We all vary in our mental health, even among people not meeting diagnostic criteria for mental illness. Understanding this individual variability may reveal factors driving the risk for mental illness, as well as factors driving subclinical problems that still adversely affect quality of life. To better understand the large-scale brain network mechanisms underlying this variability, we examined the relationship between mental health symptoms and resting-state functional connectivity patterns in cognitive control systems. One such system is the fronto-parietal cognitive control network (FPN). Changes in FPN connectivity may impact mental health by disrupting the ability to regulate symptoms in a goal-directed manner. Here we test the hypothesis that FPN dysconnectivity relates to mental health symptoms even among individuals who do not meet formal diagnostic criteria but may exhibit meaningful symptom variation. We found that depression symptoms severity negatively correlated with between-network global connectivity (BGC) of the FPN. This suggests that decreased connectivity between the FPN and the rest of the brain is related to increased depression symptoms in the general population. These findings complement previous clinical studies to support the hypothesis that global FPN connectivity contributes to the regulation of mental health symptoms across both health and disease.

Understanding how large-scale network interactions in the brain contribute to (or serve a protective role against) mental health symptoms is an important step toward developing more effective mental health treatments. Here we test the hypothesis that cognitive control networks play an important role in mental health by being highly connected to other brain networks and able to serve as a feedback mechanism capable of regulating symptoms in a goal-directed manner. We found that the more well connected the fronto-parietal cognitive control network was to other networks in the brain the less depression symptoms were reported by participants. These results contribute to our understanding of how brain network interactions are related to mental health symptoms, even in individuals who have not been diagnosed with a disorder.

People vary in their degree of mental health. Indeed, people who do not meet formal criteria for mental illness as defined by our current diagnostic systems (American Psychiatric Association, 2013; World Health Organization, 1992) may still experience a number of symptoms associated with that disorder (World Health Organization, 2017). Here we use this natural variability to better understand neural factors potentially contributing to day-to-day experiences of poor mental health, as well as (prodromal) factors that may elevate the risk for severe mental illness. We hypothesized that the variability observed in mental health symptoms among individuals is related to the function of the FPN, based on our previously developed theoretical proposal that the FPN plays a domain-general protective role against mental health symptoms (Cole et al., 2014).

The proposed theoretical framework suggests that alterations in FPN function may play a common role in multiple mental disorders by disrupting a domain-general cognitive control feedback mechanism that can regulate symptoms when they are experienced (Cole et al., 2014). The FPN is a candidate network for this function because it is a flexible hub, meaning it has a high degree of connectivity across the brain (Cole et al., 2010; Power et al., 2011) and can rapidly modify functional connections according to current goals (Cole et al., 2013). There is strong evidence that these FPN functions are domain general (Chein & Schneider, 2005; Cole et al., 2013; Dosenbach et al., 2007), such that individual differences in the general ability to regulate cognition can influence symptoms. Finally, alterations in FPN functional connectivity (FC) have been identified in a number of mental disorders, including depression (Kaiser et al., 2015), anxiety (Sylvester et al., 2012), schizophrenia (Baker et al., 2014; Cole et al., 2011; Fornito et al., 2011; Yang et al., 2016), attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (Li et al., 2014; Park et al., 2016), and eating disorders (Boehm et al., 2014; Cowdrey et al., 2014). Consistent with most of these studies, we focus here on FC measured using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), calculated as the temporal relationship in the blood oxygenation level dependent (BOLD) signal between brain regions (Biswal et al., 1995) while participants rest in the scanner.

Consistent with the flexible hub framework, a number of studies using different measures have provided converging evidence that the FPN is especially well connected to the rest of the brain (Buckner et al., 2009; Cole et al., 2010). Both of these studies calculated a summary statistic reflecting the degree of connectivity across the whole brain. However, these estimates can be influenced by the relative size of different networks. For example, nodes of a larger network will have a larger overall number of strong connections than nodes of a smaller network simply because, by definition, within-network connections are stronger on average than between-network connections (Power et al., 2013; Wig et al., 2011). Therefore, we estimated how well each region of the brain was connected to the rest of the brain by using between-network global connectivity (BGC; Ito et al., 2017), a measure not influenced by network size.

Particularly important for our specific test of the flexible hub framework here, patients diagnosed with major depression exhibit differences in FC patterns throughout the brain, including FPN functional connections (Brakowski et al., 2017). Specifically, connectivity between regions of the FPN is decreased in depressed individuals (Alexopoulos et al., 2012), as well as in undiagnosed individuals experiencing depression symptoms (Wei et al., 2014). However, Wei and colleagues (2014) looked at FC with specific seed regions, not global connectivity, in their sample. Another study found that global brain connectivity was decreased in the medial prefrontal cortex and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) portions of the FPN in depressed patients (Murrough et al., 2016). Depression has been shown to generally disrupt FC in the dlPFC as well as the default mode network (DMN) (for review see Gong & He, 2015). Decreases in within-network FPN connectivity have also been observed in a group of individuals reporting depression symptoms in the absence of a clinical diagnosis (Hwang et al., 2015). Researchers have also attempted to subdivide depression into various types based on FC patterns. Decreases in FPN connectivity were most pronounced in one particular subtype of depression associated with increased symptoms of fatigue and decreased symptoms of anxiety (Drysdale et al., 2016). Furthermore, activation in the FPN and changes in FPN FC have been observed when participants are actively attempting to regulate their emotions (Banks et al., 2007; Buhle et al., 2014; Kohn et al., 2014) and when participants are in threatening situations (Balderston et al., 2017). Decreased global brain connectivity, the mean FC value for each region’s connections, has been observed in the prefrontal cortex in depression patients (Abdallah et al., 2016). This decrease in global brain connectivity was rescued 24 hr following ketamine treatment. These previous results are broadly consistent with our hypothesis, yet the extension of results to test whether FPN BGC is related to mental health symptoms among healthy individuals would provide important new evidence for the general nature of FPN’s role in regulating mental health.

Consistent with our previously developed theoretical framework (Cole et al., 2014) along with observed FPN FC alterations in patients with major depression, we hypothesized that individual differences in depression symptoms in undiagnosed individuals would be correlated with BGC in the FPN. Support for our hypothesis would provide important evidence for a potentially general role of global FPN intrinsic FC in facilitating the regulation of mental health symptoms.

Participants

Data were collected at the Rutgers University Brain Imaging Center (RUBIC). The participants were recruited from the Rutgers University-Newark campus and surrounding community. All participants provided informed consent, and all procedures were approved by the Rutgers University-Newark Institutional Review Board. We collected data from 106 participants. Technical error or equipment malfunction during the scanning session resulted in removing six participants from the study. Four participants were removed from the study because they did not complete the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CESD) during a behavior-only session separate from the MRI session. We also collected demographic information and asked participants 11 questions about what hand they used for various activities including writing, throwing, using a scissors, holding their toothbrush, striking a match, opening a box, kicking, using a knife, using a spoon, which hand was placed on top while using a broom, and which eye they used in situations where they would only be using one eye. They replied to each question by answering always right, usually right, no preference, usually left, or always left. Answers were scored in the following manner: always right (2), usually right (1), no preference (0), usually left (−1), always left (−2). We calculated a laterality quotient (LQ) by summing these scores and dividing by the maximum score of 22. A LQ score of −100 indicates a strong left hand preference, 0 indicates no hand preference, and 100 indicates a strong right hand preference. All participants self-reported as right handed. However, one participant demonstrated a LQ indicating they had a left hand preference. The participant with a left hand preference has been included in all analyses.

Studies have proposed that the CESD can be broken down into between one and four factors (Cole et al., 2004; Herrero & Meneses, 2006). In addition to calculating the raw CESD score for each participant, we also calculated three factor scores: somatic symptoms, negative affect, and anhedonia, based on a recent study (Carleton et al., 2013). Participants also completed several measures of flexible cognition during the behavior-only session. These measures included Raven’s progressive matrices (Bilker et al., 2012), Cattell’s culture fair test (Cattell & Horn, 1978), and Duncan’s goal neglect task (Dumontheil et al., 2011). The final sample consisted of 96 participants (see Table 1).

Table 1. 
Demographic information
N 96 
Age M = 22.06, SD = 3.84 
Gender 
 Male 42 (43.8%) 
 Female 54 (56.2%) 
Handedness (LQ) M = 74.67, SD = 25.2 
Education (Highest level) 
 High school 14 (14.6%) 
 Some college 50 (52.1%) 
 Bachelor’s degree 29 (30.2%) 
 Graduate degree 3 (3.1%) 
Cognitive measures (Percent correct) 
 Raven M = 52.29, SD = 15.69 
 Cattell M = 55.20, SD = 12.03 
 Duncan M = 76.19, SD = 18.34 
CESD (raw score) M = 17.41, SD = 9.26 
N 96 
Age M = 22.06, SD = 3.84 
Gender 
 Male 42 (43.8%) 
 Female 54 (56.2%) 
Handedness (LQ) M = 74.67, SD = 25.2 
Education (Highest level) 
 High school 14 (14.6%) 
 Some college 50 (52.1%) 
 Bachelor’s degree 29 (30.2%) 
 Graduate degree 3 (3.1%) 
Cognitive measures (Percent correct) 
 Raven M = 52.29, SD = 15.69 
 Cattell M = 55.20, SD = 12.03 
 Duncan M = 76.19, SD = 18.34 
CESD (raw score) M = 17.41, SD = 9.26 

MRI Parameters

Multiband whole-brain echo-planar imaging (EPI) acquisition was collected using a 32-channel head coil on a 3T Siemens Trio MRI scanner with the following parameters: TR = 785 ms, TE = 34.8 ms, flip angle = 55°, Bandwidth 1,924/Hz/Px, in-plane FoV read =208 mm, 72 slices, 2.0-mm isotropic voxels, with a multiband acceleration factor of 8. Whole-brain high-resolution T1-weighted and T2-weighted anatomical scans with 0.8-mm isotropic voxels were also collected. Spin echo field maps were collected in both the anterior to posterior direction and the posterior to anterior direction consistent with the Human Connectome Project preprocessing pipelines (Glasser et al., 2013). The resting-state fMRI scan was 14 min (1,070 TRs) in duration.

fMRI Preprocessing

Functional imaging data were preprocessed using the Human Connectome Project minimal preprocessing pipeline version 3.5.0. Preprocessing consisted of anatomical restructuring and segmentation, EPI reconstruction, segmentation, and spatial normalization to a standard template, intensity normalization, and motion correction (Glasser et al., 2013). All further processing was conducted in CIFTI 64k greyordinate standard space. The data were parcellated into 360 regions described previously (Glasser et al., 2016), taking the average time series of the vertices within a parcel. At this point all subsequent data analysis was conducted in MATLAB R2014b (The Mathworks). We performed nuisance regression by using 12 motion parameters, ventricle and white matter time series (as well as their first derivatives), and global signal. We also performed motion scrubbing (Power et al., 2012) based on framewise displacement. Framewise displacement was calculated as the amount of head movement for each frame relative to the previous in terms of Euclidean distance. We next applied a low-pass temporal filter (0.3 Hz) to the framewise displacement vector in order to reduce the effect of respiration on our framewise displacement measure (Siegel et al., 2016). The framewise displacement threshold for motion scrubbing was set at 0.3 mm. Motion scrubbing consisted of removing the flagged frame from the time series as well as one frame prior and the two frames following. FC was estimated by calculating the Pearson correlation of the BOLD time series between each pair of the parcels defined by Glasser and colleagues (Glasser et al., 2016).

Network Assignment and Analysis

The network assignment of each of the parcels was completed on an independent dataset, the Human Connectome Project (100 unrelated; Van Essen et al., 2013) in a prior study (Spronk et al., 2017). Briefly, each parcel was assigned to a network by using the Generalized Louvain method for community detection. This process was conducted using resting-state data. We identified 12 functional networks (Spronk et al., 2017) (see Figure 1). The functional network topology findings replicate the major features of several previously published network partitions (Gordon et al., 2016; Power et al., 2011; Yeo et al., 2011).

Figure 1. 

Network assignments, as developed by Spronk et al. (2017). Resting-state fMRI data from an independent dataset (HCP: 100 unrelated) was used to assign each parcel to a network by using a community detection algorithm. This resulted in 12 total networks. Color indicates the network assignment for each parcel.

Figure 1. 

Network assignments, as developed by Spronk et al. (2017). Resting-state fMRI data from an independent dataset (HCP: 100 unrelated) was used to assign each parcel to a network by using a community detection algorithm. This resulted in 12 total networks. Color indicates the network assignment for each parcel.

Close modal

We were interested in the relationship between the frequency at which participants experienced depression symptoms and the degree of BGC. Specifically, we were interested in a measure that would estimate the strength of FC for each region of a network to all of the other regions in different networks. BGC was calculated for each region individually and defined as the mean FC for all out-of-network connections. Out-of-network connections were defined as all connections from a source region to target regions outside the source region’s network. This process was completed for all regions until we had a BGC value for each region in the brain. Then we calculated the mean BGC value for each of the functional networks to summarize effects at the network level.

More formally, BGC was defined for each region as:
where BGCi corresponds to the out-of-network weighted degree of region i in network C, jC corresponds to all regions not in network C, Wij corresponds to the FC estimate between regions i and j, Ntotal corresponds to the total number of regions, and NC corresponds to the total number of regions in network C.

We tested the relationship between BGC and depression symptoms in all brain networks. Because of the large number of statistical tests, we report the false discovery rate (FDR)-corrected p values for primary analyses (Benjamini & Hochberg, 1995). Our main hypothesis was that BGC in the FPN was correlated with depression symptoms. Because this was a primary hypothesis based on our previous publication (Cole et al., 2014) we report the uncorrected p value. Follow-up and control analyses were not independent of the primary tests and are therefore not corrected.

Depression Symptom Scores

We hypothesized that the frequency of experiencing depression symptoms is related to individual differences in the BGC of the FPN. This would be consistent with the involvement of FPN in domain-general cognitive regulation, broadly construed to include emotion (Cole et al., 2014). We used a standard measure of depression symptoms, the CESD, to measure the frequency of depression symptoms. The CESD consists of 20 questions asking how frequently participants have experienced symptoms related to depression over the last 7 days. CESD scores (M = 17.41, SD = 9.26) varied from a minimum of 0 to a maximum of 43 (out of a possible 60) (Figure 2). CESD scores from 16 to 24 indicate mild levels of depression symptoms, whereas scores above 24 indicate moderate to severe levels of depression. In our sample 48.9% of participants demonstrated at least mild levels of depression, while 25% of participants exhibited at least moderate levels of depression. The characteristics of our sample were consistent with previous studies using the CESD with undiagnosed young adults (Gress-Smith et al., 2015; Van Dam & Earleywine, 2011). However, the scores were not normally distributed based on a Kolomogorov–Smirnov test (p < 0.023). We therefore used the Box–Cox power transformation (Box & Cox, 1964), a common approach to correct for nonnormality. After the transformation the data no longer deviated from a normal distribution (p = 0.17). The transformed data were used for all subsequent analyses. CESD scores were not correlated with any of the flexible cognition measures (smallest p = 0.28).

Figure 2. 

Histogram of raw CESD scores.

Figure 2. 

Histogram of raw CESD scores.

Close modal

We also calculated CESD subscores for somatic, negative affect, and anhedonia factors. After Box–Cox transformation none of the factor score distributions significantly deviated from a normal distribution (somatic p = 0.4, negative affect p = 0.19, anhedonia p = 0.056). All three of the factors were significantly correlated with the overall CESD scores (somatic r = 0.77, uncorrected p < 0.001; negative affect r = 0.80, uncorrected p < 0.001; anhedonia r = 0.52, uncorrected p < 0.001; see Table 2). The three factors were also correlated with each other (correlation between somatic and negative affect r = 0.55, correlation between somatic and anhedonia r = 0.18, correlation between negative affect and anhedonia r = 0.26; see Table 1). Although all of the factors were highly related to the overall CESD score, they appear to have contributed unique variance as the highest correlation between any two factors was only accounting for 30% of the variance (based on R2 values).

Table 2. 
Correlation between the CESD and CESD factors
Overall CESDSomaticNegative affectAnhedonia
Overall CESD — — — — 
Somatic 0.77 — — — 
Negative affect 0.80 0.55 — — 
Anhedonia 0.52 0.18 0.26 — 
Overall CESDSomaticNegative affectAnhedonia
Overall CESD — — — — 
Somatic 0.77 — — — 
Negative affect 0.80 0.55 — — 
Anhedonia 0.52 0.18 0.26 — 

Between-Network Global Connectivity

We calculated BGC values using resting-state fMRI data for each functionally defined cortical parcel (Figure 3). DMN regions tended to have lower BGC values, consistent with the conceptually similar participation coefficient results reported by Power et al. (2011). In contrast, the FPN had high levels of BGC in lateral prefrontal portions of the network (M = 0.002, SD = 0.02) relative to the mean of regions outside of the FPN (M = −0.0081, SD = 0.013; t(95) = 5.08, p < 0.0001). However, FPN had lower BGC values in parietal (M = −0.0258, SD = 0.0190), inferior frontal (M = −0.0177, SD = 0.0209), and temporal lobe regions of the network (M = −0.0420, SD = 0.0221) relative to the mean of regions outside of the FPN (largest p < 0.0001). Sensory networks exhibited two patterns. The visual network had some of the lowest BGC values in the brain with the primary visual cortex region showing the least BGC in the network (M = −0.0742, SD = 0.0348). In contrast, the auditory network (M = 0.0212, SD = 0.0171) and portions of the sensorimotor network surrounding the central sulcus (M = 0.0117, SD = 0.0249) both showed a high degree of BGC. The sensorimotor regions closest to the central sulcus showed more moderate BGC scores (M = −0.0139, SD = 0.0289).

Figure 3. 

BGC across all cortical regions. Between-network global connectivity for region A is defined as the mean FC (Pearson correlation) between that region A and all other regions outside of region A’s network. Warm values indicate positive values and cool values indicate negative values.

Figure 3. 

BGC across all cortical regions. Between-network global connectivity for region A is defined as the mean FC (Pearson correlation) between that region A and all other regions outside of region A’s network. Warm values indicate positive values and cool values indicate negative values.

Close modal

Correlation Between BGC and Depression Symptoms

We next tested for a relationship between BGC and depression symptoms. This involved creating a mean BGC score for each functional network and testing for correlation between those values and the depression symptom measure (see Table 3). Consistent with our a priori hypothesis, we found that FPN BGC was significantly correlated with depression symptoms (r = −0.247, p = 0.015; Figure 4A). Unexpectedly, we found that DMN and language network BGC showed a similar magnitude effect (DMN r = −0.241, uncorrected p = 0.018; language r = −0.268, uncorrected p = 0.008; Figure 4B and 4C). These findings suggest that greater FC between the FPN and the rest of the brain (outside of the FPN) is related to less frequent depression symptoms. Less depression symptoms are also related to greater BGC in the DMN and language networks. We report an uncorrected p value for the test of our a priori hypothesis that FPN BGC would be related to depression symptoms. We also examined the relationship between depression symptoms and BGC in the other 11 brain networks. These p values do not survive FDR correction for multiple comparisons, but because the magnitude of the effect observed in the DMN and language network was similar to that in the FPN, we continue to report subsequent uncorrected statistics for these networks. Although it should be noted that the DMN and language network results should be interpreted with caution as they were not a priori hypotheses and do not survive multiple comparison correction.

Table 3. 
Correlation between BGC and CESD scores
NetworkPearson’s rUncorrected p value
Visual 1 0.015 0.884 
Visual 2 0.020 0.850 
Somatomotor −0.177 0.085 
Cingulo-opercular −0.143 0.164 
Language −0.268 0.008 
Default mode −0.241 0.018 
Fronto-parietal −0.247 0.015* 
Auditory −0.099 0.340 
Posterior multimodal −0.110 0.284 
Dorsal attention −0.113 0.275 
Ventral multimodal 0.116 0.261 
Orbito-affective −0.078 0.448 
NetworkPearson’s rUncorrected p value
Visual 1 0.015 0.884 
Visual 2 0.020 0.850 
Somatomotor −0.177 0.085 
Cingulo-opercular −0.143 0.164 
Language −0.268 0.008 
Default mode −0.241 0.018 
Fronto-parietal −0.247 0.015* 
Auditory −0.099 0.340 
Posterior multimodal −0.110 0.284 
Dorsal attention −0.113 0.275 
Ventral multimodal 0.116 0.261 
Orbito-affective −0.078 0.448 

Note. Bold entries indicate the a priori hypothesis.

*

Significant correlation for the a priori hypothesis.

Figure 4. 

CESD scores and BGC in the FPN, DMN, and language networks are negatively correlated. (A) BGC in the FPN is plotted on the y-axis, and CESD scores are plotted on the x-axis. This relationship was hypothesized a priori. (B) BGC in the DMN is plotted on the y-axis, and CESD scores are plotted on the x-axis. (C) BGC in the language network is plotted on the y-axis, and CESD scores are plotted on the x-axis.

Figure 4. 

CESD scores and BGC in the FPN, DMN, and language networks are negatively correlated. (A) BGC in the FPN is plotted on the y-axis, and CESD scores are plotted on the x-axis. This relationship was hypothesized a priori. (B) BGC in the DMN is plotted on the y-axis, and CESD scores are plotted on the x-axis. (C) BGC in the language network is plotted on the y-axis, and CESD scores are plotted on the x-axis.

Close modal

To address whether these findings were specific to this particular network partition, we conducted a similar analysis by using a different network partition scheme (Ito et al., 2017). This network partition includes a FPN and DMN, but lacks a network analogous to the language network. Using this network partition we found a similar negative correlation between BGC and depression symptoms in the FPN (r = −0.262, p = 0.009, FDR adjusted for multiple comparisons p = 0.044) and DMN (r = −0.250, p = 0.014, FDR adjusted p = 0.049). The results for the FPN and DMN both survive correction for multiple comparisons with this network partition.

We conducted a similar analysis, correlating BGC and depression symptoms on the region level, to examine if there were particular brain regions within these networks driving the network level results. None of the individual brain regions showed a significant correlation after correcting for multiple comparisons by using FDR. This suggests that the network level results are not being driven by particularly strong relationships in a smaller number of brain regions within these networks.

To verify that these correlations were not dependent on the Box–Cox transformation we used to normalize the depression symptom data, we also ran nonparametric Spearman’s rank correlations, which do not require normally distributed data. The results of testing our a priori hypothesis that BGC in the FPN would be related to depression symptoms were unchanged. Specifically, the BGC-depression rank correlation was consistent with significant Pearson correlations (FPN rho = −0.227, p = 0.026). The Spearman rank correlations also reflected similar results to the Pearson correlation in the DMN (rho = −0.220, uncorrected p = 0.031) and the language network (rho = −0.285, uncorrected p = 0.005).

To ensure that our observed correlation between depression symptoms and BGC in the FPN was not being driven by other factors, we removed the variance in the CESD scores that could be accounted for by age and gender factors with a regression model and used the residuals to rerun the correlation. We still found a significant relationship between BGC in the FPN and depression symptoms (r = −0.220, p = 0.031). This result suggests that the relationship between BGC in the FPN and depression scores is not being influenced by age or gender differences in our sample.

Next, we tested if the relationships between BGC and overall depression scores were driven by specific depression factors. We tested for correlation between the BGC values and three factors derived from the CESD questionnaire: somatic symptoms, negative affect, and anhedonia symptoms (Carleton et al., 2013). The three factors were correlated with one another so we also used a partial correlation approach to explore the relationship between the unique variance contributed by each factor and BGC in the FPN, DMN, and language networks. The partial correlation approach allows us to examine the relationship between BGC and each of the factors while accounting for the variance contributed by the other factors. After accounting for the variance contributed by other factors, we found that none of the factors were significantly correlated with BGC in the FPN (largest rho = −0.110, p = 0.292). We also found that the unique variance from each factor was not correlated with BGC in the DMN (largest rho = −0.137, p = 0.188). BGC in the language network was not significantly correlated with the unique variance contributed by any of the factors (largest rho = −0.185, p = 0.075). These results suggest that the relationships we observed between the overall CESD scores and BGC were driven by a combination of all factors, and not specific features of depression.

Observed BGC-Depression Correlations Were not Dependent on Connections Between FPN, DMN, and Language Network

The magnitude and direction of the BGC-depression correlations were similar for the FPN, DMN, and language network. One possible explanation for these results is that the observed BGC-depression effects for these networks were driven primarily by the FC between them. We tested this possibility by recalculating BGC for each network by removing connections between the FPN, DMN, and language network from the calculation. This approach minimizes the possibility that connections between these networks drove the original correlations between depression symptoms and BGC. The magnitude and direction of the correlations between depression symptoms and BGC in the FPN (r = −0.226, p = 0.027), the DMN (r = −0.236, uncorrected p = 0.021), and the language network (r = −0.201, uncorrected p = 0.049) were similar when we excluded the connections between these networks from the BGC calculation. This suggests the original BGC-depression correlations were not dependent on connections between the FPN, DMN, and language network.

To further explore the possibility that the observed relationship between depression symptoms and BGC in the FPN might be primarily driven by FPN connections to the DMN and language network, we calculated the correlation between the mean FPN to DMN, and FPN to language network FC with depression symptoms. There was not a significant correlation between depression symptoms and the mean connectivity between the FPN and DMN (r = −0.016, uncorrected p = 0.881) or the mean connectivity between the FPN and language network (r = −0.146, uncorrected p = 0.156). This suggests that the relationship between FPN BGC and depression symptoms is not driven by connections to the DMN and language networks, which both showed a negative correlation between BGC and depression symptoms as well.

Within-Network Connectivity Does not Correlate with Depression Symptoms

We focused on between-network connectivity, yet the degree of connectivity within a functional network might also be related to depression symptoms. For example, a high degree of FC within a network may reflect more homogeneous network activity. In this case the network would be tightly coupled and all of the component regions would likely be serving a very similar function. However, a lower degree of FC within a network may reflect more heterogeneous processing. In this case the different regions of a functional network may all be involved in the same general process, but they may be contributing in different ways. These differences in within-network functionality may, in turn, relate to depression symptoms.

Consistent with our choice to focus primarily on between-network effects, within-network FC in the FPN was not significantly correlated with depression symptoms (r = 0.066, p = 0.526). Within-network FC in the DMN was also not significantly correlated with depression symptoms (r = 0.143, uncorrected p = 0.164). The language network showed a negative correlation between depression symptoms and within-network FC (r = −0.219, uncorrected p = 0.032). We observed a significant difference in the DMN correlation coefficients comparing BGC (r = −0.241) and within-network connectivity (r = 0.143) with depression symptoms (z = 2.66, p = 0.008). It will be important for future studies to test this effect for replication, given the nonsignificance of the DMN within-network FC-depression effect relative to a correlation of 0.

On the basis of convergent evidence across a variety of mental health conditions, we predicted that individual differences in FPN BGC would be correlated with symptoms associated with depression (Cole et al., 2014). This is consistent with extensive evidence that the FPN is a domain-general cognitive control system regulating general goal pursuit processes (Cole & Schneider, 2007; Duncan, 2010; Schneider, 2003), including regulation of mental illness symptoms (Cole et al., 2014). We sought to expand the general relevance of this framework by including symptoms experienced in everyday life, which express dimensionally across health and disease. This involved investigating the relationship between the mood symptoms, one of the most commonly experienced set of symptoms in the general population (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention [CDC], 2010), and FPN global connectivity properties. We further ensured the broad relevance of the findings by focusing on how a general population who had not been diagnosed with a mental disorder varied in their depression symptoms and primarily experienced them in the normal (i.e., subclinical) range.

As predicted, we found a significant relationship between how well connected the FPN was to the rest of the brain and the frequency of depression symptoms in adults who had not been diagnosed with depression. We used BGC to estimate the degree of brain-wide connectivity for each brain region. The results suggest that individuals who report fewer depression symptoms are characterized by an FPN that is more connected to the rest of the brain. These results support our previously developed theoretical framework, suggesting natural variance in FPN global connectivity influences each individual’s ability to regulate mood symptoms in everyday life. Extending this framework to test if FPN function relates to natural variation in other symptom domains will be critical as well as examining if such effects persist when an individual crosses a threshold necessary for a formal diagnosis.

Between-Network Global Connectivity Identifies how well Each Brain Region Is Connected to Other Brain Networks

We hypothesized that individuals exhibiting greater FC between the FPN and the rest of the brain would report fewer depression symptoms. We used BGC, a measure that calculates the mean FC between each brain region and all other out-of-network brain regions (Ito et al., 2017), to evaluate this hypothesis. BGC reduces the potential bias of other graph centrality measures, which can be inflated in regions assigned to large networks (Power et al., 2013). Previous methods using global brain connectivity and degree, which both include within-network connections, result in DMN regions showing high connectivity with the rest of the brain (Buckner et al., 2009; Cole et al., 2010; Liang et al., 2013). However, we found that BGC was relatively low in the DMN. This is consistent with DMN results from studies using participation coefficient (Power et al., 2013, 2011). Although our measure of BGC showed similarity to results obtained using participation coefficient (Power et al., 2011), there were some differences. Some of these differences may be due to the differences between BGC and participation coefficient. Participation coefficient is generally calculated by first thresholding a graph and then examining the distribution of edges between networks. A high value indicates that the edges are relatively evenly distributed to other networks, and lower values indicate that the edges tend to preferentially connect nodes in a smaller number of other networks. BGC does not require thresholding a graph prior to calculation and simply measures the mean FC weight for each region to all other out-of-network regions. These results suggest that previous findings identifying the DMN as highly connected to the rest of the brain are largely driven by high FC within the DMN, and do not necessarily reflect greater connectivity between the DMN and nodes in other functional networks.

The results we observed with BGC were similar to previous attempts to identify hubs and the most well connected regions in the brain. BGC was high in the lateral prefrontal cortex, the motor and tactile cortex, the auditory cortex, and higher order visual regions. Lateral prefrontal regions and higher order visual areas show a greater degree of FC (Buckner et al., 2009) and a higher global brain connectivity (Cole et al., 2010). Higher global brain connectivity has also been reported in the lateral prefrontal cortex, higher order visual regions, auditory cortex, and somatosensory cortex (Liang et al., 2013). We observed that BGC was consistent with previous attempts to classify the degree of connectivity in many brain regions.

There were some discrepancies between previous methods and BGC. We found that BGC was quite low in lower visual regions in contrast to higher connectivity estimates calculated by others (Cole et al., 2010; Liang et al., 2013). The differences observed between BGC and other measures of connectivity strength may be driven by BGC not considering the relatively strong local connections within the visual network. In fact, primary and secondary visual cortex show high local connectivity relative to distant connectivity strength (Sepulcre et al., 2010). It will be important for future studies to identify whether differences observed in primary sensory cortices are driven by an imbalance between within- and between-network connectivity.

BGC in the FPN Is Negatively Correlated with Depression Symptoms

We found that BGC in the FPN showed a significant negative correlation with depression symptoms. This suggests that individuals exhibiting greater connectivity of the FPN with the rest of the brain experience fewer symptoms of depression. The correlation between BGC in the FPN and depression symptoms supports our hypothesis that a well-connected FPN may serve a protective role against depression symptoms and possibly mental health symptoms in general (Cole et al., 2014).

Decreases in FPN FC have been reported in individuals diagnosed with major depression (Alexopoulos et al., 2012; Murrough et al., 2016; Veer, 2010). Differences in FC patterns can also be used to divide depression into distinct subtypes (Drysdale et al., 2016). We build on these findings by observing similar results in a variable sample of undiagnosed individuals that likely includes primarily mentally healthy, but also some mentally unhealthy, participants. Another study examining FC in a group of undiagnosed individuals with higher levels of depression symptoms found that connectivity between the superior parietal lobule and the dlPFC portion of the FPN was decreased (Wei et al., 2014). Undiagnosed participants experiencing more depression symptoms have also been reported to have reduced FC between dlPFC and the supramarginal gyrus, insula, operculum, precuneus, and parahippocampal gyrus (Hwang et al., 2015). Previous studies have also found that antidepressant and electroconvulsive therapy treatments can modulate prefrontal cortex FC in depression patients (Perrin et al., 2012; Wang et al., 2015). It is possible that future therapeutic interventions could target and attempt to alter aspects of network communication. Our findings suggest that greater global connectivity of the FPN is related to reduced depression symptoms. Our data regarding global connectivity differences is consistent with a recent study that found reduced global brain connectivity in the lateral PFC in depression patients (Abdallah et al., 2016). Here we extend the relationship between global connectivity in the FPN to include samples that have not been diagnosed with a clinical disorder and further refine the global connectivity findings by suggesting that the between-network connections are particularly important. Another recent study has suggested that the FPN can be split into two subsystems, one more involved in introceptive processes, and another more involved in the regulation of perceptual attention (Dixon et al., 2018). Future studies should examine if FC relationships with depression are stronger in the introceptive FPN system that may be more involved in rumination symptoms.

Unexpectedly, we also found a negative correlation between BGC in the DMN and depression symptoms. This result did not survive multiple comparison correction, but because it was of a similar magnitude as our predicted relationship in the FPN we have explored it. It is important to note that the results in the DMN should be interpreted with caution, and future studies should attempt to replicate these results. DMN activity has been linked to rumination symptoms in depression (Hamilton et al., 2011). This finding may reflect an inability for individuals experiencing more depression symptoms to disengage the DMN in situations when attentional or cognitive resources need to be allocated (Sheline et al., 2009). Depression patients also exhibit decreased FC between the DMN and executive networks (Abbott et al., 2013; Manoliu et al., 2014). These findings provide further support for the interpretation that depressed individuals may have a difficult time disengaging the DMN because the connections from cognitive control networks to the DMN are decreased.

A number of studies have suggested that FC within portions of the DMN is increased in depression patients (Greicius et al., 2007; Li et al., 2013). Although we did not find a significant correlation between within-network FC in the DMN and depression symptoms, we did see a trend toward a positive correlation. Our lack of a correlation between within-network FC in the DMN and depression symptoms could be due to methodological differences in using a population of individuals who have not been diagnosed with a disorder versus comparing a control group to a group diagnosed with depression. Additionally, we considered the entire DMN rather than using a seed correlation approach. It will be important for future studies to investigate the relationship between within-DMN FC and depression symptoms, verifying this effect in a larger sample and tying it to specific neural mechanisms and specific depression symptoms.

We also found a negative correlation between BGC in the language network and depression symptoms. Similar to the DMN, this result did not survive multiple comparison correction, but because the magnitude of the effect was similar to that of our predicted relationship between BGC in the FPN and depression symptoms we have explored it. Because we did not have an a priori prediction about this network and the result did not survive multiple comparison correction, the interpretation of this result should be tempered. Decreases in FC in the language network have been reported in depression patients (Buchanan et al., 2014) and language performance deficits have also been reported in depression patients (Baune et al., 2010). The current results might be consistent with these observed deficits in language in depressed individuals. However, future studies should attempt to replicate these results in an independent large sample.

The present study sought to test our previously developed framework that suggests the FPN (along with other cognitive control networks) acts as a protective factor against mental disease via its widespread FC with other networks (Cole et al., 2014). We identified a negative correlation between depression symptoms and a measure of between-network, global FC in the FPN in a sample of individuals from the general population who had not been diagnosed with depression. These results suggest the human brain’s global network architecture is critical for maintaining mental health even in undiagnosed individuals, supporting the possibility that FPN maintains a goal-directed feedback loop to regulate symptoms as they arise. It will be important for future research to characterize the exact mechanisms by which FPN influences symptoms, and to assess the possibility of enhancing FPN FC in the interest of reducing symptoms and potentially preventing the onset of mental illness.

Douglas Schultz: Conceptualization; Data curation; Formal analysis; Investigation; Methodology; Project administration; Visualization; Writing – original draft; Writing – review & editing. Takuya Ito: Data curation; Methodology; Writing – review & editing. Levi Solomyak: Data curation. Richard Chen: Data curation; Writing – review & editing. Ravi Mill: Data curation; Writing – review & editing. Alan Anticevic: Writing – review & editing. Michael Cole: Conceptualization; Funding acquisition; Investigation; Methodology; Project administration; Supervision; Writing – review & editing.

Michael Cole, National Institute of Mental Health (http://dx.doi.org/10.13039/100000025), Award ID: MH096801. Michael Cole, National Institute of Mental Health (http://dx.doi.org/10.13039/100000025), Award ID: MH109520. Michael Cole, National Institute on Aging (http://dx.doi.org/10.13039/100000049), Award ID: AG055556.

Flexible hub:

A brain region with a high , degree of connectivity across the brain that can rapidly modify functional connections according to current goals.

Between-network global connectivity (BGC):

The mean functional connectivity for all out-of-network connections for a brain region.

Global brain connectivity:

The mean of all functional connections for a brain region.

The Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CESD):

A 20-item questionnaire that asks participants how frequently they have experienced depression symptoms over the last week.

Community detection:

A method used to cluster graphs into groups exhibiting greater within-community relationships than between-community relationships.

Abbott
,
C. C.
,
Lemke
,
N. T.
,
Gopal
,
S.
,
Thoma
,
R. J.
,
Bustillo
,
J.
,
Calhoun
,
V. D.
, &
Turner
,
J. A.
(
2013
).
Electroconvulsive therapy response in major depressive disorder: A pilot functional network connectivity resting state fMRI investigation
.
Frontiers in Psychiatry
,
4
. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00010
Abdallah
,
C. G.
,
Averill
,
L. A.
,
Collins
,
K. A.
,
Geha
,
P.
,
Schwartz
,
J.
,
Averill
,
C.
, …
Tang
,
C. Y.
(
2016
).
Ketamine treatment and global brain connectivity in major depression
.
Neuropsychopharmacology
. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2016.186
Alexopoulos
,
G. S.
,
Hoptman
,
M. J.
,
Kanellopoulos
,
D.
,
Murphy
,
C. F.
,
Lim
,
K. O.
, &
Gunning
,
F. M.
(
2012
).
Functional connectivity in the cognitive control network and the default mode network in late-life depression
.
Journal of Affective Disorders
,
139
,
56
65
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2011.12.002
American Psychiatric Association
. (
2013
).
Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders
(5th ed.).
Arlington, VA
:
American Psychiatric Publishing
.
Baker
,
J. T.
,
Holmes
,
A. J.
,
Masters
,
G. A.
,
Yeo
,
B. T. T.
,
Krienen
,
F.
,
Buckner
,
R. L.
, &
Öngür
,
D.
(
2014
).
Disruption of cortical association networks in schizophrenia and psychotic bipolar disorder
.
JAMA Psychiatry
,
71
,
109
. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.3469
Balderston
,
N. L.
,
Hale
,
E.
,
Hsiung
,
A.
,
Torrisi
,
S.
,
Holroyd
,
T.
,
Carver
,
F. W.
, …
Grillon
,
C.
(
2017
).
Threat of shock increases excitability and connectivity of the intraparietal sulcus
.
eLife
,
6
. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.23608
Banks
,
S. J.
,
Eddy
,
K. T.
,
Angstadt
,
M.
,
Nathan
,
P. J.
, &
Phan
,
K. L.
(
2007
).
Amygdala–frontal connectivity during emotion regulation
.
Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience
,
2
,
303
312
. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsm029
Baune
,
B. T.
,
Miller
,
R.
,
McAfoose
,
J.
,
Johnson
,
M.
,
Quirk
,
F.
, &
Mitchell
,
D.
(
2010
).
The role of cognitive impairment in general functioning in major depression
.
Psychiatry Research
,
176
,
183
189
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2008.12.001
Benjamini
,
Y.
, &
Hochberg
,
Y.
(
1995
).
Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing
.
Journal of the Royal Statistical Society
,
57
,
289
300
.
Bilker
,
W. B.
,
Hansen
,
J. A.
,
Brensinger
,
C. M.
,
Richard
,
J.
,
Gur
,
R. E.
, &
Gur
,
R. C.
(
2012
).
Development of abbreviated nine-item forms of the Raven’s standard progressive matrices test
.
Assessment
,
19
,
354
369
.
Biswal
,
B.
,
Zerrin Yetkin
,
F.
,
Haughton
,
V. M.
, &
Hyde
,
J. S.
(
1995
).
Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI
.
Magnetic Resonance in Medicine
,
34
,
537
541
.
Boehm
,
I.
,
Geisler
,
D.
,
King
,
J. A.
,
Ritschel
,
F.
,
Seidel
,
M.
,
Deza Araujo
,
Y.
, …
Ehrlich
,
S.
(
2014
).
Increased resting state functional connectivity in the fronto-parietal and default mode network in anorexia nervosa
.
Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience
. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00346
Box
,
G. E.
, &
Cox
,
D. R.
(
1964
).
An analysis of transformations
.
Journal of the Royal Statistical Society
,
26
,
211
252
.
Brakowski
,
J.
,
Spinelli
,
S.
,
Dorig
,
N.
,
Bosch
,
O. G.
,
Manoliu
,
A.
,
Holtforth
,
M. G.
, &
Seifritz
,
E.
(
2017
).
Resting state brain network function in major depression - Depression symptomatology, antidepressant treatment effects, future research
.
Journal of Psychiatric Research
,
92
,
147
159
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2017.04.007
Buchanan
,
A.
,
Wang
,
X.
, &
Gollan
,
J. K.
(
2014
).
Resting-state functional connectivity in women with major depressive disorder
.
Journal of Psychiatric Research
,
59
,
38
44
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.09.002
Buckner
,
R. L.
,
Sepulcre
,
J.
,
Talukdar
,
T.
,
Krienen
,
F. M.
,
Liu
,
H.
,
Hedden
,
T.
, …
Johnson
,
K. A.
(
2009
).
Cortical hubs revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity: Mapping, assessment of stability, and relation to Alzheimer’s disease
.
Journal of Neuroscience
,
29
,
1860
1873
. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5062-08.2009
Buhle
,
J. T.
,
Silvers
,
J. A.
,
Wager
,
T. D.
,
Lopez
,
R.
,
Onyemekwu
,
C.
,
Kober
,
H.
, …
Ochsner
,
K. N.
(
2014
).
Cognitive reappraisal of emotion: A meta-analysis of human neuroimaging studies
.
Cerebral Cortex
,
24
,
2981
2990
. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bht154
Carleton
,
R. N.
,
Thibodeau
,
M. A.
,
Teale
,
M. J. N.
,
Welch
,
P. G.
,
Abrams
,
M. P.
,
Robinson
,
T.
, &
Asmundson
,
G. J. G.
(
2013
).
The Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale: A review with a theoretical and empirical examination of item content and factor structure
.
PloS ONE
,
8
,
e58067
. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0058067
Cattell
,
R. B.
, &
Horn
,
J. L.
(
1978
).
A check on the theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence with description of new subtest designs
.
Journal of Educational Measurement
,
15
,
139
164
.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC)
. (
2010
).
Current depression among adults–United States, 2006 and 2008
.
Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report
,
59
,
1229
1235
.
Chein
,
J. M.
, &
Schneider
,
W.
(
2005
).
Neuroimaging studies of practice-related change: fMRI and meta-analytic evidence of a domain-general control network for learning
.
Cognitive Brain Research
,
25
,
607
623
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.08.013
Cole
,
J. C.
,
Rabin
,
A. S.
,
Smith
,
T. L.
, &
Kaufman
,
A. S.
(
2004
).
Development and validation of a Rasch-derived CES-D short form
.
Psychological Assessment
,
16
,
360
372
. https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.16.4.360
Cole
,
M. W.
,
Anticevic
,
A.
,
Repovs
,
G.
, &
Barch
,
D.
(
2011
).
Variable global dysconnectivity and individual differences in schizophrenia
.
Biological Psychiatry
,
70
,
43
50
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.02.010
Cole
,
M. W.
,
Pathak
,
S.
, &
Schneider
,
W.
(
2010
).
Identifying the brain’s most globally connected regions
.
NeuroImage
,
49
,
3132
3148
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.11.001
Cole
,
M. W.
,
Repovš
,
G.
, &
Anticevic
,
A.
(
2014
).
The frontoparietal control system: A central role in mental health
.
The Neuroscientist
,
20
,
652
664
.
Cole
,
M. W.
,
Reynolds
,
J. R.
,
Power
,
J. D.
,
Repovs
,
G.
,
Anticevic
,
A.
, &
Braver
,
T. S.
(
2013
).
Multi-task connectivity reveals flexible hubs for adaptive task control
.
Nature Neuroscience
,
16
,
1348
1355
. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3470
Cole
,
M. W.
, &
Schneider
,
W.
(
2007
).
The cognitive control network: Integrated cortical regions with dissociable functions
.
NeuroImage
,
37
,
343
360
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.071
Cowdrey
,
F. A.
,
Filippini
,
N.
,
Park
,
R. J.
,
Smith
,
S. M.
, &
McCabe
,
C.
(
2014
).
Increased resting state functional connectivity in the default mode network in recovered anorexia nervosa: Resting state functional connectivity in the DMN in recovered AN
.
Human Brain Mapping
,
35
,
483
491
. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22202
Dixon
,
M. L.
,
De La Vega
,
A.
,
Mills
,
C.
,
Andrews-Hanna
,
J.
,
Spreng
,
R. N.
,
Cole
,
M. W.
, &
Christoff
,
K.
(
2018
).
Heterogeneity within the frontoparietal control network and its relationship to the default and dorsal attention networks
.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1715766115
Dosenbach
,
N. U.
,
Fair
,
D. A.
,
Miezin
,
F. M.
,
Cohen
,
A. L.
,
Wenger
,
K. K.
,
Dosenbach
,
R. A.
, …
Petersen
,
S. E.
(
2007
).
Distinct brain networks for adaptive and stable task control in humans
.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
,
104
,
11073
11078
.
Drysdale
,
A. T.
,
Grosenick
,
L.
,
Downar
,
J.
,
Dunlop
,
K.
,
Mansouri
,
F.
,
Meng
,
Y.
, …
Liston
,
C.
(
2016
).
Resting-state connectivity biomarkers define neurophysiological subtypes of depression
.
Nature Medicine
,
23
,
28
38
. https://doi.org/10.1038/nm.4246
Dumontheil
,
I.
,
Thompson
,
R.
, &
Duncan
,
J.
(
2011
).
Assembly and use of new task rules in fronto-parietal cortex
.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
,
23
,
168
182
.
Duncan
,
J.
(
2010
).
The multiple-demand (MD) system of the primate brain: Mental programs for intelligent behaviour
.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences
,
14
,
172
179
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.01.004
Fornito
,
A.
,
Yoon
,
J.
,
Zalesky
,
A.
,
Bullmore
,
E. T.
, &
Carter
,
C. S.
(
2011
).
General and specific functional connectivity disturbances in first-episode schizophrenia during cognitive control performance
.
Biological Psychiatry
,
70
,
64
72
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.02.019
Glasser
,
M. F.
,
Coalson
,
T. S.
,
Robinson
,
E. C.
,
Hacker
,
C. D.
,
Harwell
,
J.
,
Yacoub
,
E.
,
Van Essen
,
D. C.
(
2016
).
A multi-modal parcellation of human cerebral cortex
.
Nature
,
536
,
171
178
. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature18933
Glasser
,
M. F.
,
Sotiropoulos
,
S. N.
,
Wilson
,
J. A.
,
Coalson
,
T. S.
,
Fischl
,
B.
,
Andersson
,
J. L.
, …
Jenkinson
,
M.
(
2013
).
The minimal preprocessing pipelines for the Human Connectome Project
.
NeuroImage
,
80
,
105
124
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.127
Gong
,
Q.
, &
He
,
Y.
(
2015
).
Depression, neuroimaging and connectomics: A selective overview
.
Biological Psychiatry
,
77
,
223
235
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.08.009
Gordon
,
E. M.
,
Laumann
,
T. O.
,
Adeyemo
,
B.
,
Huckins
,
J. F.
,
Kelley
,
W. M.
, &
Petersen
,
S. E.
(
2016
).
Generation and evaluation of a cortical area parcellation from resting-state correlations
.
Cerebral Cortex
,
26
,
288
303
. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhu239
Greicius
,
M. D.
,
Flores
,
B. H.
,
Menon
,
V.
,
Glover
,
G. H.
,
Solvason
,
H. B.
,
Kenna
,
H.
, …
Schatzberg
,
A. F.
(
2007
).
Resting-state functional connectivity in major depression: Abnormally increased contributions from subgenual cingulate cortex and thalamus
.
Biological Psychiatry
,
62
,
429
437
.
Gress-Smith
,
J. L.
,
Roubinov
,
D. S.
,
Andreotti
,
C.
,
Compas
,
B. E.
, &
Luecken
,
L. J.
(
2015
).
Prevalence, severity and risk factors for depressive symptoms and insomnia in college undergraduates: Depressive symptoms and insomnia in college
.
Stress Health
,
31
,
63
70
. https://doi.org/10.1002/smi.2509
Hamilton
,
J. P.
,
Furman
,
D. J.
,
Chang
,
C.
,
Thomason
,
M. E.
,
Dennis
,
E.
, &
Gotlib
,
I. H.
(
2011
).
Default-mode and task-positive network activity in major depressive disorder: Implications for adaptive and maladaptive rumination
.
Biological Psychiatry
,
70
,
327
333
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.02.003
Herrero
,
J.
, &
Meneses
,
J.
(
2006
).
Short web-based versions of the perceived stress (PSS) and Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression (CESD) scales: A comparison to pencil and paper responses among Internet users
.
Computers in Human Behavior
,
22
,
830
846
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2004.03.007
Hwang
,
J. W.
,
Egorova
,
N.
,
Yang
,
X. Q.
,
Zhang
,
W. Y.
,
Chen
,
J.
,
Yang
,
X. Y.
, …
Kong
,
J.
(
2015
).
Subthreshold depression is associated with impaired resting-state functional connectivity of the cognitive control network
.
Translational Psychiatry
,
5
,
e683
. https://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2015.174
Ito
,
T.
,
Kulkarni
,
K. R.
,
Schultz
,
D. H.
,
Mill
,
R. D.
,
Chen
,
R. H.
,
Solomyak
,
L. I.
, &
Cole
,
M. W.
(
2017
).
Cognitive task information is transferred between brain regions via resting-state network topology
.
Nature Communication
,
8
. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-017-01000-w
Kaiser
,
R. H.
,
Andrews-Hanna
,
J. R.
,
Wager
,
T. D.
, &
Pizzagalli
,
D. A.
(
2015
).
Large-scale network dysfunction in major depressive disorder: A meta-analysis of resting-state functional connectivity
.
JAMA Psychiatry
,
72
,
603
. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.0071
Kohn
,
N.
,
Eickhoff
,
S. B.
,
Scheller
,
M.
,
Laird
,
A. R.
,
Fox
,
P. T.
, &
Habel
,
U.
(
2014
).
Neural network of cognitive emotion regulation—An ALE meta-analysis and MACM analysis
.
NeuroImage
,
87
,
345
355
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.001
Li
,
B.
,
Liu
,
L.
,
Friston
,
K. J.
,
Shen
,
H.
,
Wang
,
L.
,
Zeng
,
L.-L.
, &
Hu
,
D.
(
2013
).
A treatment-resistant default mode subnetwork in major depression
.
Biological Psychiatry
,
74
,
48
54
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.11.007
Li
,
F.
,
He
,
N.
,
Li
,
Y.
,
Chen
,
L.
,
Huang
,
X.
,
Lui
,
S.
, …
Gong
,
Q.
(
2014
).
Intrinsic brain abnormalities in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A resting-state functional MR imaging study
.
Radiology
,
272
,
514
523
.
Liang
,
X.
,
Zou
,
Q.
,
He
,
Y.
, &
Yang
,
Y.
(
2013
).
Coupling of functional connectivity and regional cerebral blood flow reveals a physiological basis for network hubs of the human brain
.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
,
110
,
1929
1934
. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1214900110
Manoliu
,
A.
,
Meng
,
C.
,
Brandl
,
F.
,
Doll
,
A.
,
Tahmasian
,
M.
,
Scherr
,
M.
, …
Sorg
,
C.
(
2014
).
Insular dysfunction within the salience network is associated with severity of symptoms and aberrant inter-network connectivity in major depressive disorder
.
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience
,
7
. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00930
Murrough
,
J. W.
,
Abdallah
,
C. G.
,
Anticevic
,
A.
,
Collins
,
K. A.
,
Geha
,
P.
,
Averill
,
L. A.
, …
Charney
,
D. S.
(
2016
).
Reduced global functional connectivity of the medial prefrontal cortex in major depressive disorder: Global connectivity in MDD
.
Human Brain Mapping
,
37
,
3214
3223
. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23235
Park
,
B.
,
Hong
,
J.
,
Lee
,
S.-H.
, &
Park
,
H.
(
2016
).
Functional connectivity of child and adolescent attention deficit hyperactivity disorder patients: Correlation with IQ
.
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience
,
10
. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00565
Perrin
,
J. S.
,
Merz
,
S.
,
Bennett
,
D. M.
,
Currie
,
J.
,
Steele
,
D. J.
,
Reid
,
I. C.
, &
Schwarzbauer
,
C.
(
2012
).
Electroconvulsive therapy reduces frontal cortical connectivity in severe depressive disorder
.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
,
109
,
5464
5468
. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1117206109
Power
,
J. D.
,
Barnes
,
K. A.
,
Snyder
,
A. Z.
,
Schlaggar
,
B. L.
, &
Petersen
,
S. E.
(
2012
).
Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion
.
NeuroImage
,
59
,
2142
2154
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.018
Power
,
J. D.
,
Cohen
,
A. L.
,
Nelson
,
S. M.
,
Wig
,
G. S.
,
Barnes
,
K. A.
,
Church
,
J. A.
, …
Petersen
,
S. E.
(
2011
).
Functional network organization of the human brain
.
Neuron
,
72
,
665
678
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.006
Power
,
J. D.
,
Schlaggar
,
B. L.
,
Lessov-Schlaggar
,
C. N.
, &
Petersen
,
S. E.
(
2013
).
Evidence for hubs in human functional brain networks
.
Neuron
,
79
,
798
813
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2013.07.035
Schneider
,
W.
(
2003
).
Controlled and automatic processing: Behavior, theory, and biological mechanisms
.
Cognitive Science
,
27
,
525
559
. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0364-0213(03)00011-9
Sepulcre
,
J.
,
Liu
,
H.
,
Talukdar
,
T.
,
Martincorena
,
I.
,
Yeo
,
B. T. T.
, &
Buckner
,
R. L.
(
2010
).
The organization of local and distant functional connectivity in the human brain
.
PLoS Computational Biology
,
6
,
e1000808
. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000808
Sheline
,
Y. I.
,
Barch
,
D. M.
,
Price
,
J. L.
,
Rundle
,
M. M.
,
Vaishnavi
,
S. N.
,
Snyder
,
A. Z.
, …
Raichle
,
M. E.
(
2009
).
The default mode network and self-referential processes in depression
.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
,
106
,
1942
1947
.
Siegel
,
J. S.
,
Mitra
,
A.
,
Laumann
,
T. O.
,
Seitzman
,
B. A.
,
Raichle
,
M.
,
Corbetta
,
M.
, &
Snyder
,
A. Z.
(
2016
).
Data quality influences observed links between functional connectivity and behavior
.
Cerebral Cortex
. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhw253
Spronk
,
M.
,
Ji
,
J. L.
,
Kulkarni
,
K.
,
Repovs
,
G.
,
Anticevic
,
A.
, &
Cole
,
M. W.
(
2017
).
Mapping the human brain’s cortical-subcortical functional network organization
.
bioRxiv
206292
.
Sylvester
,
C. M.
,
Corbetta
,
M.
,
Raichle
,
M. E.
,
Rodebaugh
,
T. L.
,
Schlaggar
,
B. L.
,
Sheline
,
Y. I.
, &
Lenze
,
E. J.
(
2012
).
Functional network dysfunction in anxiety and anxiety disorders
.
Trends in Neurosciences
,
35
,
527
535
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2012.04.012
Van Dam
,
N. T.
, &
Earleywine
,
M.
(
2011
).
Validation of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies depression scale—Revised (CESD-R): Pragmatic depression assessment in the general population
.
Psychiatry Research
,
186
,
128
132
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2010.08.018
Van Essen
,
D. C.
,
Smith
,
S. M.
,
Barch
,
D. M.
,
Behrens
,
T. E. J.
,
Yacoub
,
E.
, &
Ugurbil
,
K.
(
2013
).
The WU-Minn Human Connectome Project: An overview
.
NeuroImage
,
80
,
62
79
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.041
Veer
,
I. M.
(
2010
).
Whole brain resting-state analysis reveals decreased functional connectivity in major depression
.
Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience
,
4
. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2010.00041
Wang
,
L.
,
Xia
,
M.
,
Li
,
K.
,
Zeng
,
Y.
,
Su
,
Y.
,
Dai
,
W.
, …
Si
,
T.
(
2015
).
The effects of antidepressant treatment on resting-state functional brain networks in patients with major depressive disorder: Antidepressant effects on depressed brain networks
.
Human Brain Mapping
,
36
,
768
778
. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22663
Wei
,
X.
,
Shen
,
H.
,
Ren
,
J.
,
Li
,
X.
,
Xu
,
X.
,
Yang
,
R.
, …
Jiang
,
X.
(
2014
).
Altered resting-state connectivity in college students with nonclinical depressive symptoms
.
PLoS ONE
,
9
,
e114603
. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0114603
Wig
,
G. S.
,
Schlaggar
,
B. L.
, &
Petersen
,
S. E.
(
2011
).
Concepts and principles in the analysis of brain networks: Brain networks
.
Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences
,
1224
,
126
146
. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05947.x
World Health Organization
. (
1992
).
The ICD-10 classifications of mental and behavioural disorder: Clinical descriptions and diagnostic guidelines
.
Geneva
:
World Health Organization
.
World Health Organization
. (
2017
).
Depression and other common mental disorders: Global health estimates
.
Geneva
:
World Health Organization
.
Yang
,
G. J.
,
Murray
,
J. D.
,
Wang
,
X.-J.
,
Glahn
,
D. C.
,
Pearlson
,
G. D.
,
Repovs
,
G.
, …
Anticevic
,
A.
(
2016
).
Functional hierarchy underlies preferential connectivity disturbances in schizophrenia
.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
,
113
,
E219
E228
. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1508436113
Yeo
,
B. T. T.
,
Krienen
,
F. M.
,
Sepulcre
,
J.
,
Sabuncu
,
M. R.
,
Lashkari
,
D.
,
Hollinshead
,
M.
, …
Buckner
,
R. L.
(
2011
).
The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity
.
Journal of Neurophysiology
,
106
,
1125
1165
. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00338.2011

Author notes

Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Handling Editor: Olaf Sporns

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. For a full description of the license, please visit https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/legalcode.