The brain continuously estimates the state of body and environment, with specific regions that are thought to act as Bayesian estimator, optimally integrating noisy and delayed sensory feedback with sensory predictions generated by the cerebellum. In control theory, Bayesian estimators are usually implemented using high-level representations. In this work, we designed a new spike-based computational model of a Bayesian estimator. The state estimator receives spiking activity from two neural populations encoding the sensory feedback and the cerebellar prediction, and it continuously computes the spike variability within each population as a reliability index of the signal these populations encode. The state estimator output encodes the current state estimate. We simulated a reaching task at different stages of cerebellar learning. The activity of the sensory feedback neurons encoded a noisy version of the trajectory after actual movement, with an almost constant intrapopulation spiking variability. Conversely, the activity of the cerebellar output neurons depended on the phase of the learning process. Before learning, they fired at their baseline not encoding any relevant information, and the variability was set to be higher than that of the sensory feedback (more reliable, albeit delayed). When learning was complete, their activity encoded the trajectory before the actual execution, providing an accurate sensory prediction; in this case, the variability was set to be lower than that of the sensory feedback. The state estimator model optimally integrated the neural activities of the afferent populations, so that the output state estimate was primarily driven by sensory feedback in prelearning and by the cerebellar prediction in postlearning. It was able to deal even with more complex scenarios, for example, by shifting the dominant source during the movement execution if information availability suddenly changed. The proposed tool will be a critical block within integrated spiking, brain-inspired control systems for simulations of sensorimotor tasks.

Humans can perform complex movements that require the coordination of many muscles and joints, automatically and unthinkingly (Thach, 1998). Even in noisy conditions (e.g., in foggy or dark environments) our brain can integrate multiple available sensory information with previous knowledge in order to estimate the current state of the body and the environment and to use this estimate to generate appropriate motor commands (Alessandro et al., 2016; Kawato, 1999; Shadmehr & Krakauer, 2008a; Wolpert, Goodbody, & Husain, 1998). This process of state estimation is essential to deal with the inherent delays in our sensory systems. As an example, during a tennis match, the information about the position of the ball extracted from the visual input becomes available to the central nervous system (CNS) with a delay of about 100 ms (Wolpert & Ghahramani, 2000). If not appropriately compensated, such noisy and delayed information would lead to inappropriate motor commands, eventually resulting in unsatisfactory movement execution. Instead, the CNS can filter the noisy sensory feedback and combine this information with fast sensory predictions that compensate for delays due to movement execution and feedback reafferences (Wolpert et al., 1998). In the previous example, the tennis player can successfully estimate the spin of the ball using reliable predictions generated by internal models of the ball and of the body dynamics, ultimately generating appropriate motor commands, not just compensating for delays but also in case of uncertain sensory feedbacks (e.g., foggy day) (Wolpert & Ghahramani, 2000).

Several brain areas are involved in this process. There is large evidence that the CNS computes sensory predictions by means of the cerebellum (Kawato, 1999; Kawato & Gomi, 1992; Shadmehr & Krakauer, 2008a; Wolpert et al., 1998). The cerebellum, acting as a forward model, learns to predict sensory consequences of actions using the planned motor commands received from the motor cortex through the efference copy (Miall & Wolpert, 1996; Popa & Ebner, 2019; Stein, 2009). Therefore, it facilitates fast and smooth coordination and aids accurate and well-timed sensorimotor execution and adaptation (Miall & Wolpert, 1996; Thach, 1998). Cerebellar predictions are integrated with the actual sensory information in a process that computes reliable estimates of the state of the body interacting with the environment. This process is thought to be carried out by the parietal cortex (Shadmehr & Krakauer, 2008a), which receives, through the thalamus, projections from both the deep cerebellar nuclei (output of the cerebellum; Palesi et al., 2014) and peripheral sensory structures (Dum, Levinthal, & Strick, 2009). Accordingly, it has been suggested that damages to the parietal cortex cause performance errors compatible with the inability to compute state estimates (Wolpert et al., 1998; Wolpert & Ghahramani, 2000).

Over the years, several theories have been proposed to explain how the CNS combines sensory information originating from the periphery and the cerebellum. It has been suggested that the CNS integrates these information sources through a process of Kalman filtering or Bayesian integration (Körding & Wolpert, 2004; Shadmehr & Krakauer, 2008a). These ideas have been investigated using computational models (de Xivry, Coppe, Blohm, & Lefèvre, 2013; Körding & Wolpert, 2004). However, given their abstract and high-level computational nature, these models provide little insight into the biological features of the underlying neural mechanisms. A more biologically plausible model of the neuromotor control system has been developed by Deneve et al. (2007), who implemented an optimal sensorimotor integration model for state estimation using recurrent neural networks of cortical circuits. However, how these processes may be implemented in spike-based systems remains an unresolved issue.

Here, we designed a spike-based state estimator model that operates with the naturalistic time coding representations typical of neuronal activity. The model receives spiking signals from two afferent neuronal populations, emulating the cerebellar output and the sensory feedback, and computes the spike synchronicity within each of these populations. These measures are used to define relative reliability of the incoming signals, allowing the computation of an optimal Bayesian estimation of the body state (Barak, 2017). The state estimator model was tested at different stages of the cerebellar learning process, emulated by modulating the spiking variability of the cerebellar output. Indeed, recent studies suggest that a strong increase of within-population synchronicity (i.e., a decrease of within-population variability) is a neural correlate of learning (Chervyakov et al., 2016; Sedaghat-Nejad, Pi, Hage, Fakharian, & Shadmehr, 2022). Accordingly, the synchronicity among cerebellar neurons is maximal at the end of the adaptation (Wagner et al., 2019).

Furthermore, we tested our model in more complex scenarios: (1) with a sudden interruption of the sensory feedback during movement execution in a middle-learning condition and (2) with an unexpected perturbation in postlearning. In all of these contexts, our model generated appropriate state estimates. In the future, we will integrate the state estimator proposed here with whole-brain models, including a realistic spiking network of the plastic cerebellar circuit (de Schepper et al., 2021; Geminiani et al., 2018; Geminiani, Pedrocchi, D'Angelo, & Casellato, 2019), paving the way of bio-inspired multiarea brain simulations in closed-loop controllers.

2.1  System Design

According to a well-established sensorimotor control model (Shadmehr & Krakauer, 2008a; Wolpert & Ghahramani, 2004), the state estimator block computes reliable estimates about the current state of the system. To this end, it integrates the sensory feedback originating from the receptors in the body with the sensory predictions computed by the cerebellum. While the former is affected by the inherent delays, the cerebellum provides, as a forward model, sensory predictions by exploiting an efference copy of the motor commands originating in the primary motor cortex (Miall & Wolpert, 1996). We therefore designed our state estimator block to receive inputs from two afferent neuronal populations (see Figure 1A). The neural activity of these afferent populations encoded the expected sensory feedback and cerebellar predictions during a reaching task, simulated by the movement of a point mass from a starting position to a desired target, in bidimensional space (mass of 1 kg). In order to simulate the delay of the sensory feedback signals, the trajectory of the point mass was delayed by 100 ms before translating it into the neural activity of the sensory population (“executed trajectory”).
Figure 1:

State estimator design. (A) Sensory reafferences from periphery and cerebellum project to the state estimator block, which in turn generates the optimal state signal. (B) Spiking architecture of the system. Each state estimator neuron (blue) receives spikes from sensory feedback and cerebellar output neurons. It stores the spikes into two separate buffers, counts the number of input spikes from each presynaptic neuron, and computes the “instantaneous” variability of each afferent population.

Figure 1:

State estimator design. (A) Sensory reafferences from periphery and cerebellum project to the state estimator block, which in turn generates the optimal state signal. (B) Spiking architecture of the system. Each state estimator neuron (blue) receives spikes from sensory feedback and cerebellar output neurons. It stores the spikes into two separate buffers, counts the number of input spikes from each presynaptic neuron, and computes the “instantaneous” variability of each afferent population.

Close modal

2.2  Neural Populations and Signal Encoding

The three neural populations—cerebellar output neurons, sensory feedback neurons, and state estimator neurons—were implemented as spiking neural networks (SNN) (Ghosh-Dastidar & Adeli, 2009) in NEST (Eppler et al., 2009; Jordan et al., 2019). The sensory feedback and cerebellar output neurons were modeled as Poisson single-point neurons whose spike trains were generated by applying a frequency coding strategy based on a Poisson distribution of spikes (Brette, Roland, Panzeri, & Graham, 2015). The controlled body was a point mass that could move over a bidimensional space: position at time t was identified by two variables x(t), y(t). Consistent with the foundational concept on neuronal population coding movement directions and the neural representation of variables with vectorial attributes (Georgopoulos, Schwartz, & Kettner, 1986), each population was subdivided into two subpopulations (x and y) and further divided into two groups (positive and negative), encoding the signal of opposite signs. The firing rates were linearly proportional to the position of the point mass along the specific axis and direction, plus a baseline firing rate of 50 Hz. This basal discharge corresponded to the initial neutral configuration and was considered the physiologically minimum neural activity (i.e., neurons functionally silent). Therefore, the position of the point mass on each axis could be decoded by computing the net firing rate (i.e., the difference between the firing rates of the positive and the negative groups) and then dividing this net rate by the gain factor. Each of these groups consisted of 100 neurons. In summary, there were 200 neurons for each axis, for a total of 400 sensory feedback neurons. The cerebellar output and state estimator populations were analogously organized. Therefore, the overall system consisted of 1200 neurons. This system is a proof-of-concept for the spiking implementation of the state estimator; in principle, it can be easily scaled in terms of multijoint tasks by increasing the number of neurons, subpopulations, and groups, accordingly.

2.3  State Estimation

The state estimator neurons exploited the theory of Bayesian inference to optimally integrate sensory feedback and cerebellar prediction. Bayes’ theorem asserts that a certain prior knowledge p(XPred) can be optimally integrated with new additional information p(XFbk) by considering its likelihood (Körding & Wolpert, 2004; Ma, Beck, Latham, & Pouget, 2006):
(2.1)
Here, p(XPred) represents the prior probability of the cerebellar prediction, p(XFbk) is the probability of the sensory feedback, and p(XFbk|XPred) represents the likelihood that the system state is equal to XFbk when the cerebellar prediction is XPred. The posterior distribution, p(XPred|XFbk) in equation 2.1 represents the optimal integration of the two sources of information. Assuming both p(XPred) and p(XFbk) follow a gaussian distribution with standard deviations σPred and σFbk, respectively, the optimal state estimate can be computed by maximizing the posterior probability, obtaining:
(2.2)
where σPred2/(σPred2 + σFbk2) represents the reliability of the current sensory feedback XFbk, σFbk2/(σPred2 + σFbk2) represents the reliability of the cerebellar prediction, and μPred is the average cerebellar prediction (i.e., mean of the prior). In other words, the optimal state estimate is the weighted sum of XFbk and μPred based on their relative variabilities.
To implement the spiking sensory feedback, we considered that according to physiological data (Wagner et al., 2019, 2021), the reliability of a spike-based signal strongly depends on the level of correlation and synchronicity across the neurons within the neural population coding that signal. Therefore, in order to estimate the reliability of the input signals, each state estimator neuron received input from all sensory feedback and cerebellar output neurons, collecting these spikes into two buffers (one for each afferent population; see Figure 1B). The position of the incoming spikes into the buffers depended on the unique ID of the presynaptic neuron, allowing us to keep track of the number of spikes received from each afferent neuron. For each buffer, the variance in the number of incoming spikes across neurons was computed within a 25 ms time window t, obtaining a time-varying signal. The two values of variance were then scaled by the average number of spikes recorded in each buffer, so getting values independent of the “instantaneous” population activity. The following equation was applied for the cerebellar output neurons:
(2.3)
where N is the number of neurons, XPredi is the sum of the input spikes received by the ith neuron from the cerebellar population, and μPred is the average number of spikes received from the cerebellar output neurons. An analogous formula was applied to the buffer of the sensory feedback neurons, obtaining σFbk from XFbki (the sum of the input spikes received by the ith neuron from the sensory feedback population) and μFbk (the average number of spikes received from the sensory feedback neurons).

Finally, these variabilities were used to compute the reliabilities of the afferent populations and the weighted sum described in equation 2.3. The obtained signal was then converted into spike patterns by the state estimator Poisson single-point neurons. The estimate can be read out as the net difference in firing rates between the positive and negative groups of the state estimator population.

2.4  Task Design and Tests

The state estimator model was evaluated in the context of a reaching movement of a point mass along the x-direction, from (0, 0) to (1, 0) m in 500 ms. The trajectory of the point mass was defined as a minimum-jerk fifth-order polynomial with bell-shaped velocity profile, the typical trajectory observed in humans during reaching tasks (Flash & Hogan, 1985). We decided to simulate these 1D movements for simplicity without loss of generality. However, our model allows the simulation of 2D movements, as illustrated in the supplementary material.

The variability among sensory feedback neurons was defined by the level of noise and the amount of the delay (Wolpert & Ghahramani, 2000), both of which affect the reliability of the sensory feedback (Wagner et al., 2021). In particular, each neuron was corrupted by a gaussian noise with zero mean and standard deviation dependent on the amplitude of the transmitted signal (i.e., signal-dependent noise; Clamann, 1969; Harris & Wolpert, 1998), such that their firing activity was slightly desynchronized as it occurs in real biological networks (Wagner et al., 2019). The amount of variability of the sensory feedback neurons was not altered depending on the stage of cerebellar learning. In contrast, it has been observed that the synchronicity among cerebellar neurons varies with learning (Casellato et al., 2014), showing its maximum at the end of the adaptation (Wagner et al., 2019). Therefore, we considered two scenarios: high variability to simulate inaccurate cerebellar predictions (prelearning), and low variability to simulate accurate cerebellar predictions (postlearning). Indeed, in the prelearning condition, the cerebellar neurons did not encode any task-relevant information, while in the postlearning condition, they encoded the planned trajectory, the way that the sensory feedback neurons do but without delay. Due to this lack of delay, postlearning cerebellar predictions should be more accurate than the sensory feedback (Shadmehr & Krakauer, 2008b); hence, we set the variability of the sensory feedback neurons to be higher than that of the cerebellar neurons. On the contrary, in the prelearning condition, we set the variability of the sensory feedback neurons to be lower than that of the cerebellar neurons.

Two further tests were carried out in order to challenge the state estimator model in the face of unexpected perturbations. First, a sudden switch-off of the sensory feedback during the movement was simulated. A reaching trajectory from (0, 0) to (1, 0) m was simulated over a period of 2 seconds. In this simulation, a scenario of intermediate cerebellar adaptation was defined by setting the cerebellar output neuron variability to be slightly higher than the feedback neuron variability. At half movement (t = 1 s), the sensory feedback was switched off (simulated with null input trajectory and variability set to infinite), leaving the cerebellar prediction, still not completely reliable as in postlearning, as the only source of information. This simulation allowed us to test the robustness of the state estimator output when the reliability of the afferent populations changed during the movement.

Second, a mismatch between the planned and the executed trajectory was simulated in postlearning (e.g., visuo-motor rotation after cerebellar adaptation). Since the postlearning condition simulates a scenario in which the cerebellum was already adapted, the cerebellar output neurons accurately (i.e., low interneuron variability) encoded the planned trajectory, from (0, 0) to (1, 0) m in 0.5 s. At the same time, however, the sensory feedback neurons encoded the actual movement under the perturbation: from (0, 0) to (−1, 0) m in 0.5 s. Therefore, the state estimator received inconsistent information from the afferent neural populations, both with a good level of reliability.

3.1  Neural Coding of the Afferent Populations to the State Estimator

The two afferent populations to the state estimator emulated the sensory feedback and the cerebellar output, and provided the state estimator with spike-based signals according to a rate-based coding during movement (see Figure 2). All results relate to one subpopulation for each neural population, as the movement occurred along the x-axis. Since the reaching movement was not perturbed in this simulation, the spike patterns of the sensory feedback neurons at pre- and postlearning conditions were very similar to each other (except for minor differences due to the Poisson characteristics of the neuron activity). Since the movement evolved in the positive direction along x-axis, the neural activity of the positive group increased along movement while that of the negative group remained constant to its background level (approximately 50 Hz). The net firing rate of the two groups encoded the executed trajectory, with the inherent delay of the movement actuation and sensory feedback loop (see Figure 2A).
Figure 2:

Spiking activity of the afferent neural populations. The first column reports the raster plots and the second column the corresponding population rate-based signals (computed with time bins of 25 ms). (A) Sensory feedback subpopulation neurons (encoding the movement along the x-axis) (prelearning ≈ postlearning). While the negative group shows a constant background firing rate (approximately 50 Hz), the positive group generates a number of spikes that depend on the progressive moving away of the point mass along the x-axis from the starting point toward the desired target. The difference in firing rate between positive and negative groups encodes the actual position profile, which ranges from 0 to 1 m (dashed black line). (B) Cerebellar output subpopulation neurons (encoding the movement along x-axis) in prelearning. Both groups show a constant background firing rate (∼50 Hz); therefore, the net activity does not encode any relevant information. (C) Cerebellar output subpopulation neurons (encoding the movement along the x-axis) in postlearning. While the negative group shows a constant background firing rate (about 50 Hz), the positive group generates a number of spikes that depend on the progressive moving away of the point mass along the x-axis, from the starting point toward the desired target. The net activity encodes the predicted position profile, which ranges from 0 to 1 m (dashed black line). Note that this predicted position (“planned trajectory”) (given as the analog input to the cerebellar output neurons in postlearning) anticipates by 100 ms the actual position (“executed trajectory”), given as analog input to the sensory feedback neurons).

Figure 2:

Spiking activity of the afferent neural populations. The first column reports the raster plots and the second column the corresponding population rate-based signals (computed with time bins of 25 ms). (A) Sensory feedback subpopulation neurons (encoding the movement along the x-axis) (prelearning ≈ postlearning). While the negative group shows a constant background firing rate (approximately 50 Hz), the positive group generates a number of spikes that depend on the progressive moving away of the point mass along the x-axis from the starting point toward the desired target. The difference in firing rate between positive and negative groups encodes the actual position profile, which ranges from 0 to 1 m (dashed black line). (B) Cerebellar output subpopulation neurons (encoding the movement along x-axis) in prelearning. Both groups show a constant background firing rate (∼50 Hz); therefore, the net activity does not encode any relevant information. (C) Cerebellar output subpopulation neurons (encoding the movement along the x-axis) in postlearning. While the negative group shows a constant background firing rate (about 50 Hz), the positive group generates a number of spikes that depend on the progressive moving away of the point mass along the x-axis, from the starting point toward the desired target. The net activity encodes the predicted position profile, which ranges from 0 to 1 m (dashed black line). Note that this predicted position (“planned trajectory”) (given as the analog input to the cerebellar output neurons in postlearning) anticipates by 100 ms the actual position (“executed trajectory”), given as analog input to the sensory feedback neurons).

Close modal

It is known that the spike patterns of the cerebellar output neurons (namely, deep cerebellar nuclei cells) are modulated according to the learning stage in terms of timing, amplitude, and synchronization (Antonietti, Martina, Casellato, D'Angelo, & Pedrocchi, 2019; Casellato et al., 2014). At prelearning, when the cerebellum had not yet encoded any task-relevant information, all neurons fired at the constant background rate (see Figure 2B). On the contrary, at postlearning, the cerebellar output neurons encoded the predicted movement, with the positive group gradually increasing its activity during movement trial and the negative group maintaining a constant background activity (see Figure 2C; Casellato et al., 2014; Naveros et al., 2019). Similar results were obtained on both subpopulations (x- and y-axes) during bidimensional movement (see Figure S1).

If there are no perturbations, the net firing rate between the positive and the negative cerebellar output neurons is therefore predictive of the executed trajectory without delay.

3.2  Reliability of the Afferent Signals to the State Estimator

The state estimator neurons computed the spike-rate variability within each of the afferent populations (see equation 2.3). As expected (see section 2), in the prelearning condition, the variability of the sensory prediction was higher than that of the sensory feedback at almost all time samples of the movement trial (see Figure 3A). On average, σFbk was 61 ± 8 Hz and 62 ± 7 Hz for the positive and negative groups, respectively, while σPred was 76 ± 9 Hz and 76 ± 14 Hz for the positive and negative groups, respectively (see Figure 3A, left). Conversely, in the postlearning condition, the variability of the sensory prediction was lower than that of the sensory feedback in almost all time samples of the movement trial (see Figure 3B, left). On average, σFbk was 64 ± 8 Hz and 69 ± 9 Hz for the positive and negative groups, respectively, while σPred was lower than at prelearning, 49 ± 7.0 Hz and 50 ± 8 Hz for the positive and negative groups, respectively (see Figure 3C, left). The reliability values (i.e., the relative weights in equation 2.2) were then computed at each time sample (see Figure 3, right column). The reliability of the cerebellar prediction was lower than that of the sensory feedback at prelearning (0.45 versus 0.55 for the cerebellum and the sensory feedback positive groups, respectively) but not at postlearning (e.g., 0.57 versus 0.43). In other words, the state estimator considered the information from the sensory feedback (affected by the delay) more trustworthy than the cerebellar prediction before learning and the cerebellar prediction more trustworthy than the sensory feedback after learning.
Figure 3:

Variability and reliability of the afferent neural populations. Time-varying spike variability of sensory feedback and cerebellar output neurons (positive and negative groups for each) in the prelearning (A) and postlearning (B) conditions. The corresponding reliability time profiles are reported on the right. These reliability values are then used as weights for the “continuous” Bayesian integration. Finally, the mean and standard deviation of variability and reliability are computed (C) across a lengthy task duration.

Figure 3:

Variability and reliability of the afferent neural populations. Time-varying spike variability of sensory feedback and cerebellar output neurons (positive and negative groups for each) in the prelearning (A) and postlearning (B) conditions. The corresponding reliability time profiles are reported on the right. These reliability values are then used as weights for the “continuous” Bayesian integration. Finally, the mean and standard deviation of variability and reliability are computed (C) across a lengthy task duration.

Close modal

3.3  Output of the State Estimator in Stable and Perturbed Environments

The reliability of the afferent populations was used by the state estimator to generate the output neural activity that encoded the estimated state of the body (see Figure 4). In both pre- and postlearning conditions, the neural activity of the positive group of the state estimator neurons was modulated during the movement trial, while the neural activity of the negative group was stable around a background firing rate. This behavior was consistent with the movement, which evolved in the positive direction. Furthermore, it suggests that the model generated state estimates independently on the stage of cerebellar learning.
Figure 4:

Spiking activity of the state estimator population. The raster plot and the corresponding population rate signals (computed with time bins of 25 ms) of the state estimator neurons are reported in the prelearning (A) and postlearning (B) conditions. While the negative group shows a constant background firing rate (∼50 Hz), the positive group generates a number of spikes that depend on the progressive moving away of the point mass along x-axis. The corresponding net activity (blue rate profile) is reported on the right and compared with the net activity of the sensory feedback and cerebellar output populations.

Figure 4:

Spiking activity of the state estimator population. The raster plot and the corresponding population rate signals (computed with time bins of 25 ms) of the state estimator neurons are reported in the prelearning (A) and postlearning (B) conditions. While the negative group shows a constant background firing rate (∼50 Hz), the positive group generates a number of spikes that depend on the progressive moving away of the point mass along x-axis. The corresponding net activity (blue rate profile) is reported on the right and compared with the net activity of the sensory feedback and cerebellar output populations.

Close modal

A closer look at the net firing rate of the state estimator output allows us to better describe the behavior of the model in the two learning conditions. In the prelearning condition, when the cerebellum did not provide task-relevant information, the output of the state estimator preferentially followed the sensory feedback (see Figure 4A, right), which was more reliable than the cerebellar prediction despite its inherent delay. As a result, the generated estimate was delayed with respect to the planned trajectory. In the postlearning condition, the state estimator mainly relied on the cerebellar prediction. As a result, the firing rate of the state estimator neurons increased earlier than in the prelearning condition (see Figure 4B left), and the generated estimate matched the planned trajectory with no delay (see Figure 4B right).

While in the experiments above, the reliability of the afferent populations was kept constant throughout the movement trial, the proposed state estimator model computes the reliability continuously. This feature allows the system to continuously modulate the relative weights of the two afferent populations and therefore generate a robust state estimate despite possible changes of environmental or internal contexts during the movement trial. When the sensory feedback became suddenly unavailable from midtrial, the state estimator continued to robustly provide state estimates solely based on the cerebellar prediction, which became the most reliable source of information, even if the cerebellar learning was still incomplete (see Figure 5).
Figure 5:

State estimation in changing environment: a sudden unavailability of the sensory feedback. (A) The net population rate signals of sensory feedback, cerebellar output, and state estimator populations (computed with time bins of 25 ms) during a reaching trial (lasting 2 s) in an intermediate cerebellar learning condition. (B) The sensory feedback is suddenly interrupted at half trial duration.

Figure 5:

State estimation in changing environment: a sudden unavailability of the sensory feedback. (A) The net population rate signals of sensory feedback, cerebellar output, and state estimator populations (computed with time bins of 25 ms) during a reaching trial (lasting 2 s) in an intermediate cerebellar learning condition. (B) The sensory feedback is suddenly interrupted at half trial duration.

Close modal
Furthermore, we tested a scenario in which sensory feedback and cerebellar predictions had comparable reliability but encoded inconsistent information. To this end, we introduced an unpredictable perturbation that created a mismatch between predicted and executed trajectory in postlearning, when both afferent populations to the state estimator were comparably reliable (see Figure 6). Since these afferent signals were equal in magnitude with opposite signs, there was activity in both positive and negative state estimator neurons (see Figure 6A). As a result, the decoded state estimate (i.e., net firing rate of the neurons) was almost the average between cerebellar prediction and sensory feedback (see Figure 6B). This inaccurate state estimation was expected and would be adjusted during cerebellar retraining as detailed in section 4.
Figure 6:

State estimation in changing environment: an unexpected perturbation. Raster plot of state estimator neurons (left) and corresponding net activity (blue rate profile) compared with the net activity of the sensory feedback and cerebellar output populations (right), in postlearning conditions with the action of a sudden force field causing a movement in the opposite direction. Both cerebellar prediction and sensory feedback convey reliable information in this case. As a result, the state estimator provides an intermediate signal with both groups (positive and negative) firing since sensory feedback and cerebellar prediction have opposite signs; therefore, the resulting net activity of the state estimator neurons is an almost flat trajectory.

Figure 6:

State estimation in changing environment: an unexpected perturbation. Raster plot of state estimator neurons (left) and corresponding net activity (blue rate profile) compared with the net activity of the sensory feedback and cerebellar output populations (right), in postlearning conditions with the action of a sudden force field causing a movement in the opposite direction. Both cerebellar prediction and sensory feedback convey reliable information in this case. As a result, the state estimator provides an intermediate signal with both groups (positive and negative) firing since sensory feedback and cerebellar prediction have opposite signs; therefore, the resulting net activity of the state estimator neurons is an almost flat trajectory.

Close modal

In this work, we implemented a spiking neural network model of a state estimator based on Bayesian integration theory. Our model is a proof-of-concept and was tested here as an isolated block, receiving spike-encoded input signals. When no reliable cerebellar predictions were available (prelearning), the model estimated body states mainly relying on (delayed) sensory feedback. In this scenario, the cerebellar network, acting as a forward internal model, is still untrained and its output is unreliable and noisy. In more advanced stages of the learning process, the cerebellum acquires an accurate internal model of the body and environment, providing a reliable prediction of the planned movement with negligible noise and delay (Freeman, 2014; Ito, 2000). Accordingly, in the postlearning condition, our model estimated the body state using cerebellar prediction as the dominant source of information. This work provides a useful tool that can be used within spiking bio-inspired sensorimotor controllers for the simulation of motor tasks and could be easily integrated into existing brain models.

Here, the functionalities of the proposed state estimator model were tested in isolation from the other brain areas (e.g., the cerebellum and the sensory feedback). To do so, we had to make assumptions on the statistics of these areas’ neural activity based on literature: in the prelearning condition, the reliability of the (delayed) sensory feedback was higher than that of the cerebellar prediction (because before learning, the cerebellum provides erroneous predictions; Shadmehr & Krakauer, 2008b); in the postlearning condition, on the contrary, the reliability of the sensory feedback was set to be lower than that of the cerebellum, which provides accurate predictions with no delay, showing a strong interneuron variability (Wagner et al., 2019). These assumptions allowed us to illustrate that the proposed model indeed worked in accordance with the Bayesian integration theory, considering the dominant source of information as that with the highest reliability.

In biological sensorimotor systems, sensory feedback is typically noisy and delayed due to latencies in information processing (Wolpert & Ghahramani, 2000) or even absent due to the lack of the appropriate sensory receptors and excessive movement speed (e.g., in ballistic movements like saccades; Abrams, Meyer, & Kornblum, 1989). Compensating for the delay or the lack of sensory feedback requires a process of state estimation that uses internal prediction about the time-varying body state. Based on the hypothesis that an optimal state estimation should exploit the variability of the incoming spike trains (Scott, 2002), the state estimator model implemented here extracted such variability metrics from sensory feedback and cerebellar output neurons to evaluate the reliability of the signals encoded by these two neural populations. This implies that along the learning process, the cerebellar output signal should acquire an advantageous signal-to-noise ratio (Wagner et al., 2021). This coherent spike activity of the cerebellar output neurons occurs when the cerebellum becomes able, throughout the repetition of several movement trials, to translate the motor efference copy it received into accurate and well-timed predictions of the corresponding sensory consequences (Shadmehr, 2017; Tseng et al., 2007).

By integrating the two source signals by means of their relative reliabilities, the spiking state estimator model was able to properly move from an initial state estimation relying only on the noisy and delayed sensory feedback in the prelearning condition (when the cerebellar spike trains were strongly desynchronized), to an accurate predictive estimate of the upcoming state of the moving body in the postlearning condition. It is worth noting that even after learning, the overall 100 ms delay was not completely compensated since a nonzero weight was set for the sensory feedback signal to face any possible further unpredictable event. Importantly, the continuous computation of the reliability measures during movement execution allows the system to deal with unexpected changes of environmental or internal contexts, such as a sudden interruption of the sensory feedback or an unpredictable perturbation (Haith & Krakauer, 2013).

It could be claimed that activity correlations may be associated with a reduction of the encoded stimulus information (e.g., impaired perceptual discrimination). However, as discussed in Valente et al. (2021), correlations are higher when correct choices and movements are made, thus showing that the effects of correlations in enhancing decoding of behavioral choices from sensory information overcome their detrimental information-limiting effects.

Accordingly, the basic assumption of our work is that correlation is associated with consistency of information across neurons and time, and therefore it is maximal when proper motor responses are learned (Valente et al., 2021). Synchronization of spikes among a group of neurons is indeed a special form of temporal coding (Sedaghat-Nejad et al., 2022).

4.1  Limitations

In our model, signals amplitude was encoded in the firing rates of direction-dependent neural subpopulations (Georgopoulos et al., 1986). Each subpopulation was in turn divided into positive and negative groups, encoding signals of opposite signs (e.g., agonist and antagonist signals) The neuron baseline value represents the background activity of a functionally silent neuron (ten Brinke et al., 2017).

In our simulations, the baseline firing rate was set for all neurons to 50 Hz to ensure a sustained neural activity in all neurons, hence guaranteeing a good resolution in signal encoding even with a relatively low number of neurons. However, in future implementations with large-scale brain models and more proper population numerosity, this baseline rate could be flexibly set to match in vivo recordings, differentiating each neuronal population.

4.2  Future Work

The results obtained here provide a solid basis for future investigations on how spiking neural mechanisms and interaction of different brain areas generate accurate and timely motor commands. Indeed, the state estimator block has been implemented as a spiking processing unit, complementing current models based on high-level representations of cortical computations or artificial neural networks (Lanillos & van Gerven, 2021; Parrell, Ramanarayanan, Nagarajan, & Houde, 2019; Xu, Hu, Han, & Zhang, 2021). The spiking approach increases the biological realism of the model and paves the way to future studies on how the brain performs the integration of sensory feedback and sensory prediction, with a level of neuronal firing variability and synchronicity strongly informative, which depends on learning stage and environmental context.

In future work, a full spiking model of the cerebellar microcircuit endowed with plasticity rules, which was previously tuned and validated on experimental data (Casali, Marenzi, Medini, Casellato, & D'Angelo, 2019; de Schepper et al., 2021; Geminiani et al., 2019), could be connected to the spiking state estimator. In this construct, the cerebellar prediction will emerge from adaptive circuit processing throughout task repetition (D'Angelo et al., 2016). This will allow us to let the spiking variability of cerebellar neuronal populations to evolve along with the acquisition of an accurate internal dynamic model. Indeed, during the formation of internal predictions, the cerebellar circuit undergoes an adaptation process based on the error between the predicted body movement and the actual movement revealed by sensory afferences. This “sensory prediction error” would be conveyed to the cerebellum through the inferior olive circuit.

We showed that in the face of unexpected perturbations in the postlearning condition, our model does not generate an estimate that corresponds to the actual executed movement. If the model was embedded in a complete sensorimotor control loop (Shadmehr & Krakauer, 2008b) in such a situation, the difference between the sensory feedback and cerebellar prediction (i.e., sensory prediction error; Tseng, Diedrichsen, Krakauer, Shadmehr, & Bastian, 2007) would generate activity in the inferior olive, signaling inaccurate predictions and triggering plastic processes that would eventually allow compensating for the perturbation. This error would increase the variability of the cerebellar output neurons, potentially due to the olivo-DCN collaterals (Lu, Yang, & Jaeger, 2016). As a result, the state estimator would start to preferentially follow the sensory feedback until the cerebellum provided new, accurate predictions (i.e., reincreased reliability). Experimental recordings from parietal cortex and cerebellum neurons in behaving mice could be fundamental to validating this process.

The spiking state estimator could also be introduced in a control system embedding spiking models of different brain areas wired using connectome data (Oh et al., 2014). This will interestingly generate sensorimotor behaviors and allow monitoring the underlying dynamics of all involved neuronal populations. This modular system could embed blocks at different scales and levels of neuronal detail and could be used to control more complex dynamic bodies, with several degrees of freedom interacting with the environment in realistic scenarios. Using this general system to control detailed models of the musculoskeletal apparatus will be instrumental to investigating open issues in the intricate relationship between neural control and musculoskeletal biomechanics (e.g., the emergence of muscle synergies; Alessandro, Carbajal, & d'Avell, 2012), and the sensory integration for the regulation of internal joint loading (Barroso, Alessandro, & Tresch, 2019). Finally, the implemented system could be used to control neurorobots, exploiting the already available interfaces with the software MUSIC to synchronize the brain controller and the actuation of a robotic body (Weidel, Djurfeldt, Duarte, & Morrison, 2016) and can be embedded into neurorobotic environments like the Neurorobotics Platform (Falotico et al., 2017) or real robots (Antonietti et al., 2019; Casellato et al., 2014).

The spiking state estimator code implemented here is available as open source at the following repository: https://github.com/dbbs-lab/state-estimator. Starting from the proof-of-concept applications tested here, it could be generalized to more complex scenarios (with additional encoded variables and increased number of neurons) and/or embedded in closed-loop control systems to simulate the full control of actions resulting from the coordinated activity of multiple brain areas.

This research has received funding from the European Union's Horizon 2020 Framework Programme for Research and Innovation under the specific grant agreement 945539 (Human Brain Project SGA3) and was supported by the EBRAINS platform and the ICEI-FENIX research infrastructure.

Abrams
,
R. A.
,
Meyer
,
D. E.
, &
Kornblum
,
S.
(
1989
).
Speed and accuracy of saccadic eye movements: Characteristics of impulse variability in the oculomotor system
.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
,
15
(
3
),
529
543
.
[PubMed]
Alessandro
,
C.
,
Beckers
,
N.
,
Goebel
,
P.
,
Resquin
,
F.
,
González
,
J.
, &
Osu
,
R
. (
2016
).
Motor control and learning theories
.
Biosystems and Biorobotics
,
10
,
225
250
.
Alessandro
C.
,
Carbajal
J. P.
, &
d'Avella
A.
(
2012
).
Synthesis and adaptation of effective motor synergies for the solution of reaching tasks
.
Lecture Notes in Artificial Intelligence
.
Berlin
:
Springer
.
Antonietti
,
A.
,
Martina
,
D.
,
Casellato
,
C.
,
D'Angelo
,
E.
, &
Pedrocchi
,
A
. (
2019
).
Control of a humanoid NAO robot by an adaptive bioinspired cerebellar module in 3D motion tasks
.
Computational Intelligence and Neuroscience
,
2019
,
1
15
.
Barak
,
O.
(
2017
).
Recurrent neural networks as versatile tools of neuroscience research
.
Current Opinion in Neurobiology
,
46
,
1
6
.
[PubMed]
Barroso
,
F. O.
,
Alessandro
,
C.
, &
Tresch
,
M. C.
(
2019
).
Adaptation of muscle activation after patellar loading demonstrates neural control of joint variables
.
Scientific Reports
,
9
(
1
),
1
12
.
[PubMed]
Brette
,
R.
,
Roland
,
P. E.
,
Panzeri
,
S.
, &
Graham
,
L. J.
(
2015
).
Philosophy of the spike: Rate-based versus spike-based theories of the brain
.
Casali
,
S.
,
Marenzi
,
E.
,
Medini
,
C.
,
Casellato
,
C.
, &
D'Angelo
,
E
. (
2019
).
Reconstruction and simulation of a scaffold model of the cerebellar network
.
Frontiers in Neuroinformatics
,
13
,
37
.
[PubMed]
Casellato
,
C.
,
Antonietti
,
A.
,
Garrido
,
J. A.
,
Carrillo
,
R. R.
,
Luque
,
N. R.
,
Ros
,
E.
, …
D'Angelo
,
E.
(
2014
).
Adaptive robotic control driven by a versatile spiking cerebellar network
.
PLOS One
,
9
(
11
),
1
17
.
Chervyakov
,
A. V.
,
Sinitsyn
,
D. O.
,
Piradov
,
M. A.
,
Lebedev
,
M.
,
Merchant
,
H.
, &
Sakurai
,
Y.
(
2016
).
Variability of neuronal responses: Types and functional significance in neuroplasticity and neural Darwinism
.
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience
,
10
(
603
).
[PubMed]
Clamann
,
H. P.
(
1969
).
Statistical analysis of motor unit firing patterns in a human skeletal muscle
.
Biophysical Journal
,
9
(
10
).
D'Angelo
,
E.
,
Antonietti
,
A.
,
Casali
,
S.
,
Casellato
,
C.
,
Garrido
,
J. A.
,
Luque
,
N. R.
, …
Ros
,
E.
(
2016
).
Modeling the cerebellar microcircuit: New strategies for a long-standing issue
.
Frontiers in Cellular Neuroscience
,
10
(
July
),
1
29
.
de Schepper
,
R.
,
Geminiani
,
A.
,
Masoli
,
S.
,
Rizza
,
M. F.
,
Antonietti
,
A.
,
Casellato
,
C.
, &
D'Angelo
,
E
. (
2021
).
Scaffold modelling captures the structure-function-dynamics relationship in brain microcircuits. bioRxiv.
de Xivry
,
J.-J. O.
,
Coppe
,
S.
,
Blohm
,
G.
, &
Lefèvre
,
P.
(
2013
).
Kalman filtering naturally accounts for visually guided and predictive smooth pursuit dynamics
.
Journal of Neuroscience
,
33
(
44
),
17301
17313
.
[PubMed]
Denève
,
S.
,
Duhamel
,
J.-R.
, &
Pouget
,
A.
(
2007
).
Optimal sensorimotor integration in recurrent cortical networks: A neural implementation of Kalman filters
.
Journal of Neuroscience
,
27
(
21
),
5744
5756
.
Dum
,
R. P.
,
Levinthal
,
D. J.
, &
Strick
,
P. L.
(
2009
).
The spinothalamic system targets motor and sensory areas in the cerebral cortex of monkeys
.
Journal of Neuroscience
,
29
(
45
),
14223
14235
.
[PubMed]
Eppler
,
J. M.
,
Helias
,
M.
,
Muller
,
E.
,
Diesmann
,
M.
,
Gewaltig
,
M.
, &
Stewart
,
T. C.
(
2009
).
PyNEST: A convenient interface to the NEST simulator
,
2
(
January
),
1
12
.
Falotico
,
E.
,
Vannucci
,
L.
,
Ambrosano
,
A.
,
Albanese
,
U.
,
Ulbrich
,
S.
,
Vasquez Tieck
,
J. C.
, …
Gewaltig
,
M.-O.
(
2017
).
Connecting artificial brains to robots in a comprehensive simulation framework: The neurorobotics platform
.
Frontiers in Neurorobotics
,
11
(
2
).
[PubMed]
Flash
,
T.
, &
Hogan
,
N.
(
1985
).
The coordination of arm movements: An experimentally confirmed mathematical model
.
Journal of Neuroscience
,
5
(
7
),
1688
1703
.
[PubMed]
Freeman
,
J. H.
(
2014
).
Cerebellar learning mechanisms
.
Brain Research
,
1621
,
1
10
.
[PubMed]
Geminiani
,
A.
,
Casellato
,
C.
,
Locatelli
,
F.
,
Prestori
,
F.
,
Pedrocchi
,
A.
, &
D'Angelo
,
E
. (
2018
).
Complex dynamics in simplified neuronal models: Reproducing Golgi cell electroresponsiveness
.
Frontiers in Neuroinformatics
,
12
(
88
),
1
19
.
[PubMed]
Geminiani
,
A.
,
Pedrocchi
,
A.
,
D'Angelo
,
E.
, &
Casellato
,
C
. (
2019
).
Response dynamics in an olivocerebellar spiking neural network with nonlinear neuron properties
.
Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience
,
13
(
68
).
Georgopoulos
,
A. P.
,
Schwartz
,
A. B.
, &
Kettner
,
R. E.
(
1986
).
Neuronal population coding of movement direction
.
Science
,
233
(
4771
),
1416
1419
.
[PubMed]
Ghosh-Dastidar
,
S.
, &
Adeli
,
H.
(
2009
).
Spiking neural networks
.
International Journal of Neural Systems
,
19
(
4
),
295
308
.
[PubMed]
Haith
,
A. M.
, &
Krakauer
,
J. W.
(
2013
).
Model-based and model-free mechanisms of human motor learning
.
Advances in Experimental Medicine and Biology
,
782
,
1
21
.
[PubMed]
Harris
,
C. M.
, &
Wolpert
,
D. M.
(
1998
).
Signal-dependent noise determines motor planning
.
Nature
,
394
(
6695
),
780
784
.
[PubMed]
Ito
,
M.
(
2000
).
Mechanisms of motor learning in the cerebellum
.
Brain Research
,
886
,
237
245
.
[PubMed]
Jordan
,
J.
,
Mørk
,
H.
,
Vennemo
,
S. B.
,
Terhorst
,
D.
,
Peyser
,
A.
,
Ippen
,
T.
,
Deepu
,
R.
, …
Plesser
,
H. E.
(
2019
).
NEST 2.18.0.
Kawato
,
M.
(
1999
).
Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning
.
Current Opinion in Neurobiology
,
9
(
6
),
718
727
.
[PubMed]
Kawato
,
M.
, &
Gomi
,
H.
(
1992
).
The cerebellum and VOR/OKR learning models
.
Trends in Neurosciences
,
15
(
11
),
445
453
.
[PubMed]
Körding
,
K. P.
, &
Wolpert
,
D. M.
(
2004
).
Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning
.
Nature
,
427
(
6971
),
244
247
.
Lanillos
,
P.
, &
van Gerven
,
M.
(
2021
).
Neuroscience-inspired perception-action in robotics: Applying active inference for state estimation, control and self-perception. arXiv:2105.04261
.
Lu
,
H.
,
Yang
,
B.
, &
Jaeger
,
D.
(
2016
).
Cerebellar nuclei neurons show only small excitatory responses to optogenetic olivary stimulation in transgenic mice: In vivo and in vitro studies
.
Frontiers in Neural Circuits
,
10
,
21
.
[PubMed]
Ma
,
W. J.
,
Beck
,
J. M.
,
Latham
,
P. E.
, &
Pouget
,
A.
(
2006
).
Bayesian inference with probabilistic population codes
.
Nature Neuroscience
,
9
(
11
).
Miall
,
R. C.
, &
Wolpert
,
D. M.
(
1996
).
Forward models for physiological motor control
.
Neural Networks
,
9
(
8
),
1265
1279
.
[PubMed]
Naveros
,
F.
,
Luque
,
N. R.
,
Ros
,
E.
, &
Arleo
,
A.
(
2019
).
VOR adaptation on a humanoid iCub robot using a spiking cerebellar model
.
IEEE Transactions on Cybernetics
,
1
14
.
Oh
,
S. W.
,
Harris
,
J. A.
,
Ng
,
L.
,
Winslow
,
B.
,
Cain
,
N.
,
Mihalas
,
S.
, …
Zeng
,
H.
(
2014
).
A mesoscale connectome of the mouse brain
.
Nature
,
508
(
7495
),
207
214
.
[PubMed]
Palesi
,
F.
,
Tournier
,
J.-D.
,
Calamante
,
F.
,
Muhlert
,
N.
,
Castellazzi
,
G.
,
Chard
,
D.
, …
Wheeler-Kingshott
,
C. A. M.
(
2014
).
Contralateral cerebello-thalamo-cortical pathways with prominent involvement of associative areas in humans in vivo
.
Brain Structure and Function
,
220
(
6
),
3369
3384
.
[PubMed]
Parrell
,
B.
,
Ramanarayanan
,
V.
,
Nagarajan
,
S.
, &
Houde
,
J.
(
2019
).
The FACTS model of speech motor control: Fusing state estimation and task-based control
.
PLOS Computational Biology
,
15
(
9
).
[PubMed]
Popa
,
L. S.
, &
Ebner
,
T.  J.
(
2019
).
Cerebellum, predictions and errors
.
Frontiers in Cellular Neuroscience
,
12
,
524
.
[PubMed]
Scott
,
S. H.
(
2002
).
Optimal strategies for movement: Success with variability
.
Nature Neuroscience
,
5
(
11
).
Sedaghat-Nejad
,
E.
,
Pi
,
J. S.
,
Hage
,
P.
,
Fakharian
,
M. A.
, &
Shadmehr
,
R.
(
2022
).
Synchronous spiking of cerebellar Purkinje cells during control of movements
. In
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
,
119
(
14
).
Shadmehr
,
R.
(
2017
).
Learning to predict and control the physics of our movements
.
Journal of Neuroscience
,
37
(
7
),
1663
1671
.
[PubMed]
Shadmehr
,
R.
, &
Krakauer
,
J. W.
(
2008a
).
A computational neuroanatomy for motor control
.
Experimental Brain Research
,
185
(
3
),
359
381
.
Shadmehr
,
R.
, &
Krakauer
,
J. W.
(
2008b
).
A computational neuroanatomy for motor control
.
Experimental Brain Research
,
185
(
3
),
359
381
.
Stein
,
J.
(
2009
).
Cerebellar forward models to control movement
.
Journal of Physiology
,
587
.
[PubMed]
ten Brinke
,
M. M.
,
Heiney
,
S. A.
,
Wang
,
X.
,
Proietti-Onori
,
M.
,
Boele
,
H. J.
,
Bakermans
,
J.
, …
de Zeeuw
,
C. I.
(
2017
).
Dynamic modulation of activity in cerebellar nuclei neurons during Pavlovian eyeblink conditioning in mice
.
eLife
,
6
,
1
27
.
Thach
,
W. T.
(
1998
).
A role for the cerebellum in learning movement coordination
.
Neurobiology of Learning and Memory
,
70
(
1–2
),
177
188
.
[PubMed]
Tseng
,
Y.
,
Diedrichsen
,
J.
,
Krakauer
,
J. W.
,
Shadmehr
,
R.
, &
Bastian
,
A. J.
(
2007
).
Sensory prediction errors drive cerebellum-dependent adaptation of reaching
.
Journal of Neurophysiology
,
98
(
1
),
54
62
.
[PubMed]
Valente
,
M.
,
Pica
,
G.
,
Bondanelli
,
G.
,
Moroni
,
M.
,
Runyan
,
C. A.
,
Morcos
,
A. S.
, …
Panzeri
,
S.
(
2021
).
Correlations enhance the behavioral readout of neural population activity in association cortex
.
Nature Neuroscience
,
24
(
7
),
975
986
.
[PubMed]
Wagner
,
M. J.
,
Kim
,
T. H.
,
Kadmon
,
J.
,
Nguyen
,
N. D.
,
Ganguli
,
S.
,
Schnitzer
,
M. J.
, &
Luo
,
L.
(
2019
).
Shared cortex-cerebellum dynamics in the execution and learning of a motor task
.
Cell
,
177
(
3
),
669
682
.
Wagner
,
M. J.
,
Savall
,
J.
,
Hernandez
,
O.
,
Mel
,
G.
,
Inan
,
H.
,
Rumyantsev
, …
Schnitzer
,
M. J
. (
2021
).
A neural circuit state change underlying skilled movements
.
Cell
,
184
(
14
),
3731
3747
.
Weidel
,
P.
,
Djurfeldt
,
M.
,
Duarte
,
R. C.
, &
Morrison
,
A.
(
2016
).
Closed loop interactions between spiking neural network and robotic simulators based on MUSIC and ROS
.
Frontiers in Neuroinformatics
,
10
(
31
).
Wolpert
,
D.
,
Goodbody
,
S.
, &
Husain
,
M.
(
1998
).
Maintaining internal representations: The role of the human superior parietal lobe
.
Nature Neuroscience
,
1
(
6
),
529
533
.
[PubMed]
Wolpert
,
D. M.
, &
Ghahramani
,
Z.
(
2000
).
Computational principles of movement neuroscience
.
Nature Neuroscience
3
(
11
),
1212
1217
.
[PubMed]
Wolpert
,
D. M.
, &
Ghahramani
,
Z.
(
2004
).
Computational motor control
.
Science
,
269
,
1880
1882
.
Xu
,
D.
,
Hu
,
A.
,
Han
,
X.
, &
Zhang
,
L.
(
2021
).
A nonlinear system state estimation method based on adaptive fusion of multiple kernel functions
.
Complexity
,
2021
.

Author notes

Alessandra Pedrocchi and Claudia Casellato are co–last authors.

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. For a full description of the license, please visit https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/legalcode

Supplementary data