Understanding the neuroplastic capacity of people with Down syndrome (PwDS) can potentially reveal the causal relationship between aberrant brain organization and phenotypic characteristics. We used resting-state EEG recordings to identify how a neuroplasticity-triggering training protocol relates to changes in the functional connectivity of the brain’s intrinsic cortical networks. Brain activity of 12 PwDS before and after a 10-week protocol of combined physical and cognitive training was statistically compared to quantify changes in directed functional connectivity in conjunction with psychosomatometric assessments. PwDS showed increased connectivity within the left hemisphere and from left-to-right hemisphere, as well as increased physical and cognitive performance. Our findings reveal a strong adaptive neuroplastic reorganization as a result of the training that leads to a less-random network with a more pronounced hierarchical organization. Our results go beyond previous findings by indicating a transition to a healthier, more efficient, and flexible network architecture, with improved integration and segregation abilities in the brain of PwDS. Resting-state electrophysiological brain activity is used here for the first time to display meaningful relationships to underlying Down syndrome processes and outcomes of importance in a translational inquiry. This trial is registered with ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier NCT04390321.

The effects of cognitive and physical training on the neuroplasticity attributes of people with and without cognitive impairment have been well documented via neurophysiological evaluations and network science indices. However, there is still insufficient evidence for people with Down syndrome (PwDS). We investigated the effects of a combinational training protocol on the brain network organization of 12 adult PwDS by using EEG and network indices coupled with tests assessing their cognitive and physical capacity. We report evidence of adaptational neuroplastic effects, pointing to a transitional state toward a healthier organization with an increased ability to integrate and segregate information. Our findings underline the ability of the Down syndrome brain to respond to the cognitive demands of external stimuli, reflecting the possibility of developing independent-living skills.

Neuroplasticity can emerge in both typical and atypical brains and allows for either development, reaction, recovery, or adaptation to internal and external stimuli (Trojan & Pokorny, 1999). In contrast to typically developed (TD) individuals, the Down syndrome (DS) brain presents atypical levels of inhibition due to gene overexpression, leading to prolonged failure of synaptic plasticity and a reduced capacity for remodeling (Baroncelli et al., 2011). These characteristics, along with morphogenetic modifications, are among the leading causes of brain disability in people with DS (PwDS) (Dierssen, Herault, & Estivill, 2009).

DS individuals, in comparison to age-matched TD individuals, have smaller frontal, amygdala, and cerebellar volumes (but increased parahippocampal volume) (Pinter, Eliez, Schmitt, Capone, & Reiss, 2001; White, Alkire, & Haier, 2003), a pattern which overpronounces after their 50th year of age (Dierssen, 2012). The current consensus has associated the cognitive capabilities and deficiencies of PwDS with several distinct brain regions emphasizing an abnormal and less efficient DS brain organization (Menghini, Costanzo, & Vicari, 2011).

Electroencephalography (EEG) and magnetoencephalography (MEG) studies have complemented the notion of this atypical organization. PwDS, when compared to TD controls, exhibit slow brain wave, especially in left posterior areas (Babiloni et al., 2009, 2010), with higher delta band and lower alpha and beta band activity, a pattern also evident in patients with Alzheimer’s disease (AD) (Babiloni et al., 2009, 2010; García-Alba et al., 2019; Hemmati, Ahmadlou, Gharib, Vameghi, & Sajedi, 2013). fMRI studies have shown a rather simplified DS network organization (Anderson et al., 2013), lacking the appropriate efficiency and flexibility (Edgin, Clark, Massand, & Karmiloff-Smith, 2015). Given the limited capacity of the DS brain to consolidate information because of its disorganized architecture of reduced segregation and impaired integration, diffused connectivity (hypersynchrony) (Anderson et al., 2013), and decreased long-range connectivity, the DS brain network’s potential for plasticity is in question (Edgin et al., 2015).

Nevertheless, the DS brain does possess neuroplastic capabilities, at least in the form of compensatory events (Haier, Head, Head, & Lott, 2008; Takashima et al., 2014). The emergence of AD-related characteristics (i.e., altered theta band activity) in PwDS reaffirms the possibility of compensatory mechanisms before and during the expression of AD (Takashima et al., 2014). Characterizing neuroplasticity in PwDS is vital in understanding causality between aberrant brain circuitry and the cognitive and behavioral phenotype. This step would allow the quantification of the remodeling potential of evidence-based interventions.

Interventional approaches have shifted from a pharmaceutical concept, with, so far, inconclusive results regarding beneficial effects (Costa, 2011; Kálmán et al., 2009; Kishnani et al., 2010; Lott et al., 2011; Mohan, Carpenter, & Bennett, 2009), to nonpharmaceutical interventions of physical and behavioral components, aiming to trigger neuroplasticity and enhance brain health or to protect against neurodegenerative events (Head, Lott, Patterson, Doran, & Haier, 2007). Such interventions have shown promise in healthy aging and populations with cognitive impairments and have provided mounting evidence for lifelong brain plasticity (Bamidis et al., 2014; Colcombe et al., 2004, 2006; Cotman, Berchtold, & Christie, 2007; Erickson et al., 2011; Klados, Styliadis, Frantzidis, Paraskevopoulos, & Bamidis, 2016; Kramer et al., 1999; Styliadis, Kartsidis, & Paraskevopoulos, 2015; Styliadis, Kartsidis, Paraskevopoulos, Ioannides, & Bamidis, 2015).

Despite evidence supporting the positive impact of exercise-based (Bertapelli, Pitetti, Agiovlasitis, & Guerra-Junior, 2016; Hardee & Fetters, 2017), as well as behavioral and cognitive interventions (Fonseca, Navatta, Bottino, & Miotto, 2015; McGlinchey, McCarron, Holland, & McCallion, 2019) for PwDS, DS literature features a substantial knowledge gap, as there are no reports of neurophysiological investigations of the training-induced effects, and a network theory–based assessment of the training-induced plasticity. Deviations in the brain functionality of PwDS have been previously addressed with the investigation of resting-state networks (RSNs) (Ahmadlou, Gharib, Vameghi, & Sajedi, 2013; Anderson et al., 2013; Babiloni et al., 2009, 2010; Pujol et al., 2015; Vega, Hohman, Pryweller, Dykens, & Thornton-Wells, 2015).

We hypothesized that combined physical (PT) and cognitive training (CT) would trigger neuroplasticity and reorganize the DS brain network to adapt to the increased cognitive and physical demand. Resting-state EEG data were acquired to characterize plasticity in the DS brain through network science and investigate how the training-induced neuroplasticity affects the organization and characteristics of the DS network, as opposed to the reported random-like architecture (Ahmadlou et al., 2013) with impaired integration and segregation capabilities.

Source analysis was performed using low-resolution electromagnetic tomography (LORETA) (Pascual-Marqui, Michel, & Lehmann, 1994), and directed functional connectivity was estimated via phase transfer entropy (PTE) in 0.53–35 Hz. This measure quantifies the direction of the information flow, allowing for a whole-head analysis without requiring an a priori head model definition. A nonparametric permutation test was applied to extract the significant, within-network differences (post- vs. pre-intervention) in connectivity. A graph-theoretical analysis was performed to index the training’s neuroplastic effects through the statistical comparison of graph measures between the two time points.

Psychometric and Somatometric Results

Psychosomatometric score comparisons between the two time points were performed with the use of nonparametric Wilcoxon tests and paired t tests. Physical assessments exhibited a significant improvement in score for the arm curl test (Table 1). Significant differences in the time of completion (decrease in duration) were evident in the Time Up and Go assessment test (Table 1). The comparison of psychometric assessments revealed an increase in the Digits Forward score (Digits Span test), the Mazes test, as well as the Ravens AB and Ravens total score (Table 1). The rest of the tests showed no significant post-pre score changes.

Table 1. 
Somatometric and psychometric tests with significant differences post vs. pre, along with the post-pre mean difference
Somatometric P value Mean difference 
Arm curl 0.017 1.75 
Time Up and Go 0.007 −1.67 
Psychometric 
Digits Span (Digit Forward) 0.032 1.50 
Mazes 0.032 0.97 
Ravens AB 0.017 2.12 
Ravens total score 0.015 3.13 
Somatometric P value Mean difference 
Arm curl 0.017 1.75 
Time Up and Go 0.007 −1.67 
Psychometric 
Digits Span (Digit Forward) 0.032 1.50 
Mazes 0.032 0.97 
Ravens AB 0.017 2.12 
Ravens total score 0.015 3.13 

Note. Results were considered significant for p < 0.05.

PTE Results

Statistical comparisons of the PTE matrices between the pre- and post-intervention EEG measurements indicate the reorganization of cortical connections (62 nodes and 40 edges), due to training, in the DS resting-state network (p < 0.05, corrected for multiple comparisons via the nonparametric network-based statistics [NBS] method; 5,000 permutations; Zalesky, Fornito, & Bullmore, 2010) (Figure 1). Specifically, the cortical reorganization in the DS brain is characterized by the strengthening of connections within (i) the left hemisphere, from nodes in the occipital (cuneus and lingual gyrus) and the temporal lobe (fusiform gyrus) to the frontal lobe (superior and middle gyrus), and from the frontal lobe to the parietal lobe (superior, precuneus and postcentral gyrus); (ii) the right hemisphere, from nodes in the occipital (cuneus, lingual and middle gyrus) and temporal (fusiform and inferior gyrus) lobe to the frontal lobe (superior, cingulate and medial gyrus); and between: (i) nodes of the left fusiform gyrus, and inferior temporal lobe and the right frontal lobe (superior, middle and inferior gyrus); (ii) the right middle occipital lobe, subgyral, parahippocampal and fusiform gyrus to the left frontal lobe (superior, middle, precentral gyrus); (iii) the left inferior temporal lobe to the right anterior cingulate; and (iv) the right frontal lobe (middle and inferior gyrus) to the left postcentral and middle frontal gyrus (Figure 1). In respect to the core RSNs, as defined by Yeo et al. (2011), we have noted increased connectivity within the dorsal attention network (DAN), and between the visual (VIS) and fronto-parietal network (FPN), VIS and default-mode-network (DMN), VIS and ventral attention network (VAN), VAN and FPN, FPN and DMN, FPN and somatosensory network (SMN), FPN and DAN, DAN and DMN, as well as DAN and SMN (Table 2).

Figure 1. 

Cortical connectivity between post- and pre-intervention networks and for each time point (t value > 3.52). The color scales represent t values. (A) Circular graph depicting the cortical reorganization in the Down syndrome (DS) brain (cyan: left hemisphere; lime: right hemisphere; blue: frontal lobe; green: occipital lobe; purple: parietal lobe; orange: temporal lobe; maroon: limbic lobe). The cortical reorganization is characterized by the strengthening of direct connections within (i) the left hemisphere, from nodes in the occipital and temporal lobe to the frontal lobe, and from the frontal lobe to the parietal lobe; (ii) the right hemisphere, from nodes in the occipital and temporal lobe to the frontal lobe; and between (i) nodes of the left fusiform gyrus, and inferior temporal lobe and the right frontal lobe; (ii) the right middle occipital lobe, subgyral, parahippocampal and fusiform gyrus to the left frontal lobe; (iii) the left inferior temporal lobe to the right anterior cingulate; and (iv) the right frontal lobe to the left postcentral and middle frontal gyrus. (B) Depiction of pre- and post-intervention networks in comparison to the null hypothesis. The t values indicate that the post-resting-state network has significantly stronger connections than the pre-network. (C) Post vs. Pre. Significant post-pre connectivity differences. Information direction is depicted through line arrows. The visualized networks are significant at a level of p < 0.05, corrected for multiple comparisons via the nonparametric NBS method with an internal threshold of 0.8. The difference in nodal size depicts the increase in the node degree centrality; the nodes with the most increased connectivity are located at the left parietal lobe.

Figure 1. 

Cortical connectivity between post- and pre-intervention networks and for each time point (t value > 3.52). The color scales represent t values. (A) Circular graph depicting the cortical reorganization in the Down syndrome (DS) brain (cyan: left hemisphere; lime: right hemisphere; blue: frontal lobe; green: occipital lobe; purple: parietal lobe; orange: temporal lobe; maroon: limbic lobe). The cortical reorganization is characterized by the strengthening of direct connections within (i) the left hemisphere, from nodes in the occipital and temporal lobe to the frontal lobe, and from the frontal lobe to the parietal lobe; (ii) the right hemisphere, from nodes in the occipital and temporal lobe to the frontal lobe; and between (i) nodes of the left fusiform gyrus, and inferior temporal lobe and the right frontal lobe; (ii) the right middle occipital lobe, subgyral, parahippocampal and fusiform gyrus to the left frontal lobe; (iii) the left inferior temporal lobe to the right anterior cingulate; and (iv) the right frontal lobe to the left postcentral and middle frontal gyrus. (B) Depiction of pre- and post-intervention networks in comparison to the null hypothesis. The t values indicate that the post-resting-state network has significantly stronger connections than the pre-network. (C) Post vs. Pre. Significant post-pre connectivity differences. Information direction is depicted through line arrows. The visualized networks are significant at a level of p < 0.05, corrected for multiple comparisons via the nonparametric NBS method with an internal threshold of 0.8. The difference in nodal size depicts the increase in the node degree centrality; the nodes with the most increased connectivity are located at the left parietal lobe.

Close modal
Table 2. 
Significant increase in connectivity of resting-state networks as defined by Yeo et al. (2011).
graphic
graphic

Note. The black color signifies the within-network connectivity, the dark gray color the bilateral connectivity between-networks, and the light gray color the unilateral connectivity between-networks from the RSN in the first column to the RSN in the first row.

Graph Measures

The significant changes (p < 0.05) in global efficiency (GE), transitivity (TS), characteristic path length (CPL), clustering coefficient (CC), betweenness centrality (BC), and the small-world (SW) measure, sigma (σ), between the pre- and post-networks were investigated using analysis of covariance, where graph measures served as the dependent values and density as the covariate (Table 3). Regarding the global measures, we report increases in GE and TS and a decrease in CPL. From the comparison of local measures, CC shows significant differences in 716 nodes, with increased values in 607 of these nodes (Figure 2). From these, 323 nodes belong to the left hemisphere (102 in the frontal lobe [FL], 39 in the central midline [CM], 56 in the parietal lobe [PL], 49 in the temporal lobe [TL], 26 in the limbic lobe [LL], 49 in the occipital lobe [OL]) and 284 to the right hemisphere (76 in FL, 51 in CM, 37 in PL, 49 in TL, 28 in LL, 44 in OL) (Figure 2). From the significant nodes, 109 showcased a significant decrease in value, with 48 nodes localized in the left hemisphere (7 in FL, 6 in CM, 6 in PL, 19 in TL, 2 in LL, 8 in OL) and 61 in the right hemisphere (22 in FL, 2 in CM, 8 in PL, 9 in TL, 3 in LL, 17 in OL). BC increased in 15 nodes with 9 pertaining to the left hemisphere (4 in the medial FL, 2 in PL [precuneus and superior PL], 1 in the inferior TL, 1 in the cingulate gyrus of LL, 1 in the middle OL) and 6 to the right (2 in FL [inferior and medial gyrus], 2 in the precuneus [PL], 1 in the cingulate gyrus [LL], 1 in the lingual gyrus [OL]), while it decreased in 2 nodes of the right angular gyrus (LL). The post- vs. pre-comparison of the SW measure revealed a significant increase in the σ-value, but the values for both the pre- and post-intervention networks did not exceed the SW threshold of σ > 1. We performed a multiscale Louvain community detection, which showed that the networks of the two time points are organized in different levels of hierarchy. The community analysis on the pre-intervention DS network indicated that its communities are nested within three levels of hierarchy with the number of communities across all levels ranging from 70 communities to 442, while the communities of the post-DS network are organized in four levels, with their number ranging from 45 to 290 across all levels.

Table 3. 
Significant post-pre changes in global (global efficiency, transitivity, characteristic path length) and local (local clustering coefficient, node betweenness centrality) graph measures as well as the small-worldness measure
Graph measureP valueDirection of effect
Global efficiency 2.91e-12 Increase 
Transitivity 3.38e-11 Increase 
Characteristic path length 3.47e-10 Decrease 
Clustering coefficient 0.0115/0.0119* Increase/decrease 
Node betweenness centrality 0.0386/0.0463* Increase/decrease 
Small-worldness 5.61e-7 Increase 
Graph measureP valueDirection of effect
Global efficiency 2.91e-12 Increase 
Transitivity 3.38e-11 Increase 
Characteristic path length 3.47e-10 Decrease 
Clustering coefficient 0.0115/0.0119* Increase/decrease 
Node betweenness centrality 0.0386/0.0463* Increase/decrease 
Small-worldness 5.61e-7 Increase 

Note. Results were considered significant for p < 0.05.

*

For the local graph measures, the reported p value is the mean value of the nodes showing a significant increase and a significant decrease, respectively.

Figure 2. 

Depiction of the shifts in the clustering coefficient (CC) of nodes. The colormap indicates significance at an alpha value (α < 0.05) coupled with the direction of the shift. The black nodes have no significant shifts in CC values post- and pre-intervention, blue nodes show a significant increase at α < 0.05, while magenta nodes show a significant decrease at α < 0.05.

Figure 2. 

Depiction of the shifts in the clustering coefficient (CC) of nodes. The colormap indicates significance at an alpha value (α < 0.05) coupled with the direction of the shift. The black nodes have no significant shifts in CC values post- and pre-intervention, blue nodes show a significant increase at α < 0.05, while magenta nodes show a significant decrease at α < 0.05.

Close modal

A 10-week intervention of combined PT and CT in adults with DS can trigger neuroplasticity resulting in cortical reorganization, quantified via EEG indices, and graph measures. Our results reveal that (i) short-term training can modify the physical and cognitive performance of adults with DS, (ii) the DS brain can adapt to novel stimulation and challenges by utilizing neuroplasticity and reorganizing itself, and (iii) adaptive neuroplasticity may lead to a more flexible and healthier functional network.

Short-Term Training Modifies Physical and Cognitive Performance

Our PT findings indicate the significant improvement of physical capacity for upper body strength and endurance, mobility, static and dynamic balance, and are in line with previous findings on adult PwDS (Hardee & Fetters, 2017; Ruiz-González, Lucena-Antón, Salazar, Martín-Valero, & Moral-Munoz, 2019). However, our protocol targets multiple physical domains, while most studies investigating the effect of PT on the somatic capacity of adult PwDS (Hardee & Fetters, 2017; Ruiz-González et al., 2019) have mainly focused on a specific domain, that is, resistance training or aerobic training and, less often, on a combination of two or more domains.

With respect to CT, our PwDS exhibited an improved general cognitive capacity, that is, in the level of general intelligence, planning and organization skills, and in short-term memory, attention, and concentration. Previous cognitive interventions on adult PwDS have mainly focused on memory (Fonseca et al., 2015) and, less often, on executive functions (McGlinchey et al., 2019). In line with the findings of the latter study, adult PwDS can complete a computerized CT program that targets multiple domains of cognition and exhibit significant improvements.

Cortical Reorganization in the DS Brain and Neuroplastic Capacity

PwDS present capacity for neuroplasticity. Regarding the type of neuroplasticity that is expressed here, we can rule out the triggering of evolutionary and reactive plasticity since the first is most active during the development of the brain and the latter is most commonly triggered by a single stimulus (Trojan & Pokorny, 1999) (our training utilizes repetitive stimuli). Adaptational and reparation plasticity can both cause cortical reorganization through long-term stimulation. Here, combined PT and CT have increased the communication within and between nodes of the two hemispheres (Figure 1A and 1C). The DS cortical networks are characterized by a simplified organization (Anderson et al., 2013) with hypersynchrony between brain regions (Anderson et al., 2013; Edgin et al., 2015; Vega et al., 2015). Therefore, the reported shifts in connectivity indicate the emergence of a reorganized network, which can be the result of either adaptational or reparation plasticity. Our finding regarding the increased left intrahemispheric information flow can be regarded as the result of adaptational neuroplasticity being triggered in the DS brain given the atypical right-hemispheric functional preference (Elliott, Weeks, & Chua, 1994). However, the left-to-right hemisphere directionality is a pattern that has been previously evidenced in young PwDS (Babiloni et al., 2009), and its enhancement in adult PwDS may indicate that the training has triggered reparation neuroplasticity.

The enhanced connectivity within (DAN) and between (VIS and FPN, VIS and DMN, VIS and VAN, VAN and FPN, FPN and DMN, FPN and SMN, FPN and DAN, DAN and DMN, as well as DAN and SMN) the core RSNs (Yeo et al., 2011) further highlights the post-intervention DS functional reorganization. In respect to the aforementioned findings of Anderson et al. (2013), the strengthening of connections between distinct RSNs may also favor that the induced neuroplastic attributes represent adaptation. Given the evidence pointing toward an inverse relationship between the level of cognitive impairment of PwDS and the within DAN (Pujol et al., 2015; Vega et al., 2015) connectivity (i.e., the lower the DAN connectivity, the higher the cognitive impairment for DS), we suggest that the increased connectivity within DAN may associate with the increased cognitive capacity due to training in our DS sample. This would rule out the possibility that the type of plasticity represents reparation. In essence, future studies could potentially confirm or rule out this possibility.

The cortical reorganization, in conjunction with the increases in general intelligence, indicates that the DS brain has entered a more flexible state. The DS network has been previously reported to lack the flexibility that characterizes the TD brain (Edgin et al., 2015) and subsequently higher intelligence (Barbey, 2018; Hilger, Fukushima, Sporns, & Fiebach, 2019). Flexibility can reflect the brain’s capacity to adapt to novel stimulations (Barbey, 2018). It is plausible that the transition to a healthier organization emerges from the increased DS network flexibility and further points to the triggering of adaptational neuroplastic attributes.

Graph Theory Characteristics Support the Neuroplastic Transition Toward a Healthier DS Network Organization

We suggest that the network’s reorganization indexes a transitional state from a random-like network toward a healthier functional architecture, exhibiting less-random characteristics, increased flexibility and robustness, and improved integration and segregation capabilities (please see Figure 3 for an illustration of our theoretical proposal).

Figure 3. 

Illustration of our theoretical proposal. Changes in connectivity and graph-theory characteristics, as an outcome of the adaptational neuroplasticity in the DS brain, characterize the post-intervention DS network (B → C) as a transitional state from the random-like organization of the pre-intervention DS network (Ahmadlou et al., 2013) (B) toward a healthier functional structure. Random networks (Erdös–Rényi, random graph) (A) are characterized by high global efficiency (GE; overintegration) and low clustering (under-segregation) and exemplify the general abilities of general intelligence (low intelligence level). Small-world (SW) networks (C) feature the functional organization of typically developed brain networks and incorporate characteristics of both random and regular networks, achieving an optimal balance between global and local characteristics. SW networks (C) are associated with the broad abilities’ component of general intelligence (higher intelligence level). The pre-intervention DS network (B) showed a random-like, simplified architecture, with impaired segregation and integration, as evidenced by the low CC (random network characteristic) and decreased GE and increased characteristic path length (CPL), respectively. This is in line with previous literature (Ahmadlou et al., 2013; Anderson et al., 2013; Pujol et al., 2015; Vega et al., 2015). The DS pre-network’s (B) integration-related characteristics (lower GE, higher CPL) are not common in random networks. Hence, the DS pre-network (B) is classified as random-like and not entirely random, maintaining an equal distance from random (A) and SW (C) networks. The DS post-network (B → C) exhibits an increase in integration (random network characteristic), as well as segregation (regular network characteristic), so it is interpreted as a step toward an SW-like architecture that highlights a healthier brain organization.

Figure 3. 

Illustration of our theoretical proposal. Changes in connectivity and graph-theory characteristics, as an outcome of the adaptational neuroplasticity in the DS brain, characterize the post-intervention DS network (B → C) as a transitional state from the random-like organization of the pre-intervention DS network (Ahmadlou et al., 2013) (B) toward a healthier functional structure. Random networks (Erdös–Rényi, random graph) (A) are characterized by high global efficiency (GE; overintegration) and low clustering (under-segregation) and exemplify the general abilities of general intelligence (low intelligence level). Small-world (SW) networks (C) feature the functional organization of typically developed brain networks and incorporate characteristics of both random and regular networks, achieving an optimal balance between global and local characteristics. SW networks (C) are associated with the broad abilities’ component of general intelligence (higher intelligence level). The pre-intervention DS network (B) showed a random-like, simplified architecture, with impaired segregation and integration, as evidenced by the low CC (random network characteristic) and decreased GE and increased characteristic path length (CPL), respectively. This is in line with previous literature (Ahmadlou et al., 2013; Anderson et al., 2013; Pujol et al., 2015; Vega et al., 2015). The DS pre-network’s (B) integration-related characteristics (lower GE, higher CPL) are not common in random networks. Hence, the DS pre-network (B) is classified as random-like and not entirely random, maintaining an equal distance from random (A) and SW (C) networks. The DS post-network (B → C) exhibits an increase in integration (random network characteristic), as well as segregation (regular network characteristic), so it is interpreted as a step toward an SW-like architecture that highlights a healthier brain organization.

Close modal

In close relation, the SW measure, which classifies the pre- and post-intervention networks as non-SW, shows a significant increase in the post-DS network and can, in turn, attribute the adult DS functional network (pre-intervention network) with features of a random network (Ahmadlou et al., 2013). Thus, the increased SW can index a transition to a less-random organization. The randomness of the pre-DS network is not surprising, given that basic cognitive abilities (low level of general intelligence, which also characterizes PwDS) are exemplified by random networks, while broad cognitive abilities (high level of general intelligence) would require a SW organization (Barbey, 2018; Hilger et al., 2019; Watts & Strogatz, 1998). SW networks maintain an optimal balance of integration and segregation (combining random and regular network characteristics) to support general and specific abilities (Barbey, 2018; Hilger et al., 2019; Watts & Strogatz, 1998). In contrast, the absence of SW properties in the DS brain indicates the increased risk of communication loss between connected regions and their causality for cognitive decline (Ahmadlou et al., 2013; Sanz et al., 2010).

Considering the DS brain (lack of functional specialization and impaired integration (Anderson et al., 2013), the simultaneous increment in GE and TS denotes the increase of both integration and segregation and the transition to a healthier network organization, with the rise in TS further supporting the reorganization to a less-random architecture. The decrease of CPL in TD individuals could be interpreted as a contradicting shift toward a more random network that disturbs the SW balance. In our case, the CPL is a step toward the opposite direction since the adult DS network deviates from random networks by displaying a greater CPL, thus characterizing it as “random-like” (Figure 3). Previous fMRI studies report that the brain network of PwDS presents decreased long-range connectivity (Anderson et al., 2013; Pujol et al., 2015) and thus increased CPL (Bassett & Bullmore, 2006). Hence, the decreased CPL supports our hypothesis and, coupled with the previously mentioned shifts, signals the decrease in wiring cost and subsequent increase in cost efficacy (Achard & Bullmore, 2007; Bullmore & Sporns, 2012).

Our hypothesis of a shift toward a healthier network organization is also evident from a local network perspective. The increased CC in most areas of the brain indicates a rise in robustness, and fault tolerance in the network (Achard & Bullmore, 2007), which can potentially serve in neuroprotection for PwDS (Ahmadlou et al., 2013). Still, the effectiveness of such a mechanism would rely on the trade-off between integration and segregation, mainly maintaining SW network characteristics (Stam, Jones, Nolte, Breakspear, & Scheltens, 2007). The DS network, when compared to a TD network architecture of distributed communities, has been found to exhibit a lobar organization with increased local communication (Anderson et al., 2013) and reduced BC (Hemmati et al., 2013), making the aggregation of information less efficient. The aforementioned evidence, coupled with our increased hierarchical organization of communities in the post-DS network, and the increase of BC in nodes, support the transition to a more hierarchical organization, rendering the network more efficient in the coherent distribution of information.

Future Directions and Limitations

A limitation of the study was the small sample size of the experimental group. Our results could potentially be interpreted differently in reference to a control group (TD or passive DS). Though this study is the first to provide insights into the benefits of combined PT and CT in adults with DS through neuropsychological and neurophysiological assessments, a gap remains in our basic understanding of the sole contributions of each training to DS-related cognition and brain function. Further studies are necessary to index the influence of each component on the triggering of neuroplasticity and subsequently network organization and cognition. Similarly, the stability of the reported neuroplastic shifts can only be addressed through additional and more in-depth investigations, including follow-up studies, to fill the gaps in the existing literature. Finally, the correlation of the somatometric and/or psychometric scores to the graph measures would increase the strength of our results, as it would indicate that the neuroplastic effect that we report is also reflected in behavior. We explored such a relationship between the changes in graph measures and the shifts in psychosomatic assessments (which showed post-pre significant changes) via a (i) correlation, (ii) linear regression, and (iii) multiple linear regression. The lack of a significant correlation was probably due to the small sample size, which did not produce enough power to quantify the effect, or due to the greater sensitivity that the neuroimaging methods show with regard to learning-induced neuroplasticity compared to the behavioral ones (Andrés, Parmentier, & Escera, 2006).

Conclusion

The rise of DS prevalence sets a significant challenge in developing innovative health care interventions that provide the best quality of care. Neurocognitive treatments can augment the functional and cognitive abilities of PwDS and thus allow for a more independent, productive, and fulfilling daily life. Our results, based on brain imaging, connectivity, and graph theory analysis, emphasize the significance of introducing stimulation and adaptable challenges in the environment of PwDS. We provide evidence that triggering adaptational neuroplasticity in the DS brain provokes the emergence of a less-random, more hierarchical, and flexible network, able to integrate and segregate information more efficiently. Designing focalized neurobehavioral interventions can aid in the development of a balanced and stable functional DS phenotype in terms of SW characteristics.

Subjects

The study’s pool consisted of 12 subjects with DS (age: 29 ± 11, 6 females). Participants were recruited from a variety of local organizations (please see Acknowledgments). The training procedure took place in the Thessaloniki Active and Healthy Ageing Living Lab (Thess-AHALL) (Konstantinidis, Billis, Bratsas, Siountas, & Bamidis, 2016), and the premises of the Greek Association of Down Syndrome. This study is part of the dsLLM clinical trial, registered with ClinicalTrials.gov, with identifier code NCT04390321. The study protocol was approved by the Bioethics Committee of the Medical School of the Aristotle University of Thessaloniki and was conducted per the Helsinki Declaration of Human Rights. The participants’ legal guardians signed written informed consent before their inclusion in the study.

Long Lasting Memories Care Intervention

The intervention protocol consisted of PT and CT Long Lasting Memories (LLM) Care (http://www.llmcare.gr/en) (Bamidis et al., 2015). The study protocol was further developed to improve the quality of life and aid in the development of independent-living skills in PwDS (Bamidis, Konstantinidis, Billis, & Siountas, 2017; Romanopoulou, Zilidou, Savvidis, Chatzisevastou-Loukidou, & Bamidis, 2018). LLM Care also aims to further improve brain functionality (Bamidis et al., 2015). All training sessions were computerized, center-based, and conducted under supervision. The sequence of training methods was pseudo-randomized and counterbalanced. The details of each training intervention have been previously described in detail (Billis et al., 2010; Smith et al., 2009) and are summarized in Figure 4.

Figure 4. 

Down Syndrome–Long Lasting Memories (DS-LLM) Care design and flow of participants with DS. CT, cognitive training; PT, physical training.

Figure 4. 

Down Syndrome–Long Lasting Memories (DS-LLM) Care design and flow of participants with DS. CT, cognitive training; PT, physical training.

Close modal

Cognitive Training

The CT component of LLM Care uses the BrainHQ software (Posit Science Corporation, San Francisco, CA), an online interactive environment in the Greek language (Romanopoulou, Zilidou, & Bamidis, 2017). It consists of six categories (29 exercises in total), with customizable difficulty levels, utilizing audiovisual stimuli. CT targets memory, attention, cognitive processing speed, navigation, and people skills. This regime was selected since patients with DS exhibit deficiencies in these processes. CT was conducted for half an hour, with a frequency of 2 days per week for 10 weeks. In every CT session, the participants were required to complete at least one task from each category. During the training, participants were urged to complete as many exercises as they could from each category.

Physical Training

The PT component of LLM Care is based on the WebFitForAll protocol (Billis et al., 2010; Konstantinidis, Bamparopoulos, & Bamidis, 2016) adequately adjusted to the needs and capacity of PwDS. It utilizes motion sensor devices (i.e., Kinect). Games and physical exercise are combined, providing a pleasant experience throughout the training. PT sessions lasted for half an hour and were conducted with the same frequency as CT. The training consists of aerobic (cycling, in-place hiking), flexibility (stretching), strength (resistance, weightlifting), and balance (static, dynamic) exercises. The warm-up and cool-down routines (5-minutes duration) signify the start and completion of every session, respectively. During aerobic exercises, the participants entered a virtual environment, set up in Google maps, and explored cities and landscapes. Upon correct completion of the flexibility and strength exercises, the trainees were progressively rewarded with an array of pleasing images. The scope of balance exercises was to move their bodies either horizontally or vertically, which was achieved through games.

Psychometric and Somatometric Assessments

The participants’ cognitive and physical capacity was assessed before and after the intervention. The psychometric evaluation consisted of a set of neurocognitive tests that measure memory, attention, concentration (WISC-III: Digits Span) (Woolger, 2001), verbal and nonverbal mental capabilities (Raven) (John & Raven, 2003), processing speed (WISC-III: Digits Span and Picture Arrangement), problem solving, visuospatial processing, organization skills (WISC-III: Block Design, Picture Arrangement, Mazes), social intelligence (WISC-III: Picture Arrangement), and identification of emotions (reading the mind in the eyes [Baron-Cohen, Jolliffe, Mortimore, & Robertson, 1997; Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Hill, Raste, & Plumb, 2001], and a variation: reading the mind in the face [emotion recognition from video]).

The somatometric evaluation included the Short Physical Performance Battery (Guralnik et al., 1994), 10-meter walk (Bohannon, 1997), back scratch (Jones & Rikli, 2002), sit and reach (Wells & Dillon, 1952), arm curl (Jones & Rikli, 2002), four square step (Whitney, Marchetti, Morris, & Sparto, 2007), stork balance (for both legs) (Johnson & Nelson, 1979), timed up and go (Shumway-Cook, Brauer, & Woollacott, 2000) tests, and body mass index. These tests appraise functioning mobility, flexibility (in specific areas), dynamic stability, strength, and static and dynamic balance.

EEG Recording

Pre- and post-intervention resting-state EEG activity was recorded for 5 minutes by using a high-density Nihon-Kohden EEG device (128 active scalp electrodes) at a sampling rate of 1,000 Hz. The EEG recordings were performed in an electrically shielded, sound and light-attenuated booth. The electrode impedances were lower than 10 kΩ. The participants were instructed to remain in a resting position while keeping their eyes open. Eyes-closed EEGs were not measured because of the limited capacity of the participants with DS to stay relaxed with their eyes closed.

EEG Data Analysis

Preprocessing.

The raw EEG data were visually inspected, and bad channels were interpolated while any eye movement–related artifacts (blinks and horizontal movement) were corrected through adaptive artifact correction (Ille, Berg, & Scherg, 2002) using the Brain Electrical Source Analysis software (BESA research, version 6, Megis Software, Heidelberg, Germany) (Figure 5, blue section). The artifact-corrected data from each measurement were imported into the Fieldtrip Matlab toolbox (Oostenveld, Fries, Maris, & Schoffelen, 2011) for additional processing (Figure 5, blue section). The signals were denoised by filtering the data (0.53 Hz high-pass IIR filter, 48–52 Hz notch IIR filter, 97 Hz low-pass IIR filter). The filtered signals were analyzed into independent components (Lee, Girolami, & Sejnowski, 1999), and the artifactual components were rejected. After signal reconstruction, the data was, once again, visually inspected for any remaining artifacts. Fifteen segments (4 seconds each) from each artifact-free EEG recording were randomly selected for additional processing.

Figure 5. 

EEG data analysis schematic. Preprocessing (blue): EEG data were interpolated and artifact corrected, visually inspected, high-pass, bandpass, and low-pass filtered. Independent component analysis (ICA), as well as visual inspection, were used to reject artifactual data. Fifteen segments of 4 seconds were randomly selected. Source reconstruction (orange): the data were processed within 0.53–35 Hz frequency range, source reconstructed (4-D LORETA, for all time points), and a previously used 863-node atlas (Paraskevopoulos et al., 2015) was applied to extract the time series of every voxel from every segment per subject, which were used to estimate 1,000 surrogate time series per subject/group. Functional connectivity (green): functional connectivity was computed for every segment of every subject, using the phase transfer entropy metric (PTE), and the 15 matrices of every subject were averaged into one. PTE networks were estimated for every surrogate time series of each subject to create a null model for the rejection of noise in the real data. A network science approach was taken for the computation of graph measures per subject. Group average statistics were calculated to identify the statistically significant differences between post- and pre-intervention networks.

Figure 5. 

EEG data analysis schematic. Preprocessing (blue): EEG data were interpolated and artifact corrected, visually inspected, high-pass, bandpass, and low-pass filtered. Independent component analysis (ICA), as well as visual inspection, were used to reject artifactual data. Fifteen segments of 4 seconds were randomly selected. Source reconstruction (orange): the data were processed within 0.53–35 Hz frequency range, source reconstructed (4-D LORETA, for all time points), and a previously used 863-node atlas (Paraskevopoulos et al., 2015) was applied to extract the time series of every voxel from every segment per subject, which were used to estimate 1,000 surrogate time series per subject/group. Functional connectivity (green): functional connectivity was computed for every segment of every subject, using the phase transfer entropy metric (PTE), and the 15 matrices of every subject were averaged into one. PTE networks were estimated for every surrogate time series of each subject to create a null model for the rejection of noise in the real data. A network science approach was taken for the computation of graph measures per subject. Group average statistics were calculated to identify the statistically significant differences between post- and pre-intervention networks.

Close modal

Source reconstruction.

Each subject’s segments (15 segments, 4,000 samples, 4 seconds duration each) were imported into BESA (Figure 5, orange section). The current density reconstructions (CDR) were estimated for each sample point, solving the inverse problem using LORETA in the 0.5–35Hz frequency range. LORETA (Pascual-Marqui et al., 1994) was utilized, as it does not require the a priori declaration of the number of sources and is suitable for whole cortex analysis. The CDRs were exported as four-dimensional images (4-D) in the Analyze format (keeping all sampling points), which were, in turn, imported into Matlab. A cortex mask was superimposed on the images. The mask includes only gray matter and excludes the subcortex, the brainstem, and cerebellum, to limit the source space (Paraskevopoulos, Kraneburg, Herholz, Bamidis, & Pantev, 2015). The source space consisted of 863 voxels.

Functional connectivity.

After extracting the time series of each voxel from the 4-D images, they were used to compute the phase transfer entropy (PTE) (Lobier, Siebenhühner, Palva, & Palva, 2014) (Figure 5, green section). The computation resulted in 863 × 863 adjacency matrices. The metric is calculated independently for every pair of voxels in each segment. Each voxel represented a node of the brain network, with the node’s coordinates corresponding to the center of each voxel. PTE was selected because its results are not based on a specific data model since its computation is reliant on nonlinear probability distributions. Therefore, it allows the detection of higher order relations in the phase information flow and renders the measure resistant to source leakage (Lobier et al., 2014). The algorithm applies the Hilbert transform to estimate the phases of each signal. It determines the number of bins by utilizing the Scott methodology (Scott, 1992), resulting in 37 bins on average (13 samples per bin). Each subject’s adjacency matrices were averaged, resulting in two sets of 12 adjacency matrices (Figure 5, green section).

When computing network science indices based on information theory, one should take into account that the derived data contain noise that may mask the “real” result. This potential limitation was dealt with by following a methodology proposed by Timme et al. (2016). Thus, subject-level null distributions were computed and then compared with the real PTEs. Two thousand surrogate time series were estimated for each subject (1,000 for the pre-intervention measurements and 1,000 for the post-intervention ones) via phase randomization, that is, calculating the Fourier transform of the real times series, randomizing their phase, inverting the transform, thus resulting in jittered time series (Timme et al., 2016). PTE networks were estimated from the shifted time series and then compared edge by edge to the subject PTE networks, where any real edge that had lesser weight than more than 50 (5% significance level) surrogate edges was considered nonsignificant and removed from the final PTE network (Timme et al., 2016). To ensure that small differences between the surrogate and real data did not affect the edge significance test, the weight difference between them was estimated, and differences less than e-03 were ignored. The comparison of the surrogate PTE distributions to the PTE data resulted in networks consisting of 863 nodes and 184,720 edges on average for the post-intervention networks and 123,980 edges on average for the pre-intervention networks.

Graph measures computation.

Using the Brain Connectivity Toolbox (Rubinov & Sporns, 2010), GE, TS (a variation of global CC), CPL, CC, BC, as well as community detection, and the measure of SW (σ) were computed for each participant. In a later step, the centrality Matlab function, which measures node importance, was used to calculate the node degree centrality (DC) of the nodes showcasing significant shifts in the postintervention network. The density of each graph was estimated by averaging the weights of each graph and multiplying the average by the maximum weight of the network.

These graph measures were chosen to examine the influence of our intervention on the characteristics of the DS brain and its functional organization. To that aim, GE and CPL were utilized to index the effects on integration (Rubinov & Sporns, 2010), TS and CC to measure the impact on segregation (Rubinov & Sporns, 2010), BC and DC to check for shifts in the importance of nodes to the flow of information, and community detection to characterize the hierarchical organization.

GE is the average inverse shortest path length of the network (Latora & Marchiori, 2001) and quantifies the efficacy of information transference and its assimilation in the network (Rubinov & Sporns, 2010). CPL is the average shortest path length of the edges connecting the nodes of the network (Watts & Strogatz, 1998), is influenced by longer paths, and characterizes the robustness of the network (Rubinov & Sporns, 2010). TS is the ratio of closed triplets to the maximum number of triplets (open and closed) (Newman, 2003), while CC of a node is the ratio of its connected neighbors to the maximum number of possible connections (Watts & Strogatz, 1998). These two measures reflect the clustering organization of the DS brain network on a global and local level, respectively (Rubinov & Sporns, 2010). BC corresponds to the fraction of shortest paths that pass through a node (Freeman, 1978) and is a measure of centrality, measuring the importance of a node in the information flow (Rubinov & Sporns, 2010), and DC estimates the number of links connected to a specific node, that is, it measures if a node exhibits higher or lower connectivity to other nodes. To characterize the DS network’s hierarchical organization, the number of communities that constitute the pre- and post-DS networks was estimated. Community detection was achieved through the implementation of a multi-iterative generalization of the Louvain community detection algorithm that optimizes modularity (Blondel, Guillaume, Lambiotte, & Lefebvre, 2008; Sporns & Betzel, 2016). Following the methodology of Akiki and Abdallah (2019), the detection was applied recursively to pinpoint the multiscale, hierarchical community organization. A single-level detection was performed for 100 iterations for every subject, and from them, a group-representing partition was selected using the partition similarity measure of normalized mutual information. This partition was then used as a base, to extract subgraphs from each community, which were then fed into the algorithm for the next level of community detections. This process was repeated until no new communities were detected. The measure quantifying a network’s small-worldness is sigma (σ). It is based on the ratio of the comparison of a network’s CC and CPL to that of random networks (Bolaños, Bernat, He, & Aviyente, 2013; Humphries & Gurney, Humphries, & Gurney, 2008) with the network characterized as SW when σ > 1. To estimate the SW measure, we generated 100 random networks from each subject’s pruned adjacency matrix while preserving the out-degree, with each edge being reshuffled at least 100 times, and calculated the σ in reference to each random network. Finally, we averaged the σ values per subject. These random networks were also used to create a personalized null distribution for each subject to mitigate the effect of density on graph measures when reporting individual results. For a summary of the graph measures used in the analysis, please refer to Supporting Information Table S1.

The comparison of our pre- and post-intervention network in relation to the exhibition of random or regular characteristics are discussed on a qualitative level, that is, whether they show characteristics exemplary for a random organization (small CPL, low CC). These random characteristics are present in many random topologies, and hence we have selected the most simplified random organization, the Erdös–Rényi. The degree distribution of the DS networks is taken into consideration when this comparison is quantified (i.e., estimation of SW), where a generative random model that preserves the degree distribution (Newman, 2002) of the DS networks is used.

Statistical analysis.

The pre- and postintervention somatometric and psychometric assessment scores were compared with the use of nonparametric Wilcoxon tests and paired t tests. The statistical comparisons were performed using the IBM SPSS 25.0 software. Wilcoxon tests were performed on the psychometric battery tests and somatometric tests with a discrete values score, while paired t tests were applied on the remaining somatometric assessment tests.

The Network-Based Statistics (NBS) (Zalesky et al., 2010) MATLAB toolbox was employed to estimate the statistically significant differences between the whole-head network of the pre- and postintervention connectivity networks of our subjects. A paired samples t test corrected for 5,000 random comparisons via the nonparametric NBS method (Zalesky et al., 2010) was performed. The permutation model of the NBS toolbox randomly exchanges the group label of each subject at every permutation level and then recalculates the statistic of interest applying the selected threshold. The maximal component size is derived from each permutation and utilized to create an empirical estimate of the null distribution of maximal component size (Zalesky et al., 2010). For further information on this process, please refer to the following work: Hayasaka and Nichols (2004) and Nichols and Holmes (2002). Significant differences between the two time points were visualized as weighted graphs through the BrainNet Viewer (Xia, Wang, & He, 2013) toolbox (Figure 1). DC was estimated from the outcome of this comparison, and the results were depicted in the same graph.

For the graph measures, analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was used, where each measure serves as the dependent variable with density as the covariate because density seriously affects the values of the other measures. For local measures (i.e., CC and BC), ANCOVA and false discovery rate correction were performed for each of the 863 nodes and p values for Type I errors, respectively. Results below the 5% threshold were considered significant.

Supporting information for this article is available at https://doi.org/10.1162/netn_a_00177.

Alexandra Anagnostopoulou: Formal analysis; Investigation; Methodology; Visualization; Writing – original draft; Writing – review & editing. Charis Styliadis: Formal analysis; Investigation; Methodology; Supervision; Visualization; Writing – original draft; Writing – review & editing. Panagiotis Kartsidis: Data curation; Formal analysis; Methodology; Validation; Writing – review & editing. Evangelia Romanopoulou: Data curation; Project administration; Resources; Writing – review & editing. Vasiliki Zilidou: Data curation; Project administration; Resources; Writing – review & editing. Chrysi Karali: Formal analysis; Investigation; Methodology; Visualization; Writing – original draft. Maria Karagianni: Data curation; Resources. Manousos Klados: Formal analysis; Methodology; Validation; Writing – review & editing. Evangelos Paraskevopoulos: Formal analysis; Methodology; Supervision; Visualization; Writing – review & editing. Panagiotis D. Bamidis: Conceptualization; Resources; Supervision; Writing – review & editing.

This study is an extension of the European CIP-ICTPSP. 2008.1.4 Long Lasting Memories (LLM) project (Project no. 238904) (http://www.longlastingmemories.eu/).

There are potential conflicts of interest (other, not financial, outside the scope of the submitted work) for the author P. Bamidis in respect of PositScience and the Aristotle University of Thessaloniki. There is a co-marketing agreement between PositScience and the Aristotle University of Thessaloniki to exploit Brain HQ within the LLM Care self-funded initiative that emerged as the non-for-profit business exploitation of the Long Lasting Memories (LLM Project) (www.longlastingmemories.eu) originally funded by the ICT-CIP-PSP Program of the European Commission. Brain HQ now forms part of LLM Care, a technology transfer/self-funded initiative that emerged as the non-for-profit business exploitation of LLM. Additionally, FitForAll (FFA) has been developed in the Aristotle University of Thessaloniki during the LLM Project (www.longlastingmemories.eu) originally funded by the ICT-CIP-PSP Program of the European Commission. It now forms part of LLM Care, a technology transfer/self-funded initiative that emerged as the non-for-profit business exploitation of LLM.

The authors would like to acknowledge the support (i.e., high-performance computing infrastructure and resources) provided by the IT Center of the Aristotle University of Thessaloniki (AUTH) throughout the progress of this research work. The authors would like to thank all the participants, their families, and caregivers, the Down Syndrome Association of Greece, Spring Children Mixed Living Center – Daily Care Activity & Training for Disabled People, The Down Syndrome Association of Serres, Center for the Rehabilitation of Social Support & Creative Employment of People with Disabilities, Child Care Center in Ptolemaida, Centre For Persons With Learning Difficulties – Zoodohos Pigi, Center of Creative Activities - Municipality of Neapoli and Sykies, Special laboratory for vocational education and training in Alexandria, Center for the Rehabilitation of Social Support & Creative Employment of People with disabilities “O Sotir,” Center of Creative Activities for People with Disabilities-Municipality of Neapoli and Sykes. Also, the authors would like to thank a list of people assisting in the trial facilitation, as follows: Dimitris Bamidis, Foteini Dolianiti, Ioanna Dratsiou, Sotiria Gilou, Maria Karagianni, Katerina Katsouli, Eleni Manthou, Maria Metaxa, Annita Varella (in alphabetical order), as well as, students from the Department of Psychology, the University of Sheffield International Faculty, City College (Greece), who were practicing under the supervision of Dr. Anna Vivas and Dr. Elissavet Chrysochoou who were also advising the authoring team on the psychological aspects of the whole endeavor. Last but not least, the authors would like to thank Emeritus Professor of Pediatrics and Genetics Dr. Charikleia Chatzisevastou-Loukidou for advising the team on all medical aspects of the project and for linking the team with many of the pilot sites.

Segregation:

Specialized processes occurring in densely connected brain regions.

Integration:

Combination of specialized information from distinct brain regions.

Directed functional connectivity:

Statistical dependencies between the time series of different brain areas that reveal their causal relation.

Phase transfer entropy:

The estimation of transfer entropy between phase timeseries, that is, it evaluates the influence of one signal’s phase on another signal’s phase.

Network-based statistic:

Nonparametric statistical method for controlling the family-wise error rate in multiple comparisons, hypothesis testing on graphs.

Sigma (σ):

The ratio of a network’s clustering coefficient and characteristic path length compared to that of random networks. When σ > 1 a network is SW.

Random network:

Network with low clustering coefficient and short characteristic path length.

Small-world network:

Network with a high clustering coefficient and short characteristic path length that achieves an optimal balance between integration and segregation.

Regular network:

Network with high clustering coefficient and large characteristic path length.

Modularity:

Dividing the network into subcommunities that maximize the within-group connections while minimizing the between-groups links.

Achard
,
S.
, &
Bullmore
,
E.
(
2007
).
Efficiency and cost of economical brain functional networks
.
PLoS Computational Biology
,
3
(
2
),
e17
.
Ahmadlou
,
M.
,
Gharib
,
S.
,
M.
Hemmati
,
Vameghi
,
R.
, &
Sajedi
,
F.
(
2013
).
Disrupted small-world brain network in children with Down syndrome
.
Clinical Neurophysiology
,
124
(
9
),
1755
1764
.
Akiki
,
T. J.
, &
Abdallah
,
C. G.
(
2019
).
Determining the hierarchical architecture of the human brain using subject-level clustering of functional networks
.
Scientific Reports
,
9
(
1
),
1
15
.
Anderson
,
J. S.
,
Nielsen
,
J. A.
,
Ferguson
,
M. A.
,
Burback
,
M. C.
,
Cox
,
E. T.
,
Dai
,
L.
, …
Korenberg
,
J. R.
(
2013
).
Abnormal brain synchrony in Down syndrome
.
NeuroImage: Clinical
,
2
,
703
715
.
Andrés
,
P.
,
Parmentier
,
F. B. R.
, &
Escera
,
C.
(
2006
).
The effect of age on involuntary capture of attention by irrelevant sounds: A test of the frontal hypothesis of aging
.
Neuropsychologia
,
44
(
12
),
2564
2568
.
Babiloni
,
C.
,
Albertini
,
G.
,
Onorati
,
P.
,
Muratori
,
C.
,
Buffo
,
P.
,
Condoluci
,
C.
, …
Rossini
,
P. M.
(
2010
).
Cortical sources of EEG rhythms are abnormal in Down syndrome
.
Clinical Neurophysiology
,
121
(
8
),
1205
1212
.
Babiloni
,
C.
,
Albertini
,
G.
,
Onorati
,
P.
,
Vecchio
,
F.
,
Buffo
,
P.
,
Sarà
,
M.
, …
Rossini
,
P. M.
(
2009
).
Inter-hemispheric functional coupling of eyes-closed resting EEG rhythms in adolescents with Down syndrome
.
Clinical Neurophysiology
,
120
(
9
),
1619
1627
.
Bamidis
,
P. D.
,
Fissler
,
P.
,
Papageorgiou
,
S. G.
,
Zilidou
,
V. I.
,
Konstantinidis
,
E. I.
,
Billis
,
A. S.
, …
Kolassa
,
I. T.
(
2015
).
Gains in cognition through combined cognitive and physical training: The role of training dosage and severity of neurocognitive disorder
.
Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience
,
7
(
152
),
152
.
Bamidis
,
P. D.
,
Konstantinidis
,
E. I.
,
Billis
,
A. S.
, &
Siountas
,
A.
(
2017
).
From e-homes to living labs: Founding and organising the Greek active and healthy ageing living lab (thess-AHALL) and its networked services
.
Hellenic Journal of Nuclear Medicine
,
20
(
2
),
112
126
.
Bamidis
,
P. D.
,
Vivas
,
A. B.
,
Styliadis
,
C.
,
Frantzidis
,
C. A.
,
Klados
,
M. A.
,
Schlee
,
W.
, …
Papageorgiou
,
S. G.
(
2014
).
A review of physical and cognitive interventions in aging
.
Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews
,
44
,
206
220
.
Barbey
,
A. K.
(
2018
).
Network neuroscience theory of human intelligence
.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences
,
22
(
1
),
8
20
.
Baron-Cohen
,
S.
,
Jolliffe
,
T.
,
Mortimore
,
C.
, &
Robertson
,
M.
(
1997
).
Another advanced test of theory of mind: Evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or Asperger syndrome
.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines
,
38
(
7
),
813
822
.
Baron-Cohen
,
S.
,
Wheelwright
,
S.
,
Hill
,
J.
,
Raste
,
Y.
, &
Plumb
,
I.
(
2001
).
The “reading the Mind in the eyes” Test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism
.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines
,
42
(
2
),
241
251
.
Baroncelli
,
L.
,
Braschi
,
C.
,
Spolidoro
,
M.
,
Begenisic
,
T.
,
Maffei
,
L.
, &
Sale
,
A.
(
2011
).
Brain plasticity and disease: A matter of inhibition
.
Neural Plasticity
,
11
.
Bassett
,
D. S.
, &
Bullmore
,
E.
(
2006
).
Small-world brain networks
.
The Neuroscientist
,
12
(
6
),
512
523
.
Bertapelli
,
F.
,
Pitetti
,
K.
,
Agiovlasitis
,
S.
, &
Guerra-Junior
,
G.
(
2016
).
Overweight and obesity in children and adolescents with Down syndrome—prevalence, determinants, consequences, and interventions: A literature review
.
Research in Developmental Disabilities
,
57
,
181
192
.
Billis
,
A. S.
,
Konstantinidis
,
E. I.
,
Mouzakidis
,
C.
,
Tsolaki
,
M. N.
,
Pappas
,
C.
, &
Bamidis
,
P. D.
(
2010
).
A game-like interface for training seniors’ dynamic balance and coordination
. In
XII Mediterranean Conference on Medical and Biological Engineering and Computing
(pp.
691
694
).
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
.
Blondel
,
V. D.
,
Guillaume
,
J. L.
,
Lambiotte
,
R.
, &
Lefebvre
,
E.
(
2008
).
Fast unfolding of communities in large networks
.
Journal of Statistical Mechanics: Theory and Experiment
,
2008
(
10
),
P10008
.
Bohannon
,
R. W.
(
1997
).
Comfortable and maximum walking speed of adults aged 20–79 years: Reference values and determinants
.
Age and Ageing
,
26
(
1
),
15
19
.
Bolaños
,
M.
,
Bernat
,
E. M.
,
He
,
B.
, &
Aviyente
,
S.
(
2013
).
A weighted small world network measure for assessing functional connectivity
.
Journal of Neuroscience Methods
,
212
(
1
),
133
142
.
Bullmore
,
E.
, &
Sporns
,
O.
(
2012
).
The economy of brain network organization
.
Nature Reviews Neuroscience
,
13
(
5
),
336
349
.
Colcombe
,
S. J.
,
Erickson
,
K. I.
,
Scalf
,
P. E.
,
Kim
,
J. S.
,
Prakash
,
R.
,
McAuley
,
E.
, …
Kramer
,
A. F.
(
2006
).
Aerobic exercise training increases brain volume in aging humans
.
The Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences
,
61
(
11
),
1166
1170
.
Colcombe
,
S. J.
,
Kramer
,
A. F.
,
Erickson
,
K. I.
,
Scalf
,
P. E.
,
McAuley
,
E.
,
Cohen
,
N. J.
, …
Elavsky
,
S.
(
2004
).
Cardiovascular fitness, cortical plasticity, and aging
.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
,
101
(
9
),
3316
3321
.
Costa
,
A. C. S.
(
2011
).
On the promise of pharmacotherapies targeted at cognitive and neurodegenerative components of down syndrome
.
Developmental Neuroscience
,
33
(
5
),
414
427
.
Cotman
,
C.
,
Berchtold
,
N. C.
, &
Christie
,
L. A.
(
2007
).
Exercise builds brain health: Key roles of growth factor cascades and inflammation
.
Trends in Neurosciences
,
30
(
9
),
464
472
.
Dierssen
,
M.
(
2012
).
Down syndrome: The brain in trisomic mode
.
Nature, Reviews Neuroscience
,
13
(
12
),
844
858
.
Dierssen
,
M.
,
Herault
,
Y.
, &
Estivill
,
X.
(
2009
).
Aneuploidy: From a physiological mechanism of variance to Down syndrome
.
Physiological Reviews
,
89
(
3
),
887
920
.
Edgin
,
J. O.
,
Clark
,
C. A. C.
,
Massand
,
E.
, &
Karmiloff-Smith
,
A.
(
2015
).
Building an adaptive brain across development: Targets for neurorehabilitation must begin in infancy
.
Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience
,
9
.
Elliott
,
D.
,
Weeks
,
D. J.
, &
Chua
,
R.
(
1994
).
Anomalous cerebral lateralization and Down syndrome
.
Brain and Cognition
,
26
(
2
),
191
195
.
Erickson
,
K. I.
,
Voss
,
M. W.
,
Prakash
,
R.
,
Basak
,
C.
,
Szabo
,
A.
,
Chaddock
,
L.
, …
Kramer
,
A. F.
(
2011
).
Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory
.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
,
108
(
7
),
3017
3022
.
Fonseca
,
L. M.
,
Navatta
,
A. C. R.
,
Bottino
,
C. M. C.
, &
Miotto
,
E. C.
(
2015
).
Cognitive rehabilitation of dementia in adults with Down syndrome: A review of non-pharmacological interventions
.
Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders Extra
,
5
(
3
),
330
340
.
Freeman
,
L. C.
(
1978
).
Centrality in social networks conceptual clarification
.
Social Networks
,
1
(
3
),
215
239
.
García-Alba
,
J.
,
Ramírez-Toraño
,
F.
,
Esteba-Castillo
,
S.
,
Bruña
,
R.
,
Moldenhauer
,
F.
,
Novell
,
R.
, …
Fernández
,
A.
(
2019
).
Neuropsychological and neurophysiological characterization of mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease in Down syndrome
.
Neurobiology of Aging
,
84
,
70
79
.
Guralnik
,
J. M.
,
Simonsick
,
E. M.
,
Ferrucci
,
L.
,
Glynn
,
R. J.
,
Berkman
,
L. F.
,
Blazer
,
D. G.
, …
Wallace
,
R. B.
(
1994
).
A short physical performance battery assessing lower extremity function: Association with self-reported disability and prediction of mortality and nursing home admission
.
Journal of Gerontology
,
49
(
2
),
M85
M94
.
Haier
,
R. J.
,
Head
,
K.
,
Head
,
E.
, &
Lott
,
I. T.
(
2008
).
Neuroimaging of individuals with Down’s syndrome at-risk for dementia: Evidence for possible compensatory events
.
NeuroImage
,
39
(
3
),
1324
1332
.
Hardee
,
J. P.
, &
Fetters
,
L.
(
2017
).
The effect of exercise intervention on daily life activities and social participation in individuals with down syndrome: A systematic review
.
Research in Developmental Disabilities
,
62
,
81
103
.
Hayasaka
,
S.
, &
Nichols
,
T. E.
(
2004
).
Combining voxel intensity and cluster extent with permutation test framework
.
NeuroImage
,
23
(
1
),
54
63
.
Head
,
E.
,
Lott
,
I. T.
,
Patterson
,
D.
,
Doran
,
E.
, &
Haier
,
R. J.
(
2007
).
Possible Compensatory events in adult Down syndrome brain prior to the development of Alzheimer disease neuropathology: Targets for nonpharmacological intervention
.
Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease
,
11
(
1
),
61
76
.
Hemmati
,
S.
,
Ahmadlou
,
M.
,
Gharib
,
M.
,
Vameghi
,
R.
, &
Sajedi
,
F.
(
2013
).
Down syndrome’s brain dynamics: Analysis of fractality in resting state
.
Cognitive Neurodynamics
,
7
(
4
),
333
340
.
Hilger
,
K.
,
Fukushima
,
M.
,
Sporns
,
O.
, &
Fiebach
,
C. J.
(
2019
).
Temporal stability of functional brain modules associated with human intelligence
.
Human Brain Mapping
,
41
(
2
),
362
372
.
Humphries
,
M. D.
, &
Gurney
,
K.
(
2008
).
Network “small-world- ness”: A quantitative method for determining canonical network equivalence
.
PLoS One
,
3
(
4
).
Ille
,
N.
,
Berg
,
P.
, &
Scherg
,
M.
(
2002
).
Artifact correction of the ongoing EEG using spatial filters based on artifact and brain signal topographies
.
Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology
,
19
(
2
),
113
124
.
John
,
J.
, &
Raven
,
J.
(
2003
).
Raven progressive matrices
.
Boston, MA
:
Springer US
.
Johnson
,
B. L.
, &
Nelson
,
J. K.
(
1979
).
Practical measurements for evaluation in physical education
.
Minneapolis, MN
:
Burgess Publishing
.
Jones
,
C. J.
, &
Rikli
,
R. E.
(
2002
).
Measuring functional fitness in older adults
.
The Journal of Active Ageing
,
1
(
1
),
24
30
.
Kálmán
,
J.
,
Juhász
,
A.
,
Laird
,
G.
,
Dickens
,
P.
,
Járdánházy
,
T.
,
Rimanóczy
,
A.
, …
Janka
,
Z.
(
2009
).
Serum interleukin-6 levels correlate with the severity of dementia in Down syndrome and in Alzheimer’s disease
.
Acta Neurologica Scandinavica
,
96
(
4
),
236
240
.
Kishnani
,
P. S.
,
Heller
,
J. H.
,
Spiridigliozzi
,
G. A.
,
Lott
,
I.
,
Escobar
,
L.
,
Richardson
,
S.
, …
McRae
,
T.
(
2010
).
Donepezil for treatment of cognitive dysfunction in children with Down syndrome aged 10–17
.
American Journal of Medical Genetics Part A
,
152
(
12
),
3028
3035
.
Klados
,
M. A.
,
Styliadis
,
C.
,
Frantzidis
,
C. A.
,
Paraskevopoulos
,
E.
, &
Bamidis
,
P. D.
(
2016
).
Beta-band functional connectivity is reorganized in mild cognitive impairment after combined computerized physical and cognitive training
.
Frontiers in Neuroscience
,
10
,
55
.
Konstantinidis
,
E. I.
,
Bamparopoulos
,
G.
, &
Bamidis
,
P. D.
(
2016
).
Moving real exergaming engines on the web: The webFitForAll case study in an active and healthy ageing living lab environment
.
IEEE Journal of Biomedical and Health Informatics
,
21
(
3
),
859
866
.
Konstantinidis
,
E. I.
,
Billis
,
A. S.
,
Bratsas
,
C.
,
Siountas
,
A.
&
Bamidis
,
P. D.
(
2016
).
Thessaloniki Active and Healthy Ageing Living Lab: The roadmap from a specific project to a living lab towards openness
. In
9th International Conference PErvasive Technologies Related to Assistive Environments (PETRA)
(pp.
1
4
).
Kramer
,
A. F.
,
Hahn
,
S.
,
Cohen
,
N. J.
,
Banich
,
M. T.
,
McAuley
,
E.
,
Harrison
,
C. R.
, …
Colcombe
,
A.
(
1999
).
Ageing, fitness and neurocognitive function
.
Nature
,
400
(
6743
),
418
419
.
Latora
,
V.
, &
Marchiori
,
M.
(
2001
).
Efficient behavior of small-world networks
.
Physical Review Letters
,
87
(
19
),
198701
.
Lee
,
T. W.
,
Girolami
,
M.
, &
Sejnowski
,
T. J.
(
1999
).
Independent component analysis using an extended infomax algorithm for mixed sub-Gaussian and super-Gaussian sources
.
Neural Computation
,
11
(
2
),
409
433
.
Lobier
,
M.
,
Siebenhühner
,
F.
,
Palva
,
S.
, &
Palva
,
J. M.
(
2014
).
Phase transfer entropy: A novel phase-based measure for directed connectivity in networks coupled by oscillatory interactions
.
NeuroImage
,
85
,
853
872
.
Lott
,
I. T.
,
Doran
,
E.
,
Nguyen
,
V. Q.
,
Tournay
,
A.
,
Head
,
E.
, &
Gillen
,
D. L.
(
2011
).
Down syndrome and dementia: A randomized, controlled trial of antioxidant supplementation
.
American Journal of Medical Genetics Part A
,
155
(
8
),
1939
1948
.
McGlinchey
,
E.
,
McCarron
,
M.
,
Holland
,
A.
, &
McCallion
,
P.
(
2019
).
Examining the effects of computerised cognitive training on levels of executive function in adults with Down syndrome
.
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
,
63
(
9
),
1137
1150
.
Menghini
,
D.
,
Costanzo
,
F.
, &
Vicari
,
S.
(
2011
).
Relationship between brain and cognitive processes in Down syndrome
.
Behavior Genetics
,
41
(
3
),
381
393
.
Mohan
,
M.
,
Carpenter
,
P. K.
, &
Bennett
,
C.
(
2009
).
Donepezil for dementia in people with Down syndrome
.
Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews
,
2009
(
1
).
Newman
,
M. E. J.
(
2002
).
Random graphs as models of networks
.
Handbook of Graphs and Networks
,
35
68
.
Newman
,
M. E. J.
(
2003
).
The structure and function of complex networks
.
SIAM Review
,
45
(
2
),
167
256
.
Nichols
,
T. E.
, &
Holmes
,
A.
(
2002
).
Nonparametric permutation tests for functional neuroimaging: A primer with examples
.
Human Brain Mapping
,
15
(
1
),
1
25
.
Oostenveld
,
R.
,
Fries
,
P.
,
Maris
,
E.
, &
Schoffelen
,
J. M.
(
2011
).
FieldTrip: Open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data
.
Computational Intelligence and Neuroscience
,
2011
,
156869
.
Paraskevopoulos
,
E.
,
Kraneburg
,
A.
,
Herholz
,
S. C.
,
Bamidis
,
P. D.
, &
Pantev
,
C.
(
2015
).
Musical expertise is related to altered functional connectivity during audiovisual integration
.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
,
112
(
40
),
2015
10662
.
Pascual-Marqui
,
R. D.
,
Michel
,
C. M.
, &
Lehmann
,
D.
(
1994
).
Low resolution electromagnetic tomography: A new method for localizing electrical activity in the brain
.
International Journal of Psychophysiology
,
18
(
1
),
49
65
.
Pinter
,
J. D.
,
Eliez
,
S.
,
Schmitt
,
J. E.
,
Capone
,
G. T.
, &
Reiss
,
A. L.
(
2001
).
Neuroanatomy of Down’s syndrome: A high-resolution MRI study
.
American Journal of Psychiatry
,
158
(
10
),
1659
1665
.
Pujol
,
J.
,
del Hoyo
,
L.
,
Blanco-Hinojo
,
L.
,
de Sola
,
S.
,
Macià
,
D.
,
Martínez-Vilavella
,
G.
, …
de la Torre
,
R.
(
2015
).
Anomalous brain functional connectivity contributing to poor adaptive behavior in down syndrome
.
Cortex
,
64
,
148
156
.
Romanopoulou
,
E. D.
,
Zilidou
,
V. I.
, &
Bamidis
,
P. D.
(
2017
).
Creating and sustaining a social health care ecosystem: The case of LLM care services in Greece
.
Hellenic Journal of Nuclear Medicine
,
20
(
2
),
40
48
.
Romanopoulou
,
E. D.
,
Zilidou
,
V. I.
,
Savvidis
,
T.
,
Chatzisevastou-Loukidou
,
C.
, &
Bamidis
,
P. D.
(
2018
).
Unmet needs of persons with Down syndrome: How assistive technology and game-based training may fill the gap
.
Studies in Health Technology and Informatics
,
251
,
15
18
.
Rubinov
,
M.
, &
Sporns
,
O.
(
2010
).
Complex network measures of brain connectivity: Uses and interpretations
.
NeuroImage
,
52
(
3
),
1059
1069
.
Ruiz-González
,
L.
,
Lucena-Antón
,
D.
,
Salazar
,
A.
,
Martín-Valero
,
R.
, &
Moral-Munoz
,
J. A.
(
2019
).
Physical therapy in Down syndrome: Systematic review and meta-analysis
.
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
,
63
(
8
),
1041
1067
.
Sanz
,
J. M.
,
Schoonheim
,
M. M.
,
Damoiseaux
,
J. S.
,
Rombouts
,
A. A.
,
Maris
,
E.
,
Barkhof
,
F.
, …
Stam
,
C. J.
(
2010
).
Loss of “small-world” networks in Alzheimer’s disease: Graph analysis of fMRI resting-state functional connectivity
.
PLoS One
,
5
(
11
).
Scott
,
D. W.
(
1992
).
Multivariate density estimation: Theory, practice, and visualization
.
Hoboken, NJ
:
Wiley
.
Shumway-Cook
,
A.
,
Brauer
,
S.
, &
Woollacott
,
M.
(
2000
).
Predicting the probability for falls in community-dwelling older adults using the Timed Up and Go Test
.
Physical Therapy
,
80
(
9
),
896
903
.
Smith
,
G. E.
,
Housen
,
P.
,
Yaffe
,
K.
,
Ruff
,
R.
,
Kennison
,
R. F.
,
Mahncke
,
H. W.
, …
Zelinski
,
E. M.
(
2009
).
A cognitive training program based on principles of brain plasticity: Results from the improvement in memory with plasticity-based adaptive cognitive training (IMPACT) study
.
Journal of the American Geriatrics Society
,
57
(
4
),
594
603
.
Sporns
,
O.
, &
Betzel
,
R. F.
(
2016
).
Modular brain networks
.
Annual Review of Psychology
,
67
(
1
),
613
640
.
Stam
,
C. J.
,
Jones
,
B. F.
,
Nolte
,
G.
,
Breakspear
,
M.
, &
Scheltens
,
P.
(
2007
).
Small-world networks and functional connectivity in Alzheimer’s disease
.
Cerebral Cortex
,
17
(
1
),
92
99
.
Styliadis
,
C.
,
Kartsidis
,
P.
, &
Paraskevopoulos
,
E.
(
2015
).
Neuroimaging approaches for elderly studies
. In
P. D.
Bamidis
,
I.
Tarnanas
,
L.
Hadjileontiadis
, &
M.
Tsolaki
(Eds.),
Handbook of research on innovations in the diagnosis and treatment of dementia
(pp.
47
86
).
Hershey, PA
:
IGI Global
.
Styliadis
,
C.
,
Kartsidis
,
P.
,
Paraskevopoulos
,
E.
,
Ioannides
,
A. A.
, &
Bamidis
,
P. D.
(
2015
).
Neuroplastic effects of combined computerized physical and cognitive training in elderly individuals at risk for dementia: An eLORETA controlled study on resting states
.
Neural Plasticity
,
2015
.
Takashima
,
M.
,
Takahashi
,
S.
,
Iwata
,
O.
,
Iwata
,
S.
,
Yokoo
,
M.
, &
Takashima
,
S.
(
2014
).
Neuronal plasticity in the developing and aging cerebral cortices of patients with Down syndrome
.
International Journal of Neurorehabilitation
,
01
(
02
).
Timme
,
N. M.
,
Ito
,
S.
,
Myroshnychenko
,
M.
,
Nigam
,
S.
,
Shimono
,
M.
,
Yeh
,
F. C.
, …
Beggs
,
J. M.
(
2016
).
High-degree neurons feed cortical computations
.
PLoS Computational Biology
,
125
(
5
),
e1004
858
.
Trojan
,
S.
, &
Pokorny
,
J.
(
1999
).
Theoretical aspects of neuroplasticity
.
Physiological Research
,
48
,
87
98
.
Vega
,
J. N.
,
Hohman
,
T. J.
,
Pryweller
,
J. R.
,
Dykens
,
E. M.
, &
Thornton-Wells
,
T. A.
(
2015
).
Resting-state functional connectivity in individuals with Down syndrome and Williams syndrome compared with typically developing controls
.
Brain Connectivity
,
5
(
8
),
461
475
.
Watts
,
D. J.
, &
Strogatz
,
S. H.
(
1998
).
Collective dynamics of ‘small-world’ networks
.
Nature
,
393
(
6684
),
440
442
.
Wells
,
K. F.
, &
Dillon
,
E. K.
(
1952
).
The sit and reach—a test of back and leg flexibility. Research Quarterly. American Association for Health
.
Physical Education and Recreation
,
23
(
1
),
115
118
.
White
,
N. S.
,
Alkire
,
M. T.
, &
Haier
,
R. J.
(
2003
).
A voxel-based morphometric study of nondemented adults with down syndrome
.
NeuroImage
,
20
(
1
),
393
403
.
Whitney
,
S. L.
,
Marchetti
,
G. F.
,
Morris
,
L. O.
, &
Sparto
,
P. J.
(
2007
).
The reliability and validity of the four square step test for people with balance deficits secondary to a vestibular disorder
.
Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation
,
88
(
1
),
99
104
.
Woolger
,
C.
(
2001
).
Wechsler intelligence scale for children-(WISC-III)
. In
Understanding Psychological Assessment
(pp.
219
233
).
Springer US
.
Xia
,
M.
,
Wang
,
J.
, &
He
,
Y.
(
2013
).
BrainNet Viewer: A network visualization tool for human brain connectomics
.
PloS One
,
8
(
7
),
e68910
.
Yeo
,
B. T. T.
,
Krienen
,
F. M.
,
Sepulcre
,
J.
,
Sabuncu
,
M. R.
,
Lashkari
,
D.
,
Hollinshead
,
M.
, …
Buckner
,
R. L.
(
2011
).
The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity
.
Journal of Neurophysiology
,
106
(
3
),
1125
1165
.
Zalesky
,
A.
,
Fornito
,
A.
, &
Bullmore
,
E. T.
(
2010
).
Network-based statistic: Identifying differences in brain networks
.
NeuroImage
,
53
(
4
),
1197
1207
.

Competing Interests

Competing Interests: See Conflict of Interest statement.

Author notes

Joint first authorship

Joint senior authorship

Handling Editor: Cornelis Jan Stam

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. For a full description of the license, please visit https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/legalcode.

Supplementary data