concentration y is always below the yc and LTP never triggers.
A fundamental difference between plots a and b in Figure 10 is the different qualitative behavior between correlated and uncorrelated spikes. While in hippocampal neurons, increasing Hebbian, potentiation is always present for low to intermediate frequencies (whether the spikes are correlated or not), in cortical neurons, our model predicts that uncorrelated spikes always produce depression, and therefore Hebbian learning requires the neurons to be at least partially correlated.
6 Comparison to Other Models
The problem of formulating plasticity in terms of the specific timing of pre- and postsynaptic spikes can be approached at different levels of detail and accuracy, ranging from simplistic phenomenological rules to detailed and complex models describing the different steps of the biological machinery responsible for STDP. In sections 3 and 4, the comparison of our model to simple forms of phenomenological rules has already been established, noting that linear combinations of spike pairs are generically not sufficient to explain the experimentally observed triplet nonlinearities.
We have also shown that while linear combinations of pairs, plus additional suppression, are enough to explain the triplet nonlinearities of cortical neurons, as shown in Froemke and Dan (2002), hippocampal triplet nonlinearities cannot be explained by suppression and a trace accumulation mechanism seems to be taking place in these synapses. In any case, these kinds of phenomenological rules are not likely to generalize well to arbitrary spike patterns since no information of the underlying plasticity mechanism is present in the formulation.
Other models, like Albers, Schmiedt, and Pawelzik (2013) and Pfister and Gerstner (2006), present interesting dynamical formulations of plasticity in terms of generic decaying markers or traces, but they do not attempt to establish a link to the biological underpinnings of STDP. Calcium concentration and NMDA receptors have been shown to play a central role in time-dependent LTP and LTD, and we therefore believe it is important to formulate plasticity in those terms. Our model, though simplified, is formulated in terms of these key ingredients and may therefore help to bridge the worlds of functional and realistic models.
An alternative approach has been proposed by Appleby and Elliott (2007), where plasticity is described in an ensemble-based formulation. The authors argue that the observed synaptic changes produced by standard protocols cannot be explained at a single synapse level, but rather state that the observed results arise at a population level. They then show how the pairwise STDP curve can be recovered at the ensemble level from all-or-nothing potentiation or depression at the single synapse level. The dependency of the model to the specific timing of triplets is, however, in this case not computed.
The model we present in this work belongs to the family of calcium-based spiking neuron models. Within this family, models formulating synaptic plasticity exclusively in terms of the calcium levels (Uramoto & Torikai, 2013; Graupner & Brunel, 2012), while tuned to reproduce a variety of experimental results, tend to show paradoxical results when tested in other setups. The model presented by Uramoto and Torikai (2013), for example, predicts synaptic changes even when only postsynaptic spikes are present. The model in Graupner and Brunel (2012) shows plasticity also when either pre- or postsynaptic spikes are absent, since both pre- and postsynaptic spikes contribute directly to the calcium level in this model, without the need of coincidence. To avoid this, in our model we demand the simultaneous presence of both pre- and postsynaptic spikes for plasticity to arise, being proportional to the products of traces x and y in our rule. We believe this to be an important feature for simulations in situations of complex spike patterns where the pre- and postsynaptic firing rates do not necessarily match.
7 Discussion
We propose a basic trace model for timing-dependent plasticity that incorporates, in a first-order approximation, the fundamental mechanisms acknowledged to be taking place in STDP. We show that the model successfully captures several main features of time-dependent plasticity, including the standard shape for low-frequency pairing, experimentally observed triplet nonlinearities, and large frequency effects.
The decay constants for the two traces and the relative intensities of LTP and LTD can be extracted directly from the standard STDP curves, as measured for isolated pairs. The model is left hereafter with only three further parameters, which can be used to fit higher-order contributions to plasticity. The model successfully reproduces the distinct and contrasting nonlinearities found in both hippocampal cultures and cortical slices.
While the model predicts a similar frequency dependence for correlated (or partially correlated) pre- and postsynaptic spikes for both hippocampal and cortical neurons, the effect of uncorrelated spikes (although smaller) differs qualitatively in these two types of neurons. In this case, the sign of the resulting plasticity depends for lower frequencies on the overall area of the pairwise STDP curve, resulting in potentiation for hippocampal neurons and depression in cortical ones and for higher frequencies on the balance between spike suppression and trace accumulation.
We show that the model is able to reproduce typical frequency dependencies for uncorrelated spikes, while fitting pairwise and triplet hippocampal parameters. We also find that fitting triplet results for L2/3 cortical neurons invariably leads to depression, for higher frequencies and of uncorrelated spikes, contrary to observations in L5 neurons. The question then arises is to what extent plasticity results for different neurons, or performed under different stimulation conditions, can be expected to match. It seems essential to have available a consistent set of experiments where pairwise, triplet, and frequency results are measured for the same type of neuron and with the same stimulation protocol. Otherwise one runs the risk of possibly trying to build a complete picture out of mismatching parts. In this sense, we hope that our predictions serve as a motivation to revisit and complete triplet and frequency-dependent studies for different types of neurons.
In order to compare our results with rate-encoding plasticity models, we have also shown, by setting the presynaptic frequency to a constant value, that the amount of synaptic change is proportional for hippocampal neurons to the product of the activities, with a threshold that depends on the presynaptic firing rate. While the system lacks a longer-term average threshold, as the one present in BCM, the presynaptic activity acts as a value of reference for the significant level of activity. If the postsynaptic activity exceeds this level, then potentiation occurs; otherwise, depression arises.
For correlated spikes, we have shown that the model leads to similar results for hippocampal and cortical neurons, with an initial Hebbian behavior for small to medium frequencies and, depending on choice for the parameters, a reversal to anti-Hebbian behavior for large frequencies, which could have the virtue of being self-limiting, avoiding runaway growth of synaptic connections. It has been shown recently (Echeveste & Gros, 2014), that this self-limitation results naturally for rate-encoding neurons from the stationarity principle for Hebbian learning. By tuning the value of yc, the reversal frequency can be made larger, to the point of producing Hebbian behavior within the entire physiological range of activities.
The simplicity of our proposed model makes it a useful tool for simulations and studies of the dynamical properties of networks adapted via these rules. First, the calculations required are straightforward, making computing time efficient. The relative small number of free parameters and their direct link to both the biophysical properties of the postsynaptic complex and the dynamical features of the trace dynamics makes it suitable when studying the interplay between neural and synaptic dynamics in neural systems.
Appendix: Dimensionality Analysis
Acknowledgments
We acknowledge Robert C. Froemke and Yang Dan for the experimental data from cortical visual neurons. The support of the German Academic Exchange Service (DAAD) is acknowledged.