The emerging neuroscientific frontier of brain fingerprinting has recently established that human functional connectomes (FCs) exhibit fingerprint-like idiosyncratic features, which map onto heterogeneously distributed behavioral traits. Here, we harness brain-fingerprinting tools to extract FC features that predict subjective drug experience induced by the psychedelic psilocybin. Specifically, in neuroimaging data of healthy volunteers under the acute influence of psilocybin or a placebo, we show that, post psilocybin administration, FCs become more idiosyncratic owing to greater intersubject dissimilarity. Moreover, whereas in placebo subjects idiosyncratic features are primarily found in the frontoparietal network, in psilocybin subjects they concentrate in the default mode network (DMN). Crucially, isolating the latter revealed an FC pattern that predicts subjective psilocybin experience and is characterized by reduced within-DMN and DMN-limbic connectivity, as well as increased connectivity between the DMN and attentional systems. Overall, these results contribute to bridging the gap between psilocybin-mediated effects on brain and behavior, while demonstrating the value of a brain-fingerprinting approach to pharmacological neuroimaging.

The trending field of brain fingerprinting focuses on characterizing fingerprint-like idiosyncratic features of human functional connectomes (FCs), which have been shown to predict heterogeneously distributed behavioral traits. Here, we apply brain-fingerprinting methods to fMRI data from subjects who were administered the psychedelic psilocybin or a placebo. We find that, compared with the placebo condition, subjects under acute psilocybin effects exhibited more idiosyncratic FCs, with idiosyncratic features being largely concentrated in the default mode network (DMN). Furthermore, we isolated an idiosyncratic FC pattern that predicted reports of subjective psilocybin experiences. This pattern was characterized by altered DMN connectivity, specifically by reduced within-DMN and DMN-limbic connectivity, and increased connectivity between the DMN and attentional systems. This work paves the way for exciting new research harnessing pharmacological brain fingerprinting.

Psychedelic drugs such as lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD), dimethyltryptamine (DMT), and psilocybin are well known for their ability to transiently yet profoundly alter an individual’s perception of oneself and the external world, inducing what is called an altered state of consciousness (ASC; Studerus et al., 2010). Despite their long history of medicinal and ceremonial use, especially in indigenous contexts, psychedelics have only recently been rediscovered (R. L. Carhart-Harris & Goodwin, 2017) as putatively effective and safe (Johnson et al., 2018, 2019) treatments for a range of psychiatric disorders. Most notably, psilocybin-assisted psychotherapy has been reported to yield significant improvements of clinical symptoms in major depressive disorder (Daws et al., 2022; Rucker et al., 2016), obsessive compulsive disorder (Moreno et al., 2006), addiction (Bogenschutz et al., 2015; Johnson et al., 2017), and end-of-life distress (Griffiths et al., 2016; Ross et al., 2016).

There is strong evidence that the characteristic mind-altering effects of psychedelics are primarily mediated via agonism of the serotonin receptor 5-HT2AR (Glennon et al., 1992; Stenbæk et al., 2021; Vollenweider et al., 1998). Nevertheless, how 5-HT2AR agonism affects whole-brain dynamics to induce the experienced ASC is still a subject of ongoing research. Major contributions to this end have been made by network neuroscience–based approaches, examining psychedelic-induced changes of the brain’s functional connectome (FC) architecture (R. L. Carhart-Harris et al., 2013). The FC is a statistical model representing the brain as a network composed of brain regions (nodes) and the connections thereof (edges), where the connection between two brain regions typically encodes the Pearson correlation of their neurophysiological time series (Bullmore & Sporns, 2009). In functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), a salient hallmark of the human FC is the strongly correlated hemodynamic activity within distinct groups of brain regions, giving rise to the so-called resting-state networks (RSNs; Yeo et al., 2011). Regions of the same RSN are believed to collaborate in serving specific functions that require increasingly complex information processing, ranging from unimodal sensory regions of the somatomotor (SM) and visual (Vis) networks to heteromodal association cortices of the frontoparietal (FPN) and default mode (DMN) networks (Luppi et al., 2022; Margulies et al., 2016).

In this context, it has been proposed that the primary action of psychedelics, whereby the subjective effects are elicited, is to reconfigure the brain’s functional hierarchy by modulating the activity of higher level cortical networks (R. L. Carhart-Harris & Friston, 2019; Corlett et al., 2019; Doss et al., 2022), which are known to exhibit exceptionally high 5-HT2AR densities (Beliveau et al., 2017). Indeed, reduced activity and functional connectivity within these higher level systems, in particular the DMN, are among the most frequently reported neural correlates of the acute psychedelic state and have been shown to correlate with the intensity of subjective effects (R. L. Carhart-Harris et al., 2012, 2016; Madsen et al., 2021; Muthukumaraswamy et al., 2013; Smigielski et al., 2019). Concomitantly, another consistently observed acute neural response to psychedelic drugs is an increase in functional connectivity between lower level systems (Preller et al., 2018, 2020).

While network-based analyses of neuroimaging data have advanced our understanding of the group-level effect of psychedelics on the functional network organization of the brain, it is less clear how each individual’s neural response to a psychedelic compound maps onto the various aspects of their unique subjective drug experience. In fact, the subjective effects of psychedelics are multifaceted and highly variable, both within and between subjects (Moujaes et al., 2022). Crucially, several lines of evidence suggest that the long-term benefits of psilocybin are influenced by the subjective acute drug experience (Bogenschutz et al., 2015; Griffiths et al., 2016; Johnson et al., 2017; Roseman et al., 2018; Ross et al., 2016). This emphasizes the need for further research investigating how an individual’s unique psychedelic experience emerges from the underlying FC reconfigurations in that person’s brain.

Mapping FC changes onto subjective drug-experience reports is a nontrivial task, and one major reason for this is the high dimensionality of the FC, comprising the edge weights of all unique pairs of brain regions, in contrast to the typically low sample sizes of neuroimaging studies. Intriguingly, seminal work from Finn et al. (2015) revealed that the weights of specific FC edges are idiosyncratic; that is, they are both unique to each subject and reliable across test-retest fMRI scanning sessions, just like the pattern of furrows and ridges that defines an individual’s fingerprint. Importantly, particular idiosyncratic edges that facilitate the identification of subjects were also shown to be most predictive of behavioral traits (Finn et al., 2015). These findings have inspired a series of other studies focusing on idiosyncratic rather than generalizable FC patterns, giving rise to the trending field of brain fingerprinting (Amico & Goñi, 2018; Mallaroni et al., 2022; Stampacchia et al., 2023; Van De Ville et al., 2021).

In this work, we propose that neuroimaging studies investigating the effects of pharmacological substances on brain activity stand to gain significantly from a pharmacological brain-fingerprinting approach that integrates brain-fingerprinting methods. We expect this approach to be especially illuminating if individual responses to the drug in question are heterogeneous. Notably, the acute subjective experience of psychedelic compounds is highly variable across individuals (Moujaes et al., 2022) and has been associated with long-term drug effects (Nichols, 2020). This renders pharmacological brain fingerprinting a particularly promising approach for studying individual drug responses to psychedelics using neuroimaging data, with great potential to inform precision medicine.

In this vein, a pioneering pharmacological fingerprinting study reported that, under the influence of the psychedelic compound ayahuasca, FC fingerprints are altered such that they predict aspects of individual psychedelic experience (Mallaroni et al., 2022). However, to the best of our knowledge, no study to date has investigated the acute effect of psilocybin on FC fingerprints and its relevance to subjective psilocybin experience.

Addressing these knowledge gaps, this study applies brain-fingerprinting tools to 7T fMRI data of 46 healthy volunteers who were administered 0.17 mg/kg psilocybin (n = 21) or a placebo (n = 25), in order to (a) compare FC fingerprints between psilocybin and placebo conditions, and (b) examine whether idiosyncratic FC patterns allow for more accurate predictions of subjective psilocybin experience with respect to FC patterns composed of randomly selected features. Our measure of subjective psilocybin experience captured 11 aspects of the unique psilocybin-induced ASC of each subject; these aspects were assessed retrospectively using the well-established 5D-ASC questionnaire (Dittrich, 1998; Studerus et al., 2010).

Interestingly, we find that in the psilocybin group FCs are significantly more dissimilar and thus allow for better subject identification than in the placebo group. Furthermore, we present a simple heuristic for estimating the idiosyncrasy of single FC edges, revealing that in placebo idiosyncratic edges are primarily located in FPN regions such as the lateral prefrontal cortex (PFC), whereas in psilocybin they concentrate in DMN regions such as medial prefrontal (mPFC), posterior cingulate (PCC), and inferior parietal (IPC) cortices. Finally, by selecting highly idiosyncratic edges we uncovered an FC pattern that significantly predicts subjective psilocybin experience, characterized by decreased within-DMN and DMN-limbic connectivity, and increased connectivity between DMN and attentional networks.

Introducing Drug Brain Fingerprinting

Step 1 of our pharmacological brain-fingerprinting approach is the derivation of two FCs per subject from the first and second temporal halves of the regional fMRI blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) time series (Figure 1A). These two FCs, which we will respectively refer to as first-half and second-half FC, are used to assess the reliability of FC features. Reliability in our analysis should therefore be interpreted in terms of the temporal stability of an individual’s FC pattern. Although related, this differs from the notion of test-retest reliability where repeated measurements are taken during separate acquisitions, so as to account for session-to-session variability due to, for instance, methodological differences.

Figure 1.

Pharmacological brain-fingerprinting workflow. (A) The first-half and second-half functional connectomes (FCs) of each subject are computed, respectively, from the first and second half of the subject’s regional fMRI BOLD time series such that the resulting symmetric square matrices encode the Pearson correlation of the BOLD signals of each unique pair of brain regions during the first half of the scan (first-half FC) and the second half of the scan (second-half FC). (B) The identifiability matrix displays the Pearson correlation between the second-half (rows) and first-half (columns) FCs of all subjects, where rows and columns are sorted such that subjects of the same condition are clustered together. Each diagonal element of the identifiability matrix encodes the Iself of one subject. The Iothers of each subject are computed by averaging over all off-diagonal elements that correspond to first-half versus second-half FC correlations between that subject and every other subject of the same condition. Finally, Idiff, which is defined as the difference between Iself and Iothers, indicates the within-group identifiability of a subject based on their FC. (C) For each condition, we computed the intraclass correlation (ICC) of each FC edge as a measure of idiosyncrasy. ICC matrices are displayed as binarized, thresholded (ICC > 0.6) matrices for better visualization. The idiosyncrasy of each brain region was assessed using ICC strength, defined as the sum of non-thresholded ICC values of all edges of that brain region.

Figure 1.

Pharmacological brain-fingerprinting workflow. (A) The first-half and second-half functional connectomes (FCs) of each subject are computed, respectively, from the first and second half of the subject’s regional fMRI BOLD time series such that the resulting symmetric square matrices encode the Pearson correlation of the BOLD signals of each unique pair of brain regions during the first half of the scan (first-half FC) and the second half of the scan (second-half FC). (B) The identifiability matrix displays the Pearson correlation between the second-half (rows) and first-half (columns) FCs of all subjects, where rows and columns are sorted such that subjects of the same condition are clustered together. Each diagonal element of the identifiability matrix encodes the Iself of one subject. The Iothers of each subject are computed by averaging over all off-diagonal elements that correspond to first-half versus second-half FC correlations between that subject and every other subject of the same condition. Finally, Idiff, which is defined as the difference between Iself and Iothers, indicates the within-group identifiability of a subject based on their FC. (C) For each condition, we computed the intraclass correlation (ICC) of each FC edge as a measure of idiosyncrasy. ICC matrices are displayed as binarized, thresholded (ICC > 0.6) matrices for better visualization. The idiosyncrasy of each brain region was assessed using ICC strength, defined as the sum of non-thresholded ICC values of all edges of that brain region.

Close modal

Furthermore, we adopt two different toolboxes from brain fingerprinting for (a) measuring the subject identifiability based on a given set of FC edges (Figure 1B), and (b) mapping edgewise and brain-regional distributions of idiosyncrasy (Figure 1C).

Subject identifiability estimates how well a subject can be identified within a group of other subjects based on a selection of FC edges. It can be measured using Idiff, which is the difference between Iself and Iothers (Amico & Goñi, 2018), where Iself and Iothers respectively capture the reliability and uniqueness of the edge selection in terms of within- and between-subject FC similarity (Pearson correlation) (Figure 1B).

Idiosyncrasy intuitively describes the property of being characteristic of an individual. Consistent with this intuition, we refer to edges that promote the identification of subjects as idiosyncratic. For each condition, we measure the edgewise idiosyncrasy using intraclass correlation (ICC), and brain-regional idiosyncrasy using ICC strength, which we define to be the total ICC of all edges corresponding to a given brain region (Figure 1C).

The identifiability metrics of the full FCs of 21 psilocybin and 25 placebo subjects were, respectively, derived from the upper left and lower right quadrants of the identifiability matrix shown in Figure 2A. Interestingly, while there was no significant group difference in Iself (df = 43, t stat. = −0.20, p = 0.8460), psilocybin subjects tended to have lower Iothers (df = 43, t stat. = −3.33, p = 0.0018) and higher Idiff (df = 43, t stat. = 2.10, p = 0.0414) than placebo subjects, suggesting that there were stable and substantial interindividual differences in the neural response to psilocybin such that subject identification was facilitated. All reported differences were corrected for head motion as a potential confounder, which was slightly but significantly higher in the psilocybin group (df = 44, t stat. = 2.06, p = 0.0456; see the Methods section).

Figure 2.

Improved identifiability in the psilocybin group due to greater FC dissimilarity. (A) Identifiability matrix as described in Figure 1B. (B) Distributions of Iself, Iothers, and Idiff in psilocybin (psi) and placebo (pla) conditions. The boxes display the mean, standard deviation, and 95% confidence interval of each distribution with the jittered raw data plotted on top. Significant group effects according to an analysis of variance (ANOVA) for predicting the measure from group and motion (see the Methods section) are labeled with asterisks (* p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01).

Figure 2.

Improved identifiability in the psilocybin group due to greater FC dissimilarity. (A) Identifiability matrix as described in Figure 1B. (B) Distributions of Iself, Iothers, and Idiff in psilocybin (psi) and placebo (pla) conditions. The boxes display the mean, standard deviation, and 95% confidence interval of each distribution with the jittered raw data plotted on top. Significant group effects according to an analysis of variance (ANOVA) for predicting the measure from group and motion (see the Methods section) are labeled with asterisks (* p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01).

Close modal

Edgewise and Brain-Regional Distributions of Idiosyncrasy

For an initial visual assessment of the spatial distribution of idiosyncrasy, we plotted the ICC of each edge (thresholded at 0.6 for better visualization) within the psilocybin and placebo group as symmetric N × N matrices, where N = 200 is the number of brain regions (Figure 3A). We will hereafter denote the non-thresholded psilocybin and placebo ICC matrices as ICCpsi and ICCpla, respectively. Figure 3B displays the difference between ICCpsi and ICCpla averaged across RSN connections. As can be seen, Vis and DMN edges tended to be more idiosyncratic in psilocybin compared with placebo, whereas the opposite trend was observed for FPN edges.

Figure 3.

Shifted spatial distributions of idiosyncrasy in psilocybin versus placebo. (A) Thresholded (>0.6) edgewise intraclass correlation (ICC) matrices of psilocybin (red) and placebo (blue) groups. (B) For each unique pair of cortical resting-state networks (RSNs), we compared the ICCs of all edges connecting the two RSNs between psilocybin and placebo conditions. The heatmap displays the resulting mean ICC differences such that positive (red) values correspond to a higher average ICC in the psilocybin group, and vice versa. False discovery rate (FDR)-corrected significant p values according to a paired t test are highlighted in the lower triangle of the symmetric heatmap (* p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001). (C) Group differences in ICC strength within each RSN. Each box displays the mean, standard deviation, and 95% confidence interval of the psilocybin-placebo differences in ICC strength of all brain regions within one RSN. Distributions with means that are significantly different from 0 according to a paired t test and post FDR correction are labeled with asterisks (* p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001). (D) Brain renderings of z-scored ICC strength in psilocybin and placebo groups, and z-scored psilocybin minus placebo ICC strength differences. (E) Group-average and corresponding standard error of Idiff, Iself, and Iothers in psilocybin (red) and placebo (blue) computed for the n = {50, 100, …, 19,900} top ICCpsi-ranked edges. Note that selecting edges based on high ICCpsi results in an optimization of Idiff in the psilocybin, but not the placebo group, suggesting a functional reconfiguration of the functional connectome fingerprint upon psilocybin administration. Vis = visual network; SM = somatomotor network; DA = dorsal attention network; VA = ventral attention network; LIM = limbic system; FPN = frontoparietal network; DMN = default mode network.

Figure 3.

Shifted spatial distributions of idiosyncrasy in psilocybin versus placebo. (A) Thresholded (>0.6) edgewise intraclass correlation (ICC) matrices of psilocybin (red) and placebo (blue) groups. (B) For each unique pair of cortical resting-state networks (RSNs), we compared the ICCs of all edges connecting the two RSNs between psilocybin and placebo conditions. The heatmap displays the resulting mean ICC differences such that positive (red) values correspond to a higher average ICC in the psilocybin group, and vice versa. False discovery rate (FDR)-corrected significant p values according to a paired t test are highlighted in the lower triangle of the symmetric heatmap (* p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001). (C) Group differences in ICC strength within each RSN. Each box displays the mean, standard deviation, and 95% confidence interval of the psilocybin-placebo differences in ICC strength of all brain regions within one RSN. Distributions with means that are significantly different from 0 according to a paired t test and post FDR correction are labeled with asterisks (* p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001). (D) Brain renderings of z-scored ICC strength in psilocybin and placebo groups, and z-scored psilocybin minus placebo ICC strength differences. (E) Group-average and corresponding standard error of Idiff, Iself, and Iothers in psilocybin (red) and placebo (blue) computed for the n = {50, 100, …, 19,900} top ICCpsi-ranked edges. Note that selecting edges based on high ICCpsi results in an optimization of Idiff in the psilocybin, but not the placebo group, suggesting a functional reconfiguration of the functional connectome fingerprint upon psilocybin administration. Vis = visual network; SM = somatomotor network; DA = dorsal attention network; VA = ventral attention network; LIM = limbic system; FPN = frontoparietal network; DMN = default mode network.

Close modal

Concurrently, we found that in the psilocybin group ICC strength was significantly higher in Vis (df = 45, t stat. = 2.77, p = 0.0099, fdr = 0.0351) and DMN (df = 45, t stat. = 5.25, p = 0.0000, fdr = 0.0000) regions and lower in FPN (df = 45, t stat. = −2.07, p = 0.0472, fdr = 0.1000) regions compared with placebo, although the FPN effect did not survive false discovery rate (FDR) correction for seven comparisons (Figure 3C). Moreover, brain renderings of the z-scored ICC strength in placebo, psilocybin, and their difference reveal a systematic reconfiguration of the brain fingerprint: Whereas in placebo idiosyncratic edges accumulate in FPN-associated right lateral prefrontal regions, in psilocybin they are primarily localized in DMN-associated regions including the mPFC, PCC, and IPC (Figure 3D).

Concurrently, we demonstrate that selecting idiosyncratic edges based on ICCpsi results in an optimization of the Idiff of psilocybin but not placebo subjects (Figure 3E), and the vice versa for edge selection based on high ICCpla (Supplementary Figure 1 in the Supporting Information). More specifically, in an iterative process we computed Idiff, Iself, and Iothers for all subjects considering only the top n edges with the highest ICCpsi with n = {50, 100, …, 19,900}. That is, the final step included all edges. As expected, the group-average Idiff of psilocybin subjects peaks at a low number of included edges (n = 100) before decreasing gradually with increasing n, as Iself declines and Iothers slightly inclines. Remarkably, however, the group-average Idiff of placebo subjects remains virtually flat across all values of n, which is despite higher Iself and due to higher Iothers in edges with high ICCpsi. This implies that temporally stable edges, which tend to be similar across individuals under normal (placebo) conditions, are differentially altered by psilocybin such that they follow a more heterogeneous distribution in psilocybin subjects.

Idiosyncrasy-Informed Edge Selection Improves Predictions of Subjective Psilocybin Experience

Our previous results prompted the question whether idiosyncrasy (ICCpsi) can be used as a selection criterion for edges that predict subjective psilocybin experience. To this end, we first ran principal component analysis (PCA) on the behavioral data describing the individual drug experience of each psilocybin subject in 11 dimensions, which was acquired using the 5D-ASC questionnaire (Dittrich, 1998; Studerus et al., 2010) (Figure 4A). This enabled us to extract the maximally heterogeneous components in the behavioral data and reduce the dimensionality of our prediction target. The resulting first principal component (PC) explained 45.7% of the variance and the four most important behavioral aspects were “insightfulness,” “experience of unity,” “blissful state,” and “changed meaning of percepts” (Figure 4B). Notably, all PC coefficients were positive. Thus, the subject scores of this PC, henceforth denoted as b, may be interpreted as capturing the overall intensity of the drug experience.

Figure 4.

Idiosyncrasy-informed feature selection improves predictions of subjective drug experience. (A) Schematic workflow for constructing linear models to predict subjective psilocybin experience from functional connectome (FC) edges. First, principal component analysis (PCA) was applied to the behavioral data, containing measurements of 11 aspects of subjective psilocybin experience from each subject. The resulting subject scores b of the first principal component (PC1) were used as the response variable. Subsequently, additional PCAs were run on a set of n FC edges from all subjects. Edges were either selected randomly or based on high intraclass correlation (ICC), and the resulting subject scores (x, y, z) of the first three principal components (PC1–3) were used as predictor variables. (B) Coefficients of PC1 of the behavioral data PCA (11 dimensions of altered states of consciousness; Dittrich, 1998; Studerus et al., 2010): UN = experience of unity; SP = spiritual experience; BS = blissful state; IS = insightfulness; DB = disembodiment; IC = impaired control and cognition; AX = anxiety; CI = complex imagery; EI = elementary imagery; SY = audiovisual synesthesia; CM = changed meaning of percepts). (C) R2 value of linear models bx + y + z based on the n top ICC-ranked edges (red line) and average R2 value with corresponding standard error of 100 linear models based on n random selections of edges (gray line). Significant (p < 0.05; uncorrected) ICC-ranked models are marked with red triangles. The *** indicate that the ICC-ranked model at n = 2,500 explains significantly (p = 1.00e-6, FDR-corrected across the 79 steps in n) more variance in b than the models based on random edge selections. (D) R2 of linear models bx (blue), by (teal), bz (yellow) based on the n top ICC-ranked edges. Filled and empty triangles highlight significant (p < 0.05; uncorrected) positive and negative correlations, respectively. (E) Correlation of b with y of the most significant idiosyncrasy-informed model by at n = 1,000 edges.

Figure 4.

Idiosyncrasy-informed feature selection improves predictions of subjective drug experience. (A) Schematic workflow for constructing linear models to predict subjective psilocybin experience from functional connectome (FC) edges. First, principal component analysis (PCA) was applied to the behavioral data, containing measurements of 11 aspects of subjective psilocybin experience from each subject. The resulting subject scores b of the first principal component (PC1) were used as the response variable. Subsequently, additional PCAs were run on a set of n FC edges from all subjects. Edges were either selected randomly or based on high intraclass correlation (ICC), and the resulting subject scores (x, y, z) of the first three principal components (PC1–3) were used as predictor variables. (B) Coefficients of PC1 of the behavioral data PCA (11 dimensions of altered states of consciousness; Dittrich, 1998; Studerus et al., 2010): UN = experience of unity; SP = spiritual experience; BS = blissful state; IS = insightfulness; DB = disembodiment; IC = impaired control and cognition; AX = anxiety; CI = complex imagery; EI = elementary imagery; SY = audiovisual synesthesia; CM = changed meaning of percepts). (C) R2 value of linear models bx + y + z based on the n top ICC-ranked edges (red line) and average R2 value with corresponding standard error of 100 linear models based on n random selections of edges (gray line). Significant (p < 0.05; uncorrected) ICC-ranked models are marked with red triangles. The *** indicate that the ICC-ranked model at n = 2,500 explains significantly (p = 1.00e-6, FDR-corrected across the 79 steps in n) more variance in b than the models based on random edge selections. (D) R2 of linear models bx (blue), by (teal), bz (yellow) based on the n top ICC-ranked edges. Filled and empty triangles highlight significant (p < 0.05; uncorrected) positive and negative correlations, respectively. (E) Correlation of b with y of the most significant idiosyncrasy-informed model by at n = 1,000 edges.

Close modal

Following the behavioral PCA, we applied additional PCAs to different selections of n edges from the FCs that were derived from the full-length BOLD time series of each psilocybin subject. Specifically, PCAs were run on (a) the n top ICCpsi-ranked edges, and (b) 100 random selections of n edges from all psilocybin subjects, where n = {250, 500, …, 19,900} (Figure 4A). Finally, for each edge selection and each n we built a linear model to predict b from the subject scores x, y, z, corresponding to the multidimensional FC patterns that are represented by the first three PCs (PC1, 2, 3) of the FC-edge PCA, respectively. More details on this method and the rationale behind it are provided in the Methods section. Remarkably, the R2 value of idiosyncrasy-informed models was consistently higher than the average R2 value of 100 models based on random edge selections for all n smaller than a threshold at which the two model types inevitably converged (Figure 4C). Moreover, a two-sided, one-sample t test comparing the R2 values of the models based on idiosyncratic versus randomly selected edges at n = 2,500, where the R2 value of idiosyncrasy-informed models was found to be maximal, yielded a highly significant result (p = 1.00e−6, FDR-corrected for 79 steps in n).

In contrast to randomly selected edges, idiosyncratic edges tend to connect brain regions that are spatially close to each other and, concomitantly, more likely to have correlated BOLD time series (see Figure 3D). Thus, to ensure that the predictive advantage of idiosyncrasy-informed models was not due to the spatial contiguity of edges, we repeated the analysis from Figure 4C with spatial-contiguity-preserving surrogate models. More specifically, at each step n we built 100 linear models that were based on n edges connecting the same brain regions as the n most idiosyncratic edges, but the assignments of BOLD time series to each brain region were shuffled by randomly rotating spherical projections of these assignments, using software provided by Váša and Mišić (2022). Notably, a two-sided, one-sample t test comparing the R2 values of the two model types at n = 2,500 revealed that idiosyncrasy-informed models continue to outperform surrogate models in predicting subjective psilocybin experience even when controlling for spatial contiguity (p = 1.00e−6, FDR-corrected for 79 steps in n; Supplementary Figure 2).

Interestingly, the majority of explained variance in b of the idiosyncrasy-informed models was always due to either y or z, but not x, suggesting that x captured a heterogeneously distributed FC pattern that was not relevant to subjective psilocybin experience such as potentially scanning-session differences (Figure 4C). Thus, we explored whether the inclusion of x as a noisy predictor might explain why we only found three slightly significant idiosyncrasy-informed linear models, namely at n = {2,250, 2,500, 2,750} (p = 0.0499; p = 0.0450; p = 0.0480, respectively; Figure 4C). Indeed, repeating the analysis with idiosyncrasy-informed single-predictor models (i.e., bx; by; bz) revealed that b is best explained by y at n = 1,000 (p = 0.0082; R2 = 0.31) (Figures 4DE). Thus, we will henceforth focus on the FC pattern described by PC2 corresponding to y at n = 1,000, which accounts for 9.91% of the variance in the 1,000 most idiosyncratic edges. Notably, this PC2 is strongly inversely related to the FC pattern that corresponds to PC3 and z at n = 6,500, where z most significantly predicts b (p = 0.0169; R2 = 0.27) (Supplementary Figure 3).

Characterizing FC Patterns of Subjective Psilocybin Experience

The PC2 of the 1,000 most idiosyncratic edges of the psilocybin group is characterized by strong, positive loadings on edges of the PCC and mPFC and strong negative loadings on edges of right anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) (Figure 5A).

Figure 5.

Characterization of idiosyncratic functional connectome (FC) pattern that predicts subjective psilocybin experience. (A) Coefficients of the PC2 corresponding to the best intraclass correlation (ICC)-based model by at n = 1,000. Coefficients are summed across all edges (within the subset of n) of each brain region to obtain brain-regional values. (B) Mean resting-state network (RSN) functional connectivity of psilocybin subjects with positive y score (rFC y+) and negative y score (rFC y−), as well as of placebo subjects. Means are computed over the 1,000 FC edges with highest ICCpsi. For all psilocybin subjects, these edges are not directly derived from the FC but are instead reconstructed from the edge PC2 and the corresponding subject scores y. Finally, the means displayed by rFC y+ and rFC y− are weighted according to the absolute y value of the subjects. (C) Correlations of b (i.e., subjective psilocybin experience) with DMN functional connectivity in psilocybin subjects. DMN functional connectivity values are averages over the 1,000 edges with the highest ICC in psilocybin, which were directly derived from the FC of each subject. DMN-attentional connectivity was computed as the average over DMN-VA and DMN-DA edges, and DMN-sensory connectivity corresponds to average DMN-Vis and DMN-SM connectivity. Vis = visual network; SM = somatomotor network; DA = dorsal attention network; VA = ventral attention network; LIM = limbic system; FPN = frontoparietal network; DMN = default mode network.

Figure 5.

Characterization of idiosyncratic functional connectome (FC) pattern that predicts subjective psilocybin experience. (A) Coefficients of the PC2 corresponding to the best intraclass correlation (ICC)-based model by at n = 1,000. Coefficients are summed across all edges (within the subset of n) of each brain region to obtain brain-regional values. (B) Mean resting-state network (RSN) functional connectivity of psilocybin subjects with positive y score (rFC y+) and negative y score (rFC y−), as well as of placebo subjects. Means are computed over the 1,000 FC edges with highest ICCpsi. For all psilocybin subjects, these edges are not directly derived from the FC but are instead reconstructed from the edge PC2 and the corresponding subject scores y. Finally, the means displayed by rFC y+ and rFC y− are weighted according to the absolute y value of the subjects. (C) Correlations of b (i.e., subjective psilocybin experience) with DMN functional connectivity in psilocybin subjects. DMN functional connectivity values are averages over the 1,000 edges with the highest ICC in psilocybin, which were directly derived from the FC of each subject. DMN-attentional connectivity was computed as the average over DMN-VA and DMN-DA edges, and DMN-sensory connectivity corresponds to average DMN-Vis and DMN-SM connectivity. Vis = visual network; SM = somatomotor network; DA = dorsal attention network; VA = ventral attention network; LIM = limbic system; FPN = frontoparietal network; DMN = default mode network.

Close modal

To better visualize the FC pattern captured by PC2, we reconstructed the functional connectivity values of the 1,000 most idiosyncratic edges for each psilocybin subject from PC2 and y, and averaged the reconstructed FC edges across RSN connections. This resulted in one 7 × 7 RSN FC matrix per subject; these matrices were subsequently averaged across subjects with positive y (rFC y+), and across subjects with negative y (rFC y−). Averages were weighted according to the absolute value of y (Figure 5B). This analysis intended to crystallize the FC patterns that are representative for the “archetype” psilocybin subjects with particularly intense (rFC y+) or mild (rFC y−) psilocybin experiences (b), which, according to our best linear model by, are associated with positive and negative values for y, respectively (see Figure 4E). Finally, we also computed the average RSN FC of placebo subjects based on the same (non-reconstructed) 1,000 edges with maximal ICCpsi.

Intriguingly, rFC y− was strikingly similar to the placebo pendant, whereas rFC y+ was distinct (Figure 5B). Notably, this trend was particularly pronounced with respect to DMN connectivity: While the DMN connectivity of rFC y− was nearly identical to placebo, rFC y+ appeared to have reduced within-DMN and DMN-LIM connectivity and increased connectivity between DMN and attentional (DA, VA), and sensory (Vis, SM) networks. The similarity of rFC y− but not FC y+ with the placebo counterpart is particularly interesting given the interpretation of b in terms of the intensity of subjective psilocybin experience (Figure 4B). In other words, psilocybin subjects with negative y are predicted to have less intensely felt psilocybin experiences, and thus it seems unsurprising that their isolated FC pattern resembles that of placebo subjects.

Corroborating the above suppositions, we find that, with the exception of average DMN-sensory connectivity (r = 0.25, p = 0.2691, fdr = 1.0000), average DMN-DMN (r = −0.46, p = 0.0345, fdr = 0.0498), DMN-LIM (r = −0.47, p = 0.0320, fdr = 0.0498), and DMN-attentional (r = −0.53, p = 0.0139, fdr = 0.0498) connectivity are significantly correlated to b (Figure 5C). (Note: DMN-sensory connectivity refers to the average across DMN-Vis and DMN-SM edges. Similarly, DMN-attentional connectivity is average DMN-VA and DMN-DA connectivity.) All significant correlations survive FDR correction. Importantly, if edges are not preselected based on idiosyncrasy and the above correlations are instead computed across all FC edges, only DMN-LIM connectivity is significantly correlated to b after FDR correction (Supplementary Figure 4). This further supports the argument that idiosyncrasy represents a useful edge-selection criterion for improving predictions of subjective psilocybin experience, and possibly behavior in general.

Human neuroimaging studies have traditionally focused on characterizing populational-level blueprints of brain activity and structure by averaging over data from many subjects (Finn et al., 2015). While this approach has led to important insights into the general principles of neuroscience, there is no doubt that substantial variability exists not only between but also within populations. Notably, a stronger research focus on idiosyncratic rather than generalizable features may be key to the advancement of personalized medical approaches. Arguably, it is especially important in the context of psychedelic neuroimaging studies, as psychedelic drugs hold great therapeutic promise while at the same time are known to induce highly variable responses in individuals (Moujaes et al., 2022).

Our study contributes to this end by demonstrating that idiosyncrasy can serve as a useful heuristic for identifying clinically relevant brain connectivity patterns that map onto individual drug responses. Additionally, we presented a simple, brain-fingerprinting-inspired framework for isolating idiosyncratic FC patterns. Applying this framework to high-resolution fMRI data of healthy volunteers under the acute influence of psilocybin or a placebo, we revealed that psilocybin appears to alter not only how identifiable subjects are based on their FC, but also what makes them identifiable—that is to say, which features in the FC are most idiosyncratic. Finally, we were able to extract idiosyncratic functional connections between cortical RSNs that predicted the unique drug experience of individual psilocybin subjects.

Psilocybin-Induced Changes to the Functional Connectome Fingerprint

Whereas psilocybin did not seem to affect the temporal stability (Iself) of FC edges, our results implied significantly greater FC heterogeneity (lower Iothers) in the psilocybin than in the placebo group. This finding sheds an interesting new light onto the effects of psilocybin on BOLD signal dynamics. Specifically, despite previous work reporting that acute psilocybin leads to increased BOLD signal variability (Tagliazucchi et al., 2014) and the formation of short-lived communities of interacting brain regions (Petri et al., 2014), our finding suggests that an individual’s FC nevertheless exhibits unique features that remain stable over the course of a few minutes, in line with previous work (Van De Ville et al., 2021). This is relevant because, although the subjective effects of psilocybin have been described as “labile” (Griffiths et al., 2006), these effects are arguably unlikely to rapidly shift on a minute-by-minute basis. Our findings suggest that the stable and unique temporal features carried forward in the FC can be informative about the subjective effects of psilocybin. Notably, this highlights the utility of assessing the reliability of FC features in terms of temporal stability.

Additionally, the finding of lower Iothers and unchanged Iself in the psilocybin versus placebo group also matches the intuition that the subjective experiences of individuals, who are lying in an fMRI scanner, are likely to be more heterogeneous when under the influence of peak psilocybin drug effects. Indeed, as aforementioned, psychedelic experiences are commonly characterized by fluctuations across various cognitive and emotional aspects (Girn et al., 2020; Griffiths et al., 2006). Remarkably, similar work mapping functional connectome fingerprints of a group of individuals at baseline and after administration with ayahuasca, a psychedelic compound that targets the same 5-HT2AR neuroreceptor as psilocybin, observed a decrease in FC heterogeneity (higher Iothers) in the drug condition (Mallaroni et al., 2022). While on the face of it this may seem to contradict our findings, it is important to note that the participants of the mentioned ayahuasca study were in fact members of the same religious group, who convene on a regular basis to practice a ritualistic ceremony that involves the intake of ayahuasca. Thus, ayahuasca may have induced comparatively homogeneous subjective experiences in the religious practitioners by triggering a shared memory of common rituals, which in turn may have been reflected by greater FC similarity. In contrast, the subjects of our study did not engage in common ritualistic practices that involved psilocybin intake, and hence they likely had more heterogeneous associations with the psilocybin-induced psychedelic state. As such, both studies provide evidence to believe that FCs capture neural activity that is relevant to the subjective experience of acute psychedelic drug effects.

When mapping the cerebral distribution of idiosyncrasy, we observed, in line with previous work (Finn et al., 2015; Mallaroni et al., 2022), that FPN regions tended to have exceptionally idiosyncratic functional connectivity profiles under non-psychedelic (placebo) conditions. Additionally, we found that in psilocybin subjects the distribution of idiosyncrasy was systematically shifted, and in particular shifted away from FPN and towards DMN and Vis regions.

Intriguingly, DMN and Vis regions are known to exhibit exceptionally high densities of the 5-HT2A receptor, which is targeted by psilocybin and believed to be the primary mediator of the drug’s hallucinogenic effects (Beliveau et al., 2017; Nichols, 2016). Moreover, both the DMN and the Vis network have previously been directly associated with the subjective effects of psychedelics (Ballentine et al., 2022; R. L. Carhart-Harris et al., 2012). For instance, the DMN is assumed to play a key role in self-consciousness, and its perturbation through drugs or invasive electrical stimulation has been linked to distorted or weakened perceptions of self, commonly referred to as “ego dissolution” (Blanke et al., 2002; R. L. Carhart-Harris & Friston, 2010). Furthermore, a recent study from Ballentine and colleagues, analyzing 6,850 free-form testimonials of hallucinogenic experiences of various compounds, discovered a significant association between reports of drug-induced visualizations and the distinct neuroreceptor profile of cortical areas in the Vis system (Ballentine et al., 2022).

Hence, the first part of our analysis acted as an important proof of concept, indicating that a brain-fingerprinting approach might be capable of revealing the particular FC features that are relevant to the subjective experience of psilocybin.

Another noteworthy insight from the work of Ballentine and colleagues (Ballentine et al., 2022) was the finding that the subjective effects of psychedelics seem to emerge from the complex interplay of multiple neuroreceptors throughout the cortex, not limited to 5-HT2AR alone, which has hitherto been the main focus of psychedelic research. Thus, it would be highly interesting for future work to investigate the extent to which interindividual variability in the subjective effects of psychedelics is driven by interindividual variability in neuroreceptor expression as captured by “neuroreceptor fingerprints.”

Functional Connectome Correlates of Subjective Psilocybin Experience

Leveraging our brain-fingerprinting-inspired framework, we isolated an idiosyncratic FC pattern that predicted subjective psilocybin experience and was characterized by altered DMN connectivity. Specifically, decreased DMN-DMN and DMN-LIM, and increased DMN-attentional connectivity was associated with an intense psychedelic drug experience, and vice versa. These results contribute to the growing body of evidence that points to a central role of DMN connectivity in psychedelic action (Gattuso et al., 2022).

Most notably, DMN disintegration has been consistently associated with the acute psychedelic state across various neuroimaging modalities and psychedelic compounds (R. L. Carhart-Harris et al., 2012, 2016; Müller et al., 2018; Muthukumaraswamy et al., 2013; Preller et al., 2018), and a recent study demonstrated that the degree of DMN disintegration in subjects treated with psilocybin was determined by their psilocybin plasma concentration (Madsen et al., 2021). Moreover, within-DMN connectivity was repeatedly shown to be inversely related to the intensity of subjective psychedelic effects (R. L. Carhart-Harris et al., 2016; Madsen et al., 2021; Smigielski et al., 2019). In particular, lower within-DMN connectivity in response to psychedelic-drug administration tends to coincide with stronger subjective ratings of ego dissolution (R. L. Carhart-Harris et al., 2016; Smigielski et al., 2019). Interestingly, while our measure of subjective psilocybin experience constituted a one-dimensional summary of 11 aspects of psilocybin experience, it is worth mentioning that the three most dominant aspects (insightfulness, experience of unity, blissful state) were in fact subscales of the 5D-ASC dimension of “oceanic boundlessness,” commonly used to measure ego dissolution (Studerus et al., 2010).

It is largely agreed that the DMN relies on mnemonic and emotional input from the limbic system for a number of tasks, including self-referential thinking (R. L. Carhart-Harris & Friston, 2010; Lebedev et al., 2015; Mason et al., 2020). As such, it has been hypothesized that the acute breakdown of DMN-LIM coupling in response to psychedelics may prevent the DMN from accessing autobiographical memory that is essential for generating self-consciousness (Millière et al., 2018). This might explain our finding that DMN-LIM decoupling predicted intense psychedelic experiences. Remarkably, DMN connectivity with hippocampal regions of the limbic system has been reported to decrease acutely post administration with psychedelics, which was associated with subjective reports of ego dissolution (R. L. Carhart-Harris et al., 2014; Lebedev et al., 2015; Mason et al., 2020). Interestingly, a recent analysis of the dataset studied here found that in psilocybin subjects lower hippocampal glutamate levels were related to positively experienced ego dissolution, whereas elevated glutamate levels in the mPFC, a key DMN area, were linked to negatively experienced ego dissolution (Mason et al., 2020). Another limbic region that was shown to be acutely affected by psilocybin is the ACC (R. L. Carhart-Harris et al., 2012; Tagliazucchi et al., 2014), which in our study was found to have an exceptionally idiosyncratic connectivity profile. Specifically, the temporal BOLD signal variance in the ACC has been reported to increase in response to psilocybin administration (Tagliazucchi et al., 2014), and psilocybin-induced changes in the ACC BOLD signal amplitude were shown to be negatively correlated with the intensity of subjective effects (R. L. Carhart-Harris et al., 2012).

Furthermore, the relaxed beliefs under psychedelics (REBUS) theory (R. L. Carhart-Harris & Friston, 2019) offers a potential explanation for our finding that subjective psilocybin experience was correlated with DMN-attentional connectivity. This theory adopts the popular view of the brain as a hierarchical prediction machine, where top-down prior beliefs from higher level brain regions suppress expected activity in lower level brain regions, which in turn report back prediction errors via bottom-up information flow (Clark, 2013). Crucially, DMN regions are believed to be situated at the top end of this hierarchy, where they orchestrate the computation of high-level priors that purportedly constrain our perception of reality (R. L. Carhart-Harris & Friston, 2019; Margulies et al., 2016). Hence, by disrupting DMN activity, psychedelics are proposed to weaken the suppressive influence of top-down priors on lower level systems, leading to increased bottom-up signaling and, consequently, enabling unconstrained cognition such as that manifested in perceptual distortions and subjectively experienced increases in insightfulness, as frequently reported by individuals in the psychedelic state (R. L. Carhart-Harris & Friston, 2019; Studerus et al., 2010). (As an interesting side note, a recent study indicated that there is a mismatch between subjective and objective ratings of insightfulness during acute stages of psilocybin [Mason et al., 2021].)

In this context, the attention system is commonly assumed to sit at an intermediate level in the hierarchy, passing up information about salient sensory stimuli to higher level networks, including the DMN (R. L. Carhart-Harris & Friston, 2010; Margulies et al., 2016). Thus, our findings might reflect increased bottom-up signaling from attention networks to the DMN during peak psilocybin drug effects, consistent with the REBUS theory. Notably, our finding is also consistent with several lines of previous work, reporting an increase in DMN-attentional connectivity in response to psychedelics (R. L. Carhart-Harris et al., 2013, 2016; Kometer et al., 2015; Müller et al., 2018).

We acknowledge that our results may be equally well accounted for by alternative, possibly complementary, theories that also frame psychedelic action in terms of disrupted higher level association-network activity and increased bottom-up signaling, namely the cortico-claustro-cortical (Doss et al., 2022) and the cortico-striato-thalamocortical (Vollenweider & Geyer, 2001) theory, respectively. Yet, the fact that the REBUS model links psychedelic experience explicitly to modulations of DMN activity and connectivity makes this model especially illuminating in regards to our findings.

Paving the Way to Personalized Psychedelic Therapy

Much of the recent scientific interest in psychedelics is attributable to their putatively remarkable therapeutic potential (R. L. Carhart-Harris & Goodwin, 2017). For instance, early clinical trials testing the antidepressant effect of psilocybin-assisted psychotherapy have yielded promising results (R. Carhart-Harris et al., 2021; R. L. Carhart-Harris et al., 2018; Davis et al., 2021; Griffiths et al., 2016; Ross et al., 2016). Nevertheless, there is strong evidence that depression is in fact a highly heterogeneous disorder, urging the call for a more personalized depression-treatment plan (Buch & Liston, 2021; Fried & Nesse, 2015). One attractive route involves the use of computational brain simulations that can be informed by patient-individual neuroimaging data (Deco et al., 2018; Deco & Kringelbach, 2014; Moujaes et al., 2022; Vohryzek et al., 2023). Specifically, future personalized depression treatments may be chosen based on a two-step approach: (1) simulating the patient’s individual neural response to a (possibly psychedelic) treatment, and (2) predicting the treatment outcome based on the simulated neuroimaging data. Tantalizingly, this approach would enable individualized predictions of the counterfactual effects of various therapeutic interventions. In the context of psilocybin-assisted depression therapy, our study may provide key insights that can be used to inform the treatment-outcome predictions in Step 2 of the proposed approach. Indeed, several lines of clinical evidence imply that the acute psychedelic experience and the long-term antidepressant effects of psilocybin-assisted psychotherapy for depression may be coupled (Griffiths et al., 2016; Roseman et al., 2018; Ross et al., 2016). Moreover, similar results have been reported in two independent samples of patients with substance-use disorder (Bogenschutz et al., 2015; Johnson et al., 2017).

Limitations

Our study is subject to several limitations that future work may want to address. For instance, unlike classical brain-fingerprinting approaches, where reliability of FC features is commonly measured based on test-retest data from two or more separate scanning sessions, we assessed the reliability of FC features in terms of their temporal stability across one continuous scan. While we argue that the high interpretability of our results and the fact that they are largely consistent with previous work clearly suggest that our approach provided meaningful results, it would be interesting to replicate our study with test-retest data that were acquired during separate scanning sessions. In fact, our finding that the PC1 components, resulting from the FC-edge PCAs, consistently failed to explain the subjective effects of psilocybin implies that scanning-session differences may have been responsible for the largest proportion of interindividual FC variance.

Additionally, it may be worth noting that the choice of a between-subjects design in our study, as opposed to the perhaps more commonly employed within-subjects design in the literature, was based on two main considerations. First, psilocybin is believed to induce neuroplastic changes in the brain that can have sustained effects on mood and cognition, lasting weeks or even months (R. L. Carhart-Harris & Goodwin, 2017). Such prolonged effects can confound the acute measurements in a within-subjects, but not in a between-subjects, design. Second, the between-subjects design has been shown to reduce de-blinding risks compared with the within-subjects design, where all participants receive both the active and the control condition (Aday et al., 2022; Muthukumaraswamy et al., 2021).

Finally, we detected slightly more head motion in the psilocybin group, which may have biassed the identifiability results. However, we addressed this limitation by controlling for motion when testing for group differences. Also, we note that the majority of our analysis was based on ICC, which is calculated based on the whole group of subjects. Thus, while ICC group-comparisons may have been affected by motion differences, the ICC ranks of single edges within one group are likely insensitive to motion outliers.

Conclusion

In summary, our work points to a key role of DMN functional connectivity to regions of the default mode, limbic, and attentional networks, in mediating the acute subjective effects of psilocybin. Additionally, our results imply that idiosyncrasy provides a useful heuristic for identifying clinically relevant FC patterns that predict individual drug responses, and we present a simple framework for isolating these patterns. Notably, this framework may prove fruitful in future pharmacological neuroimaging studies. Overall, our study chimes in with the trending call (Moujaes et al., 2022) for a stronger research focus on idiosyncratic rather than merely generalizable brain-activity patterns, paving the way for more effective, personalized medical approaches.

Participants

Data from 60 healthy volunteers with previous, but not recent (i.e., not within the 3 months prior to the study), experience with a psychedelic drug was collected between July 2017 and June 2018 at Maastricht University. Following a randomized, placebo-controlled, double-blind parallel group design, participants were assigned to one of two conditions (0.17 mg/kg psilocybin, or placebo) such that groups were matched for age, sex, and educational level. In the psilocybin condition, subjects were administered psilocybin dissolved in bitter lemon. In the placebo condition, subjects were administered plain bitter lemon instead. The study obtained ethical approval from the Maastricht University’s Medical Ethics Committee and was in accordance with the Medical Research Involving Human Subjects Act (WMO) as well as with the code of ethics on human experimentation from the declaration of Helsinki (1964) and its amendments made in October 2013 in Fortaleza, Brazil. All participants were made fully aware of all procedures, possible adverse and expected beneficial effects, as well as their responsibilities and rights, including the right for voluntary termination without consequences. The data were collected as part of a larger clinical trial (Netherlands Trial Register: NTR6505). More detailed descriptions can be found in previous publications (Mason et al., 2020, 2021).

Out of the 60 participants, only 49 provided resting-state fMRI data. Furthermore, one subject was excluded because of missing values in the BOLD time series of five brain regions. Another subject had missing values in the BOLD time series of only one brain region in the visual subnetwork. This subject was not excluded. Instead, the corrupted time series was replaced by the average time series of the remaining 28 parcels in the visual subnetwork. Two further participants were excluded because of high head motion (more than 10% of fMRI volumes contained motion artifacts). The final sample comprised 21 psilocybin and 25 placebo subjects.

Neuroimaging

Participants underwent structural MRI 50-min post psilocybin/placebo administration, in addition to 6-min resting-state fMRI 102-min post psilocybin/placebo administration during the peak subjective drug effects. All images were acquired in a MAGNETOM 7T MRI scanner. The following acquisition parameters were used for the fMRI scans. TR = 1,400 ms; TE = 21 ms; field of view = 198 mm; flip angle = 60°; oblique acquisition orientation; interleaved slice acquisition; 72 slices; slice thickness = 1.5 mm; voxel size = 1.5 mm, isotropic. Furthermore, participants were presented with a black fixation cross on a white background during the scanning session and were asked to focus on the cross, clear their minds, and lie as still as possible.

Data Preprocessing

The neuroimaging data were preprocessed following the pipeline from Amico et al. (2017) and using the open-source MATLAB toolbox Apéro (https://github.com/juancarlosfarah/apero), which combines functionality from various software packages, including the FMRIB Software Library FSL (Jenkinson et al., 2012). We preprocessed each subject’s data only once, which involved preprocessing the entire, unsplit time series.

The T1-weighted (T1w) structural MRI images were denoised, skull stripped (HD-BET; Isensee et al., 2019) and segmented (FSL fast) to obtain individual gray matter, white matter, and cerebrospinal-fluid tissue masks for each subject. Next, the T1w images were registered to Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI-152) standard space by sequentially applying rigid-body (6 df; FSL flirt), affine (12 df; FSL flirt), and nonlinear (12 df; FSL fnirt) transformations. The inverse transformation matrices from this latter step were used to convert the Schaefer parcellation (Schaefer et al., 2018) with 200 cortical regions of interest (ROIs) from MNI-152 standard space to subject space. The parcellation was chosen based on a recent study showing that the generated connectomes were relatively robust to changes in the preprocessing pipeline (Luppi & Stamatakis, 2021). Individual gray matter parcellations were subsequently obtained by applying the subject’s gray matter tissue mask to the subject-space parcellation.

The fMRI BOLD time series for each ROI were extracted as follows. First, fMRI volumes were corrected for slice-timing (FSL slicetimer) and the skull was removed (FSL bet) prior to correcting for motion (FSL mcflirt). The signal was normalized to mode 1,000 and subsequently demeaned and linearly detrended. To enable the isolation of signals from different brain tissues and ROIs, the tissue masks and gray matter parcellations, which were previously derived from the T1w images, were registered to each subject’s fMRI mean volume via 6-df rigid-body transformation (FSL flirt), followed by inverse boundary-based T1w-to-fMRI image registration (FSL flirt -bbr). Then, a total of 18 regressors, comprising 3 translations (x, y, z), 3 rotations (pitch, yaw, roll), the average cerebrospinal-fluid, white matter, and whole-brain (i.e., global signal regression) signals, and all corresponding derivatives, were removed from the overall BOLD signal. The resulting signal was bandpass filtered, retaining frequencies between 0.01 and 0.25 Hz, by applying a first-order Butterworth filter. Subsequently, the first principal component (PC1) of the cerebrospinal-fluid, white matter, and whole-brain signals were, sequentially, regressed out in an additional cleaning step (note that this step is equivalent to the CompCor method described by Behzadi et al., 2007). Specifically, these principal components were included as nuisance parameters within a general linear model for the BOLD time series data. The time series were extracted by regressing out the nuisance parameters from the BOLD time series and were averaged across adjacent voxels to obtain one time series per ROI.

Assessing Head Motion

The number of fMRI volumes with motion artifacts was used to gauge the amount of head motion of each participant during the scan. Following Amico et al. (2017), motion artifacts were detected based on three parameters: (a) frame displacement, which estimates the displacement of the head between consecutive volumes in mm (FSL fsl_motion_outliers); (b) DVARS, which detects abrupt changes in signal intensity from one volume to the next and is defined as the root mean square variance over voxels of the temporal derivatives of the time series (FSL fsl_motion_outliers); and (c) the standard deviation of the BOLD signal across voxels at each time point. Specifically, a volume was marked to contain motion artifacts if any of the three conditions was met: (a) frame displacement > 0.55; (b) DVARS > 75 percentile + 1.5 of the interquartile range; (c) standard deviation > 75 percentile + 1.5 of the interquartile range. There was a slightly significant difference in motion between the groups, indicating that psilocybin subjects tended to move more than placebo subjects (mean difference = 4.22; t stat. = 2.06; p = 0.0456). Thus, we accounted for motion as confounder wherever applicable, as described in the Statistical Analysis subsection.

Ratings of Subjective Drug Experience

Participants were asked to retrospectively report their subjective drug experience by completing the Five Dimensions of Altered States of Consciousness (5D-ASC) questionnaire (Dittrich, 1998) 360 min post drug administration. This well-established questionnaire was originally designed to measure five etiology-independent dimensions of the subjective experience of ASCs. However, more recent evidence suggests that the proposed dimensions can be decomposed into 11 subdimensions with greater interpretability and improved ability to discriminate between distinct psychoactive-drug conditions (Studerus et al., 2010). Thus, our analysis focuses on these 11 subdimensions, which are UN = experience of unity; SP = spiritual experience; BS = blissful state; IS = insightfulness; DB = disembodiment; IC = impaired control and cognition; AX = anxiety; CI = complex imagery; EI = elementary imagery; SY = audiovisual synesthesia; and CM = changed meaning of percepts. For more details, we refer the reader to Studerus et al. (2010).

Functional Connectomes

We constructed FCs with N = 200 nodes, where each node i represented one of 200 cortical brain regions of the Schaefer parcellation (Schaefer et al., 2018) and was connected to every other node j according to the Pearson correlation of the fMRI BOLD time series of i and j. For each subject, we derived one FC from the first and another from the second half of the fMRI time series. The resulting two FCs per subject were referred to as first-half and second-half FC, respectively, and we used them to calculate the brain-fingerprinting metrics (Iself, Iothers, Idiff) and edgewise ICC values. Additionally, we computed another FC per subject from the unsplitted fMRI time series. Edges from this latter FC served as potential predictors of subjective drug experience.

Brain Fingerprinting

Building on the work of Amico and Goñi (2018), we measured the identifiability of a subject i given a set of FC edges X using the metric Idiff, which is defined as the difference of that subject’s Iself and Iothers.
Iself and Iothers in turn can be derived from the (nonsymmetric, square) identifiability matrix A, in which each element ai,j encodes the Pearson correlation coefficient of the second-half FC of subject i and the first-half FC of subject j, considering the edge selection X. The diagonal elements of A correspond to the first-half versus the second-half similarities of the subjects with respect to X, which we refer to as Iself.
Furthermore, the off-diagonal elements of A encode the between-subject similarities with respect to X. Hence, the Iothers of subject i was calculated by averaging across the off-diagonal elements of the ith row and column in A. (Note: We derived a separate identifiability matrix for each condition such that only within-group, between-subject correlation values were used to compute Iothers.)
where s is the number of subjects in the condition of subject i. Taken together, edge selections with high average Idiff tend to be stable throughout the 6-min fMRI scan of the same subject, but vary strongly between subjects. In other words, they are highly subject-specific just like a person’s individual fingerprint.

Intraclass Correlation

Intraclass correlation (ICC) represents a widely used statistical measure to quantify the reliability of repeated measurements taken from data that are structured in groups. A high ICC value indicates high similarity among measurements from the same group and strong between-group variance. Importantly, previous work on brain fingerprinting using ICC to assess the reliability of FC traits (= groups) across test-retest FCs (= measurements) suggests that selecting edges based on high ICC can improve subject identifiability and behavioral predictions (Amico & Goñi, 2018; Finn et al., 2015). Following this rationale, we calculated class-1 intraclass correlation of FC edges across first- and second-half FCs as follows.
Here, var(μ) is the variance of μ, μ is a vector encoding the average edge value across first- and second-half FCs for each subject, s is the number of subjects, and σ is a vector encoding the variance of edge values across first- and second-half FCs for each subject. Put simply, MSR is the variance of group means and MSW is the total within-group variance. Hence, an FC edge with high ICC tends to differ strongly between subjects, while it remains temporally stable throughout the scan of the same subject. (Note: We computed the edgewise ICCs for each condition separately, as ICC is blind to potential systematic differences between groups of subjects.)

Predicting Subjective Psilocybin Experience

To examine whether idiosyncratic FC patterns allow for improved predictions of interindividual differences in subjective psilocybin experience, we leveraged PCA to isolate independent, heterogeneously distributed traits and reduce the dimensionality of our prediction target (subjective psilocybin experience) and the predictors (FC edges). PCA is a widely applied statistical technique that can be used to re-represent the data in the space of principal components (PCs). Importantly, these PCs are orthogonal and hierarchically ordered such that the first and last PCs point in the direction of the largest and smallest variance in the data, respectively. Hence, each PC accounts for an independent proportion of variance in the data, which is encoded in the projections of each data point onto the PC, also known as the PC scores.

We ran PCAs on two different sets of data: (a) the behavioral data (21 × 11; subjects × ASC aspects), and (b) the functional connectivity values of n FC edges from all subjects (21 × n; subjects × FC edges), where the n edges were selected based on either idiosyncrasy (ICCpsi) or randomly. The resulting PCs can therefore, respectively, be interpreted in terms of independently distributed ASC and FC patterns with the corresponding PC scores indicating the extent to which these patterns were present in each subject. Subsequent to the PCAs, we constructed linear models to predict subjective psilocybin experience from FC edges, where the former was represented by the scores of PC1 from the behavioral PCA and the latter by the scores of PC1–3 from the FC-edge PCA.

The choice of our prediction-target representation was based on the fact that the PC1 of the behavioral PCA accounted for a reasonable proportion of variance in the data (45.7%) and it captured a highly interpretable ASC pattern that is arguably of great interest, namely the intensity of the drug experience (see Figure 4B).

The line of reasoning behind the construction of our predictors shall be illustrated in the following. First, our motivation for using PCA in this context was to remove potential redundancies in the selected edges and isolate the independent, maximally heterogeneous FC patterns that we are interested in. Furthermore, recall that the core hypothesis of our study is that selecting edges based on idiosyncrasy has the effect of enriching for interindividual variance that maps onto heterogeneous behavioral traits. That is, behaviorally relevant neural processes are hypothesized to be more important contributors to the variance of idiosyncratic edges than to that of random edges. As such, we expect that in idiosyncratic edges, behavioral heterogeneity may be reflected by one or more of the few most important PCs, whereas in random edges it may be reflected by less important PCs, or not at all. For this reason, we argue that a small number of PC-score predictors k is sufficient for testing our hypothesis. Specifically, evidence in support of our hypothesis would be if idiosyncrasy-informed models consistently outperform random models for a constant, small k, and the reverse would represent conflicting evidence. We used k = 3; however, we show that our overall results are replicated with different choices of k (Supplementary Figure 5). We further argue that choosing a small number for k is not only sufficient but also preferable to avoid the risk of overfitting as demonstrated in Supplementary Figure 5, showing that with k = 20 all idiosyncrasy-informed and random models explain 100% of subjective psilocybin experience. Finally, although in classical PCA-based predictive modeling approaches it is common to choose k such that the cumulative percentage of variance in the data explained by PC1-k is close to 100%, this rule of thumb was not reasonable in our case (a) because of the risk of overfitting, as aforementioned, and (b) because this approach would have required us to change k for each n, which was incremented at each iteration, and for each random model; even if we had done so, it would not have been possible to perfectly control the percentage of variance explained by PC1-k, rendering model comparisons difficult.

Statistical Analysis

When comparing brain-fingerprinting measures between drug-placebo conditions, we included motion as a potential confounding factor in the linear model to predict the measure of interest from the categorical group variable (i.e., y ∼ group + motion), and reported the effect of the group variable. Otherwise, the default statistical test was a paired t test. Where applicable, false discovery rate (FDR)-corrected p values are given (indicated as “fdr”).

E.A. acknowledges financial support from the SNSF Ambizione project “Fingerprinting the brain: Network science to extract features of cognition, behaviour and dysfunction” (grant number PZ00P2_185716).

The connectomes and the accompanying covariates used to differentiate individuals can be made available to qualified research institutions upon reasonable request to J.G.R. and a data use agreement executed with Maastricht University.

All code used for analysis is available on E.A.’s GitHub page (Amico, 2023).

Supporting information for this article is available at https://doi.org/10.1162/netn_a_00349.

Hanna M. Tolle: Conceptualization; Data curation; Formal analysis; Methodology; Software; Writing – original draft; Writing – review & editing. Juan Carlos Farah: Data curation; Methodology; Software; Writing – review & editing. Pablo Mallaroni: Data curation; Writing – review & editing. Natasha L. Mason: Data curation; Writing – review & editing. Johannes G. Ramaekers: Data curation; Writing – review & editing. Enrico Amico: Conceptualization; Funding acquisition; Investigation; Methodology; Supervision; Writing – review & editing.

Enrico Amico, Schweizerischer Nationalfonds zur Förderung der Wissenschaftlichen Forschung (https://dx.doi.org/10.13039/501100001711), Award ID: PZ00P2_185716.

Altered states of consciousness (ASC):

Profound changes in subjective experience, such as those induced by drugs or meditation.

Functional connectome (FC):

Network representation of the brain, highlighting brain regions that are connected according to the statistical dependencies of their neurophysiological activity.

Edge:

Connection between two components in a network. For example, functional connectome edges represent the functional coupling between two brain regions.

Resting-state networks:

Strongly coupled brain regions, forming subnetworks within the functional connectome that are especially prominent when the brain is at rest.

Brain’s functional hierarchy:

The gradient of information-processing complexity from unimodal sensory-motor cortices to transmodal association cortices.

Idiosyncrasy:

The property of being characteristic of an individual.

Test-retest reliability:

The property of remaining stable across test and retest data acquisitions, which are typically performed on different days.

Brain fingerprinting:

A field of network neuroscience studying features of the functional connectome that, just like fingerprints, encode information about subject identity.

Aday
,
J. S.
,
Heifets
,
B. D.
,
Pratscher
,
S. D.
,
Bradley
,
E.
,
Rosen
,
R.
, &
Woolley
,
J. D.
(
2022
).
Great expectations: Recommendations for improving the methodological rigor of psychedelic clinical trials
.
Psychopharmacology
,
239
(
6
),
1989
2010
. ,
[PubMed]
Amico
,
E.
(
2023
).
Psilocybin fingerprints, GitHub
, https://github.com/eamico/Psilocybin_fingerprints
Amico
,
E.
, &
Goñi
,
J.
(
2018
).
The quest for identifiability in human functional connectomes
.
Scientific Reports
,
8
(
1
),
8254
. ,
[PubMed]
Amico
,
E.
,
Marinazzo
,
D.
,
Di Perri
,
C.
,
Heine
,
L.
,
Annen
,
J.
,
Martial
,
C.
,
Dzemidzic
,
M.
,
Kirsch
,
M.
,
Bonhomme
,
V.
,
Laureys
,
S.
, &
Goñi
,
J.
(
2017
).
Mapping the functional connectome traits of levels of consciousness
.
NeuroImage
,
148
,
201
211
. ,
[PubMed]
Ballentine
,
G.
,
Friedman
,
S. F.
, &
Bzdok
,
D.
(
2022
).
Trips and neurotransmitters: Discovering principled patterns across 6850 hallucinogenic experiences
.
Science Advances
,
8
(
11
),
eabl6989
. ,
[PubMed]
Behzadi
,
Y.
,
Restom
,
K.
,
Liau
,
J.
, &
Liu
,
T. T.
(
2007
).
A component based noise correction method (CompCor) for BOLD and perfusion based fMRI
.
NeuroImage
,
37
(
1
),
90
101
. ,
[PubMed]
Beliveau
,
V.
,
Ganz
,
M.
,
Feng
,
L.
,
Ozenne
,
B.
,
Højgaard
,
L.
,
Fisher
,
P. M.
,
Svarer
,
C.
,
Greve
,
D. N.
, &
Knudsen
,
G. M.
(
2017
).
A high-resolution in vivo atlas of the human brain’s serotonin system
.
Journal of Neuroscience
,
37
(
1
),
120
128
. ,
[PubMed]
Blanke
,
O.
,
Ortigue
,
S.
,
Landis
,
T.
, &
Seeck
,
M.
(
2002
).
Stimulating illusory own-body perceptions
.
Nature
,
419
(
6904
),
269
270
. ,
[PubMed]
Bogenschutz
,
M. P.
,
Forcehimes
,
A. A.
,
Pommy
,
J. A.
,
Wilcox
,
C. E.
,
Barbosa
,
P.
, &
Strassman
,
R. J.
(
2015
).
Psilocybin-assisted treatment for alcohol dependence: A proof-of-concept study
.
Journal of Psychopharmacology
,
29
(
3
),
289
299
. ,
[PubMed]
Buch
,
A. M.
, &
Liston
,
C.
(
2021
).
Dissecting diagnostic heterogeneity in depression by integrating neuroimaging and genetics
.
Neuropsychopharmacology
,
46
(
1
),
156
175
. ,
[PubMed]
Bullmore
,
E.
, &
Sporns
,
O.
(
2009
).
Complex brain networks: Graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems
.
Nature Reviews Neuroscience
,
10
(
3
),
186
198
. ,
[PubMed]
Carhart-Harris
,
R.
,
Giribaldi
,
B.
,
Watts
,
R.
,
Baker-Jones
,
M.
,
Murphy-Beiner
,
A.
,
Murphy
,
R.
,
Martell
,
J.
,
Blemings
,
A.
,
Erritzoe
,
D.
, &
Nutt
,
D. J.
(
2021
).
Trial of psilocybin versus escitalopram for depression
.
New England Journal of Medicine
,
384
(
15
),
1402
1411
. ,
[PubMed]
Carhart-Harris
,
R. L.
,
Bolstridge
,
M.
,
Day
,
C. M. J.
,
Rucker
,
J.
,
Watts
,
R.
,
Erritzoe
,
D. E.
,
Kaelen
,
M.
,
Giribaldi
,
B.
,
Bloomfield
,
M.
,
Pilling
,
S.
,
Rickard
,
J. A.
,
Forbes
,
B.
,
Feilding
,
A.
,
Taylor
,
D.
,
Curran
,
H. V.
, &
Nutt
,
D. J.
(
2018
).
Psilocybin with psychological support for treatment-resistant depression: Six-month follow-up
.
Psychopharmacology
,
235
(
2
),
399
408
. ,
[PubMed]
Carhart-Harris
,
R. L.
,
Erritzoe
,
D.
,
Williams
,
T.
,
Stone
,
J. M.
,
Reed
,
L. J.
,
Colasanti
,
A.
,
Tyacke
,
R. J.
,
Leech
,
R.
,
Malizia
,
A. L.
,
Murphy
,
K.
,
Hobden
,
P.
,
Evans
,
J.
,
Feilding
,
A.
,
Wise
,
R. G.
, &
Nutt
,
D. J.
(
2012
).
Neural correlates of the psychedelic state as determined by fMRI studies with psilocybin
.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
,
109
(
6
),
2138
2143
. ,
[PubMed]
Carhart-Harris
,
R. L.
, &
Friston
,
K. J.
(
2010
).
The default-mode, ego-functions and free-energy: A neurobiological account of Freudian ideas
.
Brain
,
133
(
4
),
1265
1283
. ,
[PubMed]
Carhart-Harris
,
R. L.
, &
Friston
,
K. J.
(
2019
).
REBUS and the anarchic brain: Toward a unified model of the brain action of psychedelics
.
Pharmacological Reviews
,
71
(
3
),
316
344
. ,
[PubMed]
Carhart-Harris
,
R. L.
, &
Goodwin
,
G. M.
(
2017
).
The therapeutic potential of psychedelic drugs: Past, present, and future
.
Neuropsychopharmacology
,
42
(
11
),
2105
2113
. ,
[PubMed]
Carhart-Harris
,
R. L.
,
Leech
,
R.
,
Erritzoe
,
D.
,
Williams
,
T. M.
,
Stone
,
J. M.
,
Evans
,
J.
,
Sharp
,
D. J.
,
Feilding
,
A.
,
Wise
,
R. G.
, &
Nutt
,
D. J.
(
2013
).
Functional connectivity measures after psilocybin inform a novel hypothesis of early psychosis
.
Schizophrenia Bulletin
,
39
(
6
),
1343
1351
. ,
[PubMed]
Carhart-Harris
,
R. L.
,
Leech
,
R.
,
Hellyer
,
P. J.
,
Shanahan
,
M.
,
Feilding
,
A.
,
Tagliazucchi
,
E.
,
Chialvo
,
D. R.
, &
Nutt
,
D.
(
2014
).
The entropic brain: A theory of conscious states informed by neuroimaging research with psychedelic drugs
.
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience
,
8
,
20
. ,
[PubMed]
Carhart-Harris
,
R. L.
,
Muthukumaraswamy
,
S.
,
Roseman
,
L.
,
Kaelen
,
M.
,
Droog
,
W.
,
Murphy
,
K.
,
Tagliazucchi
,
E.
,
Schenberg
,
E. E.
,
Nest
,
T.
,
Orban
,
C.
,
Leech
,
R.
,
Williams
,
L. T.
,
Williams
,
T. M.
,
Bolstridge
,
M.
,
Sessa
,
B.
,
McGonigle
,
J.
,
Sereno
,
M. I.
,
Nichols
,
D.
,
Hellyer
,
P. J.
, …
Nutt
,
D. J.
(
2016
).
Neural correlates of the LSD experience revealed by multimodal neuroimaging
.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
,
113
(
17
),
4853
4858
. ,
[PubMed]
Clark
,
A.
(
2013
).
Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science
.
Behavioral and Brain Sciences
,
36
(
3
),
181
204
. ,
[PubMed]
Corlett
,
P. R.
,
Horga
,
G.
,
Fletcher
,
P. C.
,
Alderson-Day
,
B.
,
Schmack
,
K.
, &
Powers
,
A. R.
(
2019
).
Hallucinations and strong priors
.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences
,
23
(
2
),
114
127
. ,
[PubMed]
Davis
,
A. K.
,
Barrett
,
F. S.
,
May
,
D. G.
,
Cosimano
,
M. P.
,
Sepeda
,
N. D.
,
Johnson
,
M. W.
,
Finan
,
P. H.
, &
Griffiths
,
R. R.
(
2021
).
Effects of psilocybin-assisted therapy on major depressive disorder: A randomized clinical trial
.
JAMA Psychiatry
,
78
(
5
),
481
489
. ,
[PubMed]
Daws
,
R. E.
,
Timmermann
,
C.
,
Giribaldi
,
B.
,
Sexton
,
J. D.
,
Wall
,
M. B.
,
Erritzoe
,
D.
,
Roseman
,
L.
,
Nutt
,
D.
, &
Carhart-Harris
,
R.
(
2022
).
Increased global integration in the brain after psilocybin therapy for depression
.
Nature Medicine
,
28
(
4
),
844
851
. ,
[PubMed]
Deco
,
G.
,
Cruzat
,
J.
,
Cabral
,
J.
,
Knudsen
,
G. M.
,
Carhart-Harris
,
R. L.
,
Whybrow
,
P. C.
,
Logothetis
,
N. K.
, &
Kringelbach
,
M. L.
(
2018
).
Whole-brain multimodal neuroimaging model using serotonin receptor maps explains non-linear functional effects of LSD
.
Current Biology
,
28
(
19
),
3065
3074
. ,
[PubMed]
Deco
,
G.
, &
Kringelbach
,
M. L.
(
2014
).
Great expectations: Using whole-brain computational connectomics for understanding neuropsychiatric disorders
.
Neuron
,
84
(
5
),
892
905
. ,
[PubMed]
Dittrich
,
A.
(
1998
).
The standardized psychometric assessment of altered states of consciousness (ASCs) in humans
.
Pharmacopsychiatry
,
31
,
80
84
. ,
[PubMed]
Doss
,
M. K.
,
Madden
,
M. B.
,
Gaddis
,
A.
,
Nebel
,
M. B.
,
Griffiths
,
R. R.
,
Mathur
,
B. N.
, &
Barrett
,
F. S.
(
2022
).
Models of psychedelic drug action: Modulation of cortical-subcortical circuits
.
Brain
,
145
(
2
),
441
456
. ,
[PubMed]
Finn
,
E. S.
,
Shen
,
X.
,
Scheinost
,
D.
,
Rosenberg
,
M. D.
,
Huang
,
J.
,
Chun
,
M. M.
,
Papademetris
,
X.
, &
Constable
,
R. T.
(
2015
).
Functional connectome fingerprinting: Identifying individuals using patterns of brain connectivity
.
Nature Neuroscience
,
18
(
11
),
1664
1671
. ,
[PubMed]
Fried
,
E. I.
, &
Nesse
,
R. M.
(
2015
).
Depression is not a consistent syndrome: An investigation of unique symptom patterns in the STAR*D study
.
Journal of Affective Disorders
,
172
,
96
102
. ,
[PubMed]
Gattuso
,
J. J.
,
Perkins
,
D.
,
Ruffell
,
S.
,
Lawrence
,
A. J.
,
Hoyer
,
D.
,
Jacobson
,
L. H.
,
Timmermann
,
C.
,
Castle
,
D.
,
Rossell
,
S. L.
,
Downey
,
L. A.
,
Pagni
,
B. A.
,
Galvão-Coelho
,
N. L.
,
Nutt
,
D.
, &
Sarris
,
J.
(
2022
).
Default mode network modulation by psychedelics: A systematic review
.
International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology
,
26
(
3
),
155
188
. ,
[PubMed]
Girn
,
M.
,
Mills
,
C.
,
Roseman
,
L.
,
Carhart-Harris
,
R. L.
, &
Christoff
,
K.
(
2020
).
Updating the dynamic framework of thought: Creativity and psychedelics
.
NeuroImage
,
213
,
116726
. ,
[PubMed]
Glennon
,
R. A.
,
Teitler
,
M.
, &
Sanders-Bush
,
E.
(
1992
).
Hallucinogens and serotonergic mechanisms
.
NIDA Research Monograph
,
119
,
131
135
.
[PubMed]
Griffiths
,
R. R.
,
Johnson
,
M. W.
,
Carducci
,
M. A.
,
Umbricht
,
A.
,
Richards
,
W. A.
,
Richards
,
B. D.
,
Cosimano
,
M. P.
, &
Klinedinst
,
M. A.
(
2016
).
Psilocybin produces substantial and sustained decreases in depression and anxiety in patients with life-threatening cancer: A randomized double-blind trial
.
Journal of Psychopharmacology
,
30
(
12
),
1181
1197
. ,
[PubMed]
Griffiths
,
R. R.
,
Richards
,
W. A.
,
McCann
,
U.
, &
Jesse
,
R.
(
2006
).
Psilocybin can occasion mystical-type experiences having substantial and sustained personal meaning and spiritual significance
.
Psychopharmacology
,
187
(
3
),
268
283
;
discussion 284–292
. ,
[PubMed]
Isensee
,
F.
,
Schell
,
M.
,
Pflueger
,
I.
,
Brugnara
,
G.
,
Bonekamp
,
D.
,
Neuberger
,
U.
,
Wick
,
A.
,
Schlemmer
,
H.-P.
,
Heiland
,
S.
,
Wick
,
W.
,
Bendszus
,
M.
,
Maier-Hein
,
K. H.
, &
Kickingereder
,
P.
(
2019
).
Automated brain extraction of multisequence MRI using artificial neural networks
.
Human Brain Mapping
,
40
(
17
),
4952
4964
. ,
[PubMed]
Jenkinson
,
M.
,
Beckmann
,
C. F.
,
Behrens
,
T. E. J.
,
Woolrich
,
M. W.
, &
Smith
,
S. M.
(
2012
).
FSL
.
NeuroImage
,
62
(
2
),
782
790
. ,
[PubMed]
Johnson
,
M. W.
,
Garcia-Romeu
,
A.
, &
Griffiths
,
R. R.
(
2017
).
Long-term follow-up of psilocybin-facilitated smoking cessation
.
American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse
,
43
(
1
),
55
60
. ,
[PubMed]
Johnson
,
M. W.
,
Griffiths
,
R. R.
,
Hendricks
,
P. S.
, &
Henningfield
,
J. E.
(
2018
).
The abuse potential of medical psilocybin according to the 8 factors of the Controlled Substances Act
.
Neuropharmacology
,
142
,
143
166
. ,
[PubMed]
Johnson
,
M. W.
,
Hendricks
,
P. S.
,
Barrett
,
F. S.
, &
Griffiths
,
R. R.
(
2019
).
Classic psychedelics: An integrative review of epidemiology, therapeutics, mystical experience, and brain network function
.
Pharmacology and Therapeutics
,
197
,
83
102
. ,
[PubMed]
Kometer
,
M.
,
Pokorny
,
T.
,
Seifritz
,
E.
, &
Volleinweider
,
F. X.
(
2015
).
Psilocybin-induced spiritual experiences and insightfulness are associated with synchronization of neuronal oscillations
.
Psychopharmacology
,
232
(
19
),
3663
3676
. ,
[PubMed]
Lebedev
,
A. V.
,
Lövdén
,
M.
,
Rosenthal
,
G.
,
Feilding
,
A.
,
Nutt
,
D. J.
, &
Carhart-Harris
,
R. L.
(
2015
).
Finding the self by losing the self: Neural correlates of ego-dissolution under psilocybin
.
Human Brain Mapping
,
36
(
8
),
3137
3153
. ,
[PubMed]
Luppi
,
A. I.
,
Mediano
,
P. A. M.
,
Rosas
,
F. E.
,
Holland
,
N.
,
Fryer
,
T. D.
,
O’Brien
,
J. T.
,
Rowe
,
J. B.
,
Menon
,
D. K.
,
Bor
,
D.
, &
Stamatakis
,
E. A.
(
2022
).
A synergistic core for human brain evolution and cognition
.
Nature Neuroscience
,
25
(
6
),
771
782
. ,
[PubMed]
Luppi
,
A. I.
, &
Stamatakis
,
E. A.
(
2021
).
Combining network topology and information theory to construct representative brain networks
.
Network Neuroscience
,
5
(
1
),
96
124
. ,
[PubMed]
Madsen
,
M. K.
,
Stenbæk
,
D. S.
,
Arvidsson
,
A.
,
Armand
,
S.
,
Marstrand-Joergensen
,
M. R.
,
Johansen
,
S. S.
,
Linnet
,
K.
,
Ozenne
,
B.
,
Knudsen
,
G. M.
, &
Fisher
,
P. M.
(
2021
).
Psilocybin-induced changes in brain network integrity and segregation correlate with plasma psilocin level and psychedelic experience
.
European Neuropsychopharmacology
,
50
,
121
132
. ,
[PubMed]
Mallaroni
,
P.
,
Mason
,
N. L.
,
Kloft
,
L.
,
Reckweg
,
J. T.
,
van Oorsouw
,
K.
,
Toennes
,
S. W.
,
Tolle
,
H. M.
,
Amico
,
E.
, &
Ramaekers
,
J. G.
(
2022
).
Ritualistic use of ayahuasca enhances a shared functional connectome identity with others
.
bioRxiv
.
Margulies
,
D. S.
,
Ghosh
,
S. S.
,
Goulas
,
A.
,
Falkiewicz
,
M.
,
Huntenburg
,
J. M.
,
Langs
,
G.
,
Bezgin
,
G.
,
Eickhoff
,
S. B.
,
Castellanos
,
F. X.
,
Petrides
,
M.
,
Jefferies
,
E.
, &
Smallwood
,
J.
(
2016
).
Situating the default-mode network along a principal gradient of macroscale cortical organization
.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
,
113
(
44
),
12574
12579
. ,
[PubMed]
Mason
,
N. L.
,
Kuypers
,
K. P. C.
,
Müller
,
F.
,
Reckweg
,
J.
,
Tse
,
D. H. Y.
,
Toennes
,
S. W.
,
Hutten
,
N. R. P. W.
,
Jansen
,
J. F. A.
,
Stiers
,
P.
,
Feilding
,
A.
, &
Ramaekers
,
J. G.
(
2020
).
Me, myself, bye: Regional alterations in glutamate and the experience of ego dissolution with psilocybin
.
Neuropsychopharmacology
,
45
(
12
),
2003
2011
. ,
[PubMed]
Mason
,
N. L.
,
Kuypers
,
K. P. C.
,
Reckweg
,
J. T.
,
Müller
,
F.
,
Tse
,
D. H. Y.
,
Da Rios
,
B.
,
Toennes
,
S. W.
,
Stiers
,
P.
,
Feilding
,
A.
, &
Ramaekers
,
J. G.
(
2021
).
Spontaneous and deliberate creative cognition during and after psilocybin exposure
.
Translational Psychiatry
,
11
(
1
),
209
. ,
[PubMed]
Millière
,
R.
,
Carhart-Harris
,
R. L.
,
Roseman
,
L.
,
Trautwein
,
F.-M.
, &
Berkovich-Ohana
,
A.
(
2018
).
Psychedelics, meditation, and self-consciousness
.
Frontiers in Psychology
,
9
,
1475
. ,
[PubMed]
Moreno
,
F. A.
,
Wiegand
,
C. B.
,
Taitano
,
E. K.
, &
Delgado
,
P. L.
(
2006
).
Safety, tolerability, and efficacy of psilocybin in 9 patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder
.
Journal of Clinical Psychiatry
,
67
(
11
),
1735
1740
. ,
[PubMed]
Moujaes
,
F.
,
Preller
,
K. H.
,
Ji
,
J. L.
,
Murray
,
J. D.
,
Berkovitch
,
L.
,
Vollenweider
,
F. X.
, &
Anticevic
,
A.
(
2022
).
Toward mapping neurobehavioral heterogeneity of psychedelic neurobiology in humans
.
Biological Psychiatry
,
93
(
12
),
1061
1070
. ,
[PubMed]
Müller
,
F.
,
Dolder
,
P. C.
,
Schmidt
,
A.
,
Liechti
,
M. E.
, &
Borgwardt
,
S.
(
2018
).
Altered network hub connectivity after acute LSD administration
.
NeuroImage: Clinical
,
18
,
694
701
. ,
[PubMed]
Muthukumaraswamy
,
S. D.
,
Carhart-Harris
,
R. L.
,
Moran
,
R. J.
,
Brookes
,
M. J.
,
Williams
,
T. M.
,
Errtizoe
,
D.
,
Sessa
,
B.
,
Papadopoulos
,
A.
,
Bolstridge
,
M.
,
Singh
,
K. D.
,
Feilding
,
A.
,
Friston
,
K. J.
, &
Nutt
,
D. J.
(
2013
).
Broadband cortical desynchronization underlies the human psychedelic state
.
Journal of Neuroscience
,
33
(
38
),
15171
15183
. ,
[PubMed]
Muthukumaraswamy
,
S. D.
,
Forsyth
,
A.
, &
Lumley
,
T.
(
2021
).
Blinding and expectancy confounds in psychedelic randomized controlled trials
.
Expert Review of Clinical Pharmacology
,
14
(
9
),
1133
1152
. ,
[PubMed]
Nichols
,
D. E.
(
2016
).
Psychedelics
.
Pharmacological Reviews
,
68
(
2
),
264
355
. ,
[PubMed]
Nichols
,
D. E.
(
2020
).
Psilocybin: From ancient magic to modern medicine
.
Journal of Antibiotics
,
73
(
10
),
679
686
. ,
[PubMed]
Petri
,
G.
,
Expert
,
P.
,
Turkheimer
,
F.
,
Carhart-Harris
,
R.
,
Nutt
,
D.
,
Hellyer
,
P. J.
, &
Vaccarino
,
F.
(
2014
).
Homological scaffolds of brain functional networks
.
Journal of the Royal Society Interface
,
11
(
101
),
20140873
. ,
[PubMed]
Preller
,
K. H.
,
Burt
,
J. B.
,
Ji
,
J. L.
,
Schleifer
,
C. H.
,
Adkinson
,
B. D.
,
Stämpfli
,
P.
,
Seifritz
,
E.
,
Repovs
,
G.
,
Krystal
,
J. H.
,
Murray
,
J. D.
,
Vollenweider
,
F. X.
, &
Anticevic
,
A.
(
2018
).
Changes in global and thalamic brain connectivity in LSD-induced altered states of consciousness are attributable to the 5-HT2A receptor
.
eLife
,
7
,
e35082
. ,
[PubMed]
Preller
,
K. H.
,
Duerler
,
P.
,
Burt
,
J. B.
,
Ji
,
J. L.
,
Adkinson
,
B.
,
Stämpfli
,
P.
,
Seifritz
,
E.
,
Repovš
,
G.
,
Krystal
,
J. H.
,
Murray
,
J. D.
,
Anticevic
,
A.
, &
Vollenweider
,
F. X.
(
2020
).
Psilocybin induces time-dependent changes in global functional connectivity
.
Biological Psychiatry
,
88
(
2
),
197
207
. ,
[PubMed]
Roseman
,
L.
,
Nutt
,
D. J.
, &
Carhart-Harris
,
R. L.
(
2018
).
Quality of acute psychedelic experience predicts therapeutic efficacy of psilocybin for treatment-resistant depression
.
Frontiers in Pharmacology
,
8
,
974
. ,
[PubMed]
Ross
,
S.
,
Bossis
,
A.
,
Guss
,
J.
,
Agin-Liebes
,
G.
,
Malone
,
T.
,
Cohen
,
B.
,
Mennenga
,
S. E.
,
Belser
,
A.
,
Kalliontzi
,
K.
,
Babb
,
J.
,
Su
,
Z.
,
Corby
,
P.
, &
Schmidt
,
B. L.
(
2016
).
Rapid and sustained symptom reduction following psilocybin treatment for anxiety and depression in patients with life-threatening cancer: A randomized controlled trial
.
Journal of Psychopharmacology
,
30
(
12
),
1165
1180
. ,
[PubMed]
Rucker
,
J. J.
,
Jelen
,
L. A.
,
Flynn
,
S.
,
Frowde
,
K. D.
, &
Young
,
A. H.
(
2016
).
Psychedelics in the treatment of unipolar mood disorders: A systematic review
.
Journal of Psychopharmacology
,
30
(
12
),
1220
1229
. ,
[PubMed]
Schaefer
,
A.
,
Kong
,
R.
,
Gordon
,
E. M.
,
Laumann
,
T. O.
,
Zuo
,
X.-N.
,
Holmes
,
A. J.
,
Eickhoff
,
S. B.
, &
Yeo
,
B. T. T.
(
2018
).
Local-global parcellation of the human cerebral cortex from intrinsic functional connectivity MRI
.
Cerebral Cortex
,
28
(
9
),
3095
3114
. ,
[PubMed]
Smigielski
,
L.
,
Scheidegger
,
M.
,
Kometer
,
M.
, &
Vollenweider
,
F. X.
(
2019
).
Psilocybin-assisted mindfulness training modulates self-consciousness and brain default mode network connectivity with lasting effects
.
NeuroImage
,
196
,
207
215
. ,
[PubMed]
Stampacchia
,
S.
,
Asadi
,
S.
,
Tomczyk
,
S.
,
Ribaldi
,
F.
,
Scheffler
,
M.
,
Lövblad
,
K.-O.
,
Pievani
,
M.
,
Frisoni
,
G. B.
,
Garibotto
,
V.
, &
Amico
,
E.
(
2023
).
Fingerprinting of brain disease: Connectome identifiability in cognitive decline and Alzheimer’s disease
.
bioRxiv
.
Stenbæk
,
D. S.
,
Madsen
,
M. K.
,
Ozenne
,
B.
,
Kristiansen
,
S.
,
Burmester
,
D.
,
Erritzoe
,
D.
,
Knudsen
,
G. M.
, &
Fisher
,
P. M.
(
2021
).
Brain serotonin 2A receptor binding predicts subjective temporal and mystical effects of psilocybin in healthy humans
.
Journal of Psychopharmacology
,
35
(
4
),
459
468
. ,
[PubMed]
Studerus
,
E.
,
Gamma
,
A.
, &
Vollenweider
,
F. X.
(
2010
).
Psychometric evaluation of the altered states of consciousness rating scale (OAV)
.
PLOS ONE
,
5
(
8
),
e12412
. ,
[PubMed]
Tagliazucchi
,
E.
,
Carhart-Harris
,
R.
,
Leech
,
R.
,
Nutt
,
D.
, &
Chialvo
,
D. R.
(
2014
).
Enhanced repertoire of brain dynamical states during the psychedelic experience
.
Human Brain Mapping
,
35
(
11
),
5442
5456
. ,
[PubMed]
Van De Ville
,
D.
,
Farouj
,
Y.
,
Preti
,
M. G.
,
Liégeois
,
R.
, &
Amico
,
E.
(
2021
).
When makes you unique: Temporality of the human brain fingerprint
.
Science Advances
,
7
(
42
),
eabj0751
. ,
[PubMed]
Váša
,
F.
, &
Mišić
,
B.
(
2022
).
Null models in network neuroscience
.
Nature Reviews Neuroscience
,
23
(
8
),
493
504
. ,
[PubMed]
Vohryzek
,
J.
,
Cabral
,
J.
,
Castaldo
,
F.
,
Sanz-Perl
,
Y.
,
Lord
,
L.-D.
,
Fernandes
,
H. M.
,
Litvak
,
V.
,
Kringelbach
,
M. L.
, &
Deco
,
G.
(
2023
).
Dynamic sensitivity analysis: Defining personalised strategies to drive brain state transitions via whole brain modelling
.
Computational and Structural Biotechnology Journal
,
21
,
335
345
. ,
[PubMed]
Vollenweider
,
F. X.
, &
Geyer
,
M. A.
(
2001
).
A systems model of altered consciousness: Integrating natural and drug-induced psychoses
.
Brain Research Bulletin
,
56
(
5
),
495
507
. ,
[PubMed]
Vollenweider
,
F. X.
,
Vollenweider-Scherpenhuyzen
,
M. F.
,
Bäbler
,
A.
,
Vogel
,
H.
, &
Hell
,
D.
(
1998
).
Psilocybin induces schizophrenia-like psychosis in humans via a serotonin-2 agonist action
.
Neuroreport
,
9
(
17
),
3897
3902
. ,
[PubMed]
Yeo
,
B. T. T.
,
Krienen
,
F. M.
,
Sepulcre
,
J.
,
Sabuncu
,
M. R.
,
Lashkari
,
D.
,
Hollinshead
,
M.
,
Roffman
,
J. L.
,
Smoller
,
J. W.
,
Zöllei
,
L.
,
Polimeni
,
J. R.
,
Fischl
,
B.
,
Liu
,
H.
, &
Buckner
,
R. L.
(
2011
).
The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity
.
Journal of Neurophysiology
,
106
(
3
),
1125
1165
. ,
[PubMed]

Competing Interests

Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Author notes

Handling Editor: Olaf Sporns

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. For a full description of the license, please visit https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/legalcode.

Supplementary data