Hearing one’s own speech allows for acoustic self-monitoring in real time. Left-hemisphere motor planning regions are thought to give rise to efferent predictions that can be compared to true feedback in sensory cortices, resulting in neural suppression commensurate with the degree of overlap between predicted and actual sensations. Sensory prediction errors thus serve as a possible mechanism of detection of deviant speech sounds, which can then feed back into corrective action, allowing for online control of speech acoustics. The goal of this study was to assess the integrity of this detection–correction circuit in persons with aphasia (PWA) whose left-hemisphere lesions may limit their ability to control variability in speech output. We recorded magnetoencephalography (MEG) while 15 PWA and age-matched controls spoke monosyllabic words and listened to playback of their utterances. From this, we measured speaking-induced suppression of the M100 neural response and related it to lesion profiles and speech behavior. Both speaking-induced suppression and cortical sensitivity to deviance were preserved at the group level in PWA. PWA with more spared tissue in pars opercularis had greater left-hemisphere neural suppression and greater behavioral correction of acoustically deviant pronunciations, whereas sparing of superior temporal gyrus was not related to neural suppression or acoustic behavior. In turn, PWA who made greater corrections had fewer overt speech errors in the MEG task. Thus, the motor planning regions that generate the efferent prediction are integral to performing corrections when that prediction is violated.

Sensory feedback is central to motor control, as it delivers information about the consequences of our actions. During speech production, hearing one’s own voice allows for acoustic self-monitoring in real time. Some disorders of speech production are thought to derive, in part, from failures of monitoring and concomitantly poor auditory-motor integration (e.g., stuttering: Bradshaw et al., 2021; Cai et al., 2012; Daliri & Max, 2015; Parkinson’s disease: Huang et al., 2016; Mollaei et al., 2013; Railo et al., 2020; vocal hyperfunction: Abur et al., 2021). In post-stroke aphasia, the classic behavioral measure of auditory-motor disruption is speech repetition, where errors reflect a breakdown of the mapping from afferent speech input (the auditory target) to motor-articulatory representations (Hickok & Poeppel, 2004). The idea that the left-hemisphere lesions that cause aphasia may also damage the sensorimotor network for speech feedback processing is suggested by altered auditory feedback studies, in which persons with aphasia (PWA) make slower and/or smaller adjustments in response to externally applied pitch perturbations (Behroozmand et al., 2018; Behroozmand et al., 2022; Johnson et al., 2020). While these laboratory assays help to pinpoint the anatomical and functional bases of speech motor impairments, they fall short of addressing how PWA achieve, or fail to achieve, accuracy in their own volitional speech—in which the auditory target is speaker-internal (i.e., unlike a repetition task) and the feedback is veridical (i.e., unlike an altered auditory feedback paradigm). As such, the current study examines the relationships among acoustic behavior, surviving neural architecture, and the cortical processing of sensory feedback resulting from one’s own speech.

Aphasic speech is often errorful, containing distorted pronunciations and perceived phonemic substitutions, omissions, and additions. Even in correct utterances, PWA tend to be more acoustically variable than their neurologically intact peers (Haley et al., 2001; Niziolek & Kiran, 2018; Ryalls, 1986). While variability alone might not be substantially detrimental to communication, there is an emerging understanding that some perceived errors stem from uncontrolled phonetic variability. That is, some phonemic paraphasias that appear to occur at the level of phonological encoding (Parsons et al., 1988) contain trace phonetic effects consistent with failures of online detection and correction of deviant acoustics during the course of articulation (Kurowski & Blumstein, 2016; also see Buckingham & Yule, 1987). These findings suggest that the “lower-order” sensorimotor network, in addition to the “higher-order” language network, may be affected in aphasia. Further, sensorimotor impairment in aphasia could be at the level of feedforward or feedback control. Analyses of acoustic variability can help to distinguish these etiologies: Feedforward impairments may manifest in greater variability at syllable onset, reflecting abnormalities in top-down motor commands; feedback impairments may manifest in less online corrective action while speaking (Niziolek & Kiran, 2018). Specifically, typical speakers’ non-errorful speech is characterized by a phenomenon called vowel centering, in which productions that are initially somewhat off-target show “centering” toward the speaker’s median formant values over the course of the syllable (Niziolek et al., 2013). Moreover, there is evidence that this reduction of natural acoustic variability is reduced under masking noise (Niziolek et al., 2015; but cf. Parrell et al., 2021), highlighting the role of auditory feedback in preventing small deviations from becoming full-blown speech errors. In pilot data collected for the present study, PWA did exhibit vowel centering behavior, partially overcoming their initial feedforward variability (Niziolek & Kiran, 2018). Nevertheless, it is not well understood what surviving neural mechanisms subserve the detection and correction of deviations from intended speech sounds in PWA.

In typical brains, sensory feedback (reafference) is processed differently from externally generated sensory input (afference). Motor cortex is thought to send an internal signal to sensory cortices, enabling prediction of the motor act’s sensory consequences. This predictive signaling has been referred to as corollary discharge (Sperry, 1950) and efference copy (von Holst & Mittelstaedt, 1950). When the efferent prediction is a good match to the true sensory input, the prediction error is small. Correspondingly, human and mammalian neurophysiology reveals that the responses of certain neural populations are suppressed during self-produced vocalization versus passive listening to the same stimuli (Creutzfeldt et al., 1989; Eliades & Wang, 2003; Flinker et al., 2010; Houde et al., 2002; Müller-Preuss & Ploog, 1981), thus providing a general mechanism for distinguishing between self-generated (with an efferent prediction) and external (without an efferent prediction) sounds. When measured during speech production with respect to a passive listening condition, there is some evidence that this speaking-induced suppression (SIS) is left-lateralized (Houde et al., 2002; Niziolek et al., 2013). Critical for our purposes, however, is evidence that SIS is not all-or-none, but rather graded by the degree of match between prediction and input (e.g., Behroozmand & Larson, 2011) and related to adaptive change in response to a mismatch (Kim et al., 2023). We have previously shown that left-hemisphere SIS similarly shows a graded sensitivity to the “goodness” of a given utterance (Niziolek et al., 2013; Tang et al., 2023); that is, the neural response to off-target productions (at the periphery of a speaker’s distribution) is less suppressed than that to on-target productions (at the center of that distribution), reflecting a greater prediction error at the periphery. This fall-off of SIS from center to periphery indicates that deviant productions are detected by the brain. The aforementioned behavioral centering of vowel acoustics indicates that such productions are corrected. The purpose of the current study was to assess the integrity of this detection–correction circuit in PWA whose left-hemisphere lesions may disrupt the motor efferent prediction, the feedback comparison in auditory cortex, and/or the online control of speech.

Speech motor control is accomplished by large-scale networks across the cerebral cortex, the cerebellum, and subcortical structures, comprising feedforward and feedback control systems (Perkell, 2012; Simonyan et al., 2016; Tremblay et al., 2016). All participants with aphasia in this study had damage from a stroke in the left middle cerebral artery, but their diverse lesion patterns within its territory (Tatu et al., 1998) permitted us to relate lesion profiles to subcomponents of speech motor control. We adopted an anatomical region of interest (ROI) approach to interrogate a putative origin and target of the efferent prediction: the left inferior frontal gyrus pars opercularis (IFO) and the left superior temporal gyrus (STG), respectively (e.g., Hickok, 2012; Rauschecker & Scott, 2009). IFO is one of several adjacent motor planning regions, upstream of primary motor cortex, implicated in speech articulation (Blank et al., 2002; Fridriksson et al., 2016). Various instantiations of the DIVA (directions into velocities of articulators) model of speech production have placed the speech sound map, representing the sounds that speakers intend to produce, in IFO and/or adjacent ventral premotor cortex (Guenther, 2006; Guenther & Vladusich, 2012; Miller & Guenther, 2021; Tourville & Guenther, 2011). Activity in IFO precedes the suppression of sensory-evoked responses (Wang et al., 2014); therefore, IFO may be functionally analogous to the murine premotor area that has been shown to suppress ipsilateral auditory cortical activity (Schneider et al., 2014; for a review of nonhuman primate work in this area, see Eliades & Wang, 2019). STG, in addition to housing auditory cortex that performs spectrotemporal computations on speech input (Bhaya-Grossman & Chang, 2022; Hickok & Poeppel, 2007), contains the neural populations that respond to mismatches between efferent predictions and that input (Meekings & Scott, 2021; Niziolek & Guenther, 2013; Tourville et al., 2008). Moreover, overt versus silent articulation strengthens the functional connectivity between IFO and posterior STG, perhaps reflecting the engagement of auditory feedback mechanisms for error detection and correction (Zhang et al., 2023). In line with this, Behroozmand and colleagues (2018) found that post-stroke damage to the left inferior frontal and superior temporal gyri impaired compensation for perturbations to vocal pitch feedback. Left-hemisphere IFO and STG therefore served as our two ROIs for predicting the functioning of the detection–correction circuit for volitional speech production in aphasia.

We used magnetoencephalography (MEG) to measure SIS of the peak auditory cortical response, the M100, in order to characterize the efferent prediction circuit for speech and assess its integrity in post-stroke aphasia. We first compared the magnitude of SIS, an indicator of prediction delivery, among intact hemispheres (left and right in the control group, and right in PWA) and the damaged left hemispheres of PWA. We then related individual lesion profiles to SIS and to speech behavior, where vowel centering reflects feedback-guided motor control, and overt speech errors reflect failures of feedforward planning and/or online correction. Finally, taking advantage of natural, trial-to-trial variability in pronunciation, we compared the SIS of productions near the center of the vowel-formant distribution with the SIS of less typical productions at its periphery; we interpret systematic differences in SIS as an index of cortical sensitivity to the typicality of self-produced acoustics. In a secondary aim, we explored whether left-hemisphere damage was associated with atypical patterns of response lateralization across the two hemispheres. By using SIS as an objective neural measure of the efferent prediction, and considering vowel centering and speech accuracy as evidence of successful speech deviance detection and correction, we aimed to make progress on characterizing the underlying pathology of speech deficits in aphasia.

Participants

Fifteen persons with aphasia (PWA) and 15 age-matched controls took part in the experiment. All PWA were right-handed, monolingual speakers of American English and had chronic aphasia subsequent to a single stroke of the left middle cerebral artery. Individuals who were unable to undergo magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), had lesioned subcortical tissue, and/or had a major neurological or psychiatric diagnosis other than stroke were excluded from the study. A lesion overlap map for the aphasia group, showing the distribution of damaged voxels across the brain, is provided in Figure 1. PWA were 11 males and 4 females who ranged in age from 46 to 63 years. On average, they were 108 months post-stroke (SD = 111, range = 19–470). Clinical diagnoses were of Broca’s aphasia (n = 5) and anomic aphasia (n = 10). Three PWA (n = 1 Broca’s; n = 2 anomic) had a comorbid diagnosis of apraxia of speech, a disorder in which the capacity to program the movements of the articulators is impaired (Ziegler, 2008). See Table 1 for a full characterization.

Figure 1.

Lesion overlap map. Lesion overlap map for n = 15 PWA displayed on an MNI (Montreal Neurological Institute) template brain. Warmer colors indicate a higher frequency of lesioned tissue at a given voxel.

Figure 1.

Lesion overlap map. Lesion overlap map for n = 15 PWA displayed on an MNI (Montreal Neurological Institute) template brain. Warmer colors indicate a higher frequency of lesioned tissue at a given voxel.

Close modal
Table 1.

Characteristics of the aphasia group

PWAAge (years)SexTime post-stroke (months)Total lesion volume (mm3)IFO spared (%)STG spared (%)Heschl’s spared (%)SubtypeWAB AQPALPA7 (%)PALPA8 (%)PAL7 (%)Speech errors in the MEG task
OmissionsMisselectionsDistortionsOther wordsTotal
s01 58 49 56664 96 39 27 anomic 97 100.0 76.7 87.5 
s02 58 48 73912 100 51 anomic 82 95.8 66.7 70.0 
s03 52 141 265360 17 Broca’s + apraxia 64 73.3 43.3 72.5 19 22 61 
s04 51 85 285800 28 16 Broca’s 36 87.5 53.3 55.0 37 72 125 
s05 51 89 50464 95 94 81 anomic 95 95.8 96.7 95.0 
s06 57 163 193304 62 33 Broca’s 50 91.7 53.3 65.0 104 25 58 189 
s07 54 32 80520 100 63 77 anomic 85 87.5 43.3 40.0 16 21 40 
s08 56 29 70424 100 39 anomic 95 100.0 66.7 57.5 
s09 55 117 217520 24 anomic 92 100.0 86.7 80.0 
s10 52 85 76792 75 29 12 anomic + apraxia 86 100.0 86.7 92.5 35 24 55 124 
s11 63 177 136464 42 45 21 Broca’s 73 100.0 73.3 75.0 24 48 70 149 
s12 58 62 107872 14 89 71 anomic 85 83.3 63.3 72.5 27 61 10 50 149 
s13 46 49 43648 44 91 86 anomic + apraxia 94 62.5 53.3 50.0 
s14 53 470 119968 20 29 22 anomic 91 n/a 80.0 n/a 23 15 15 58 
s15 62 19 95424 59 48 32 Broca’s 56 100.0 3.3 52.5 92 10 21 137 
PWAAge (years)SexTime post-stroke (months)Total lesion volume (mm3)IFO spared (%)STG spared (%)Heschl’s spared (%)SubtypeWAB AQPALPA7 (%)PALPA8 (%)PAL7 (%)Speech errors in the MEG task
OmissionsMisselectionsDistortionsOther wordsTotal
s01 58 49 56664 96 39 27 anomic 97 100.0 76.7 87.5 
s02 58 48 73912 100 51 anomic 82 95.8 66.7 70.0 
s03 52 141 265360 17 Broca’s + apraxia 64 73.3 43.3 72.5 19 22 61 
s04 51 85 285800 28 16 Broca’s 36 87.5 53.3 55.0 37 72 125 
s05 51 89 50464 95 94 81 anomic 95 95.8 96.7 95.0 
s06 57 163 193304 62 33 Broca’s 50 91.7 53.3 65.0 104 25 58 189 
s07 54 32 80520 100 63 77 anomic 85 87.5 43.3 40.0 16 21 40 
s08 56 29 70424 100 39 anomic 95 100.0 66.7 57.5 
s09 55 117 217520 24 anomic 92 100.0 86.7 80.0 
s10 52 85 76792 75 29 12 anomic + apraxia 86 100.0 86.7 92.5 35 24 55 124 
s11 63 177 136464 42 45 21 Broca’s 73 100.0 73.3 75.0 24 48 70 149 
s12 58 62 107872 14 89 71 anomic 85 83.3 63.3 72.5 27 61 10 50 149 
s13 46 49 43648 44 91 86 anomic + apraxia 94 62.5 53.3 50.0 
s14 53 470 119968 20 29 22 anomic 91 n/a 80.0 n/a 23 15 15 58 
s15 62 19 95424 59 48 32 Broca’s 56 100.0 3.3 52.5 92 10 21 137 

PWA = persons with aphasia; IFO = inferior frontal gyrus par opercularis; STG = superior temporal gyrus; WAB AQ = Western Aphasia Battery Aphasia Quotient (Kertesz, 2007); PALPA7 = Psycholinguistic Assessments of Language Processing in Aphasia word repetition (Kay et al., 1992); PALPA8 = PALPA nonword repetition (Kay et al., 1992); PAL7 = Psycholinguistic Assessments of Language word and nonword repetition (Waters & Caplan, 1995); MEG = magnetoencephalography.

The control group was composed of 15 adults who were recruited to match each PWA on sex and age ±6 years. These individuals reported no history of speech, hearing, or language disorder and were also right-handed, native speakers of English. These 11 males and 4 females ranged in age from 43 to 69 years.

Because the study was concerned with auditory feedback processing, pure-tone audiometry air-conduction thresholds at 250, 500, 1000, 2000, and 4000 Hz were obtained to characterize hearing abilities in both PWA and controls. There were no significant group differences in thresholds in either ear at any of the frequencies tested (all ps > 0.12; Figure S1 in the Supporting Information available at https://doi.org/10.1162/nol_a_00139). Two individuals in each group self-reported tinnitus.

Participants provided written and verbal informed consent to all study procedures, which were approved and overseen by the Institutional Review Boards of Boston University’s Charles River Campus and the Massachusetts Institute of Technology.

Procedure

The study consisted of two visits for each PWA and one visit for each control participant. At the first visit, PWA completed standardized assessments and performed a behavioral-only version of the MEG speaking task to ensure that they could read and articulate the stimulus words and sustain attention for the length of the task. Some of these feasibility data have been previously reported (Niziolek & Kiran, 2018). The neuroimaging visit, scheduled on a separate day, included MEG acquisition followed by MRI scanning. All acoustic data reported here are from the speaking task performed in the MEG scanner during the second visit, as described below.

Standardized assessments

The speech and language abilities of PWA were assessed using the Western Aphasia Battery (WAB-R; Kertesz, 2007), the Psycholinguistic Assessments of Language Processing in Aphasia (PALPA7 word repetition and PALPA8 nonword repetition; Kay et al., 1992), and the Psycholinguistic Assessments of Language (PAL7 word and nonword repetition; Waters & Caplan, 1995). The Aphasia Quotient of the WAB-R, a 0–100 measure in which lower scores indicate greater global severity, ranged in this group from 36 to 97 (Table 1); the lowest five scores were from the individuals with Broca’s aphasia and the highest ten scores were from the individuals with anomic aphasia. All PWA made errors in the repetition of words and/or nonwords during standardized assessments (Table 1), making them a suitable population in which to test hypotheses about the neural bases of speech error detection and correction.

MEG speaking task

Participants read aloud the words “eat,” “Ed,” and “add” (chosen to elicit the vowels /i/, /ε/, and /æ/) while hearing their own speech played back in real time (speak condition). These simple vowel-initial words were chosen to minimize jaw movement that could produce artifacts in the MEG signal and to elicit numerous correct productions from impaired speakers. Words were projected on the screen one at a time in white on a black background for 1,000 ms with a random intertrial interval between 750 and 1,500 ms. Participants were instructed to speak clearly while minimizing head and jaw movement. Fifty randomized tokens of each of the three words were presented, with a short break provided after every 30 trials. After the speak block, participants saw the same stimuli in the same order with the same timing and heard their prior productions played back to them (listen condition). Thus, the listen condition provided the same visual and air-conducted acoustic stimulation as the speak condition, absent the motor activity. There were four runs of the experiment, for a total of 600 speak trials and 600 listen trials (200 for each of “eat,” “Ed,” and “add”). The interleaving of speaking and listening helped with engagement throughout the task, which lasted approximately 45 min. In addition, researchers communicated with participants via intercom during the breaks between speak and listen blocks, approximately every 5 min.

In a few cases, the paradigm was adjusted to the abilities of PWA as determined in the first visit. Participant s03 was given a longer intertrial interval in order to minimize apparent fatigue. Participant s04 had difficulty reading the words when they were randomized and instead saw the same word presented on every trial within a block. Participant s06 had difficulty reading “Ed” and so saw only “eat” and “add” during the experiment, completing 450 speak and 450 listen trials.

MEG acquisition

MEG was acquired during the task on an Elekta Triux scanner located in a quiet, magnetically shielded room. Whole-head coverage was obtained with 306 channels (102 magnetometers and 204 planar gradiometers). Signals were recorded at a sampling rate of 1000 Hz and filtered online between 0.03 and 330 Hz. Continuous head position measurements were collected from five coils affixed to the scalp. These coil locations, in addition to three anatomical landmarks (nasion and left and right preauricular points) and about 100 points on the scalp and face, were digitized using a Polhemus Fastrak system (Colchester, VT) and used to co-register the MEG and MRI scans.

Participants’ speech was captured at 11025 Hz on a Shure SM93 lavalier microphone modified for MEG compatibility and a Focusrite Scarlett 2i2 sound card. The audio was split, routed to both the participant and an input channel on the MEG scanner. Playback during both speak and listen conditions was delivered to the participant without delay or manipulation via insert earphones (Etymotic, Oak Grove Village, IL) and matched in sound level across the two conditions. The scanner’s audio channel had the same sampling rate as the MEG signal, allowing for precise time alignment of neural recording and auditory stimulation.

MRI acquisition

Subsequent to the MEG session, a T1-weighted anatomical MRI scan (voxel size 1 × 1 × 1 mm; field of view 256 mm; 176 slices; repetition time 2,530 ms) was obtained on a 3T Siemens Magnetom TrioTim scanner for use in estimating the neural sources of the MEG signals, and, in PWA, characterizing lesions.

Analysis

Acoustic analysis

The acoustics of each spoken word were analyzed in order to measure trial-to-trial variability in production and real-time adjustments over the course of each syllable. First, errorful trials were discarded from analysis. A research assistant listened to a recording of each production, viewed the corresponding word stimulus, and marked it for rejection if it contained a perceptible error, including an omission (no vocal response), a misselection (e.g., saying “eat” for “add”), a severe vowel distortion, a production of any other word outside the stimulus set (e.g., “head,” “had”) (Table 1), or any audible artifact. The remaining trials therefore included a range of pronunciations that were nonetheless perceived as correct by an unfamiliar listener. For these trials, trajectories of the first and second formant frequencies (F1 and F2) were tracked using wave_viewer (Niziolek & Houde, 2015), a Matlab interface to Praat software (Boersma & Weenink, 2019). The onset and offset of voicing were detected with an automated intensity threshold and, when necessary, manually corrected after visual inspection of periodicity in the waveform and spectrogram. Formants during the voiced syllable were estimated with linear predictive coding (LPC). The LPC order and pre-emphasis were selected to achieve stable tracking on a per-participant and per-vowel basis. Implausible values or discontinuities in the trajectories were corrected by adjusting these two values on a per-trial basis. Formants were calculated in Hz and converted to the logarithmic mel scale to accord with human perceptual sensitivity.

Time windows for formant averaging were defined as in prior studies (Niziolek et al., 2013; Niziolek & Kiran, 2018). The initial time window, from 0 (sound onset) to 50 ms, reflected the feedforward speech motor command for vowel onset. The mid-utterance time window consisted of the middle 50% of each formant trajectory, reflecting the vowel’s steady state—with sufficient time for the incorporation of auditory feedback into ongoing production. Previous studies have reported latencies of online compensation for real-time formant perturbations on the order of 150 to 160 ms (Cai et al., 2012; Caudrelier & Rochet-Capellan, 2019); adjustments for externally applied speech perturbation and speaker-internal variability are thought to rely on similar preconscious auditory feedback processing mechanisms (Munhall et al., 2009; Niziolek et al., 2013; Niziolek & Guenther, 2013).

We created trial subsets using formant values in the initial time window (0–50 ms) such that this acoustic information would precede, and therefore could contribute to, the M100 neural response. Specifically, we defined “center” trials and “peripheral” trials with respect to each participant’s median F1 and F2 values for each vowel (i.e., the center of its distribution). For each production of a given vowel, we calculated the Euclidean distance to the median in two-dimensional F1–F2 space; center trials were the n = 125 with the smallest initial distances, and peripheral trials were the n = 125 with the largest initial distances (see also MEG Analysis, below). Two key acoustic metrics, variability and centering, were defined with respect to this median. Variability was calculated as the average Euclidean distance, in mels, to the median in each time window, such that a greater distance indicates a broader distribution of formants across repeated productions of the same vowel. To determine whether speakers correct trials that are initially off-target, we calculated the centering of peripheral trials, subtracting their mid-utterance variability from their initial variability, representing the reduction in variability, in mels, from the beginning to the middle of the vowel (see Figure 2BC in Results).

To ensure that centering was not simply due to random measurement error, we performed the same verification as in Niziolek and Kiran (2018), that is, we compared the movement over time of peripheral trials (as defined above) with the movement of a different subset of trials defined as peripheral when the order of the two time windows was reversed. Specifically, this latter subset was identified as the trials with the largest mid-utterance distances, and we calculated their change in distance from mid-utterance backwards to the initial window. If centering merely reflects regression to the mean, then we would expect the movement of extreme trials to be statistically equivalent whether measured forwards or backwards in time, as indicated by a nonsignificant effect of time direction in an analysis of variance (ANOVA) comparing centering with time-reversed centering.

MEG preprocessing

Raw data were processed with Maxfilter software (Elekta, Stockholm) to remove external noise and correct for head movement. Subsequent processing was conducted in Brainstorm (Tadel et al., 2011). MEG sensors were co-registered to the participant’s MRI using six anatomical landmarks and digitized head points. Artifactual time segments were marked by hand and rejected from the continuous data. Eye-blink and cardiac artifacts were removed using signal-space projection. To reduce the influence of speaking-related movement artifacts, the data were band-pass filtered between 4 and 40 Hz, similar to the parameters in other MEG studies of speech production (Curio et al., 2000; Heinks-Maldonado et al., 2006). In some participants, low-frequency artifacts consistent with jaw opening and closing were evident in the sensor data even after filtering. In these cases, the topography of the artifact was used as an additional signal-space projector.

Sound onsets were automatically detected from the amplitude of the audio channel; onsets that immediately followed a visual-stimulus trigger were defined as speak and listen trial events (time = 0 at sound onset). The continuous data were epoched with respect to sound onset from −700 to 400 ms and baseline corrected between −700 and −400 ms to avoid contaminating the baseline with preparatory movement. Trials without an exact match between speak and listen conditions were excluded from analysis: For example, the corresponding listen trial was excluded for every speak trial on which the participant made an error, and the corresponding speak trial was excluded for every listen trial on which there was a severe MEG artifact. Thus, speak and listen conditions were fully matched in terms of acoustics. Out of a possible 600 total acoustic stimuli (200 for each of the three words), an average of 558 remained for controls (SD = 48, range = 440–600) and 486 for PWA (SD = 86, range = 257–594). This difference was significant, t(28) = 2.81, p = 0.01.

Neural sources were estimated from the MEG sensor data using minimum-norm imaging. The noise covariance was derived from the baselines (−700 to −400 ms) of all of the participant’s usable speak and listen trials. The forward model was computed by modeling the head as a set of overlapping spheres. Current density maps were then computed across 15,000 dipoles modeled as perpendicular to the cortical surface.

MRI preprocessing and lesion analysis

For all participants, the cortical surface was reconstructed from the T1 structural scan with Freesurfer (Fischl, 2012), enabling the estimation of neural sources using individual anatomy in Brainstorm as described above.

For PWA, lesion volumes and the percentage of spared tissue in anatomical ROIs were obtained in a separate processing pipeline. First, brain lesions were segmented in ITK-SNAP 4.0.0 (www.itksnap.org; Yushkevich et al., 2006), using a semiautomated method in which a Random Forests classifier was trained with six tissue labels (lesion [low intensity], lesion [medium intensity], gray matter, white matter, cerebrospinal fluid, bone) placed throughout the T1 scan. The classifier took as features the intensities of each voxel and its neighbors, as well as voxels’ coordinates, in order to distinguish the lesion from all other types of background tissue. Consistent with previous studies (Meier et al., 2019), enlarged ventricles were excluded from the lesion maps. Segmentation was then guided by an active contour algorithm that was seeded in the approximate center of mass of the lesion and expanded into regions where the posterior probability of the lesion was greater than that of the background (Yushkevich et al., 2016).

Subsequent processing was performed in SPM12 (https://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm12/). The lesion was incorporated into the segmentation procedure by defining it as a tissue type in SPM’s tissue probability map. Warping was performed to the ICBM European brain template. The structural scan and the lesion map were then each normalized from native to MNI space, with the former using fourth-degree B-spline interpolation and the latter using nearest-neighbor interpolation to preserve its binary format.

Anatomical regions of interest

Anatomical ROIs were defined using the AAL atlas (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002) with the MarsBAR toolbox (Brett et al., 2002). The AAL atlas, based on a normalized brain in MNI space, has been used in numerous other studies of aphasia (Behroozmand et al., 2022; Billot et al., 2022; Shah-Basak et al., 2020; Yourganov et al., 2015). In this atlas, the superior temporal gyrus is bounded by the Sylvian fissure and the superior temporal sulcus. It excludes the transverse (Heschl’s) gyrus but includes the planum temporale. The inferior frontal gyrus is bounded by the inferior frontal sulcus and the precentral sulcus. Its pars opercularis is bounded rostrally by the anterior ascending Sylvian ramus and caudally by the precentral sulcus (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002). For a post-hoc analysis in which we attempted to explain neural amplitudes in terms of primary auditory cortical tissue and hearing ability, we additionally used Heschl’s gyrus as an ROI.

As in previous studies (Johnson et al., 2019; Sims et al., 2016), calculations were performed on normalized lesion maps and brain volumes. Overlap between the patient’s lesion map and the left-hemisphere anatomical ROI was deleted and the resulting difference volume was divided by the total volume of the ROI per the AAL atlas, yielding the percent spared of each ROI. The total lesion volume, in mm3, was also used as a predictor of speech, language, and neural responses.

MEG analysis

The key neural metric was the peak amplitude of the M100 auditory-evoked response. Owing to the diversity of individual profiles in this study, identification of the M100 was performed manually for each participant. First, the sensor average of all listen trials and its scalp topography was examined to identify the latency of the peak auditory response. We looked for the characteristic three-peak complex of the M50, M100, and M200 (analogous to the P1, N1, and P2 event-related potentials measured using electroencephalography) in combination with the dipolar scalp topography of the auditory response. At the time of the M100 peak, we selected the highest-amplitude vertex on the lateral surface of the brain as the seed for a functional ROI in source space. We grew this seed to a size of 10 adjacent vertices (approximately 1 to 2 cm2), restricted to vertices whose amplitude was at least 33% of the maximum amplitude across the cortex at that latency. When necessary, this amplitude threshold was reduced by one percentage point until 10 adjacent above-threshold vertices could be captured. This procedure was repeated in the left and right hemispheres, producing two functional ROIs per participant. The locations of these ROIs are shown as projections on the ICBM cortical surface template (see Figure 3B in Results). All seeds in intact hemispheres fell within perisylvian and/or temporal cortex. In lesioned hemispheres, careful efforts were made to identify the M100 in spared perisylvian and/or temporal cortex depending on the individual’s lesion profile; therefore, once projected onto the intact template brain, the left-hemisphere functional ROIs for the PWA show less spatial clustering. (For further discussion, see Piastra et al., 2022.)

To obtain one time series per participant per hemisphere, we took the root mean square of the 10 vertices’ time series for the average response to all trials in the speak and listen conditions separately. Following Niziolek and colleagues (2013), we z-scored each time series with respect to the baseline interval of −700 to −400 ms. The M100 peak of these waveforms was then confirmed by visual inspection and established the peak latency and peak amplitude of each response. The difference in amplitude between the two conditions (listen − speak) yielded the metric of SIS, expressed as a z-score.

To examine the effect of acoustic typicality on neural responses, these procedures were repeated for center and peripheral subsets of speak and listen trials. Specifically, from among the trials with usable MEG data, the 125 speak trials with formants closest to the F1–F2 median, sampling the three vowels equally, were defined as the speakcenter trials, and the 125 corresponding listen trials were defined as the listencenter trials. The speakperiph and listenperiph trial types were defined in the same way, using the 125 trials with formants farthest from the F1–F2 median. The previously reported reduction in SIS for peripheral trials (listenperiph − speakperiph) as compared to center trials (listencenter − speakcenter) is termed SIS fall-off and can be interpreted as cortical sensitivity to acoustic typicality within a vowel’s formant distribution. The number 125 was chosen so that the number of center and peripheral trials analyzed would be consistent across participants; this was limited by the participant with the fewest trials remaining after error and artifact rejection (n = 257). However, analysis of SIS fall-off was ultimately limited by the signal quality obtained during MEG recording. We could not perform the intended analysis because we could not reliably identify the M100 peak in each participant’s waveform with only 125 trials contributing to the average. Instead, we report a comparison of the center-most half and the peripheral-most half of trials. (The tradeoff for more trial data is that this median split compares trials that abut one another in acoustic vowel space, decreasing the acoustic difference and therefore the hypothesized neural difference between them. And, paradoxically, including more trials potentially exacerbates the issue of data loss in PWA who made many speech errors: these individuals had fewer trials in their center half and peripheral half, making their average evoked response less reliable than that of a participant with all 600 trials available.)

Lateralization of neural responses was evaluated using laterality indices, ranging from −1 (fully left-lateralized) to +1 (fully right-lateralized), and calculated as (right − left) / (right + left). The latency laterality index indicated the degree to which an individual’s peak latency of the listen-evoked M100 was imbalanced between their left and right hemispheres. The amplitude laterality index indicated the degree to which their listen-evoked M100 peak amplitude was imbalanced, and the SIS laterality index indicated the degree to which their magnitude of SIS was imbalanced. To prevent extreme values (Jansen et al., 2006), and consistent with our interest in neural suppression specifically, negative SIS values (i.e., speaking-induced enhancement) were replaced with 0 (zero suppression) for the purposes of computing laterality. It was necessary to exclude the n = 2 controls who showed enhancement in both hemispheres from the SIS laterality analysis because the denominator of the index was 0.

Hearing thresholds

We analyzed the contribution of hearing thresholds to neural responses evoked by speech using the pure-tone average (PTA), computed as the mean of each participant’s 500-, 1000-, and 2000-Hz thresholds (dB HL) obtained from the pure-tone audiometry in the left and right ears separately.

Statistical analysis

Neural responses were tested via type III sum-of-squares ANOVA. Peak latencies and peak amplitudes of the M100 were analyzed with fixed factors of Group (control, aphasia), Hemisphere (left, right), and Condition (speak, listen) and—because of the repeated measures of Hemisphere and Condition within each participant—with a random factor of subject nested within Group. The magnitude of suppression (SIS) across all experimental trials was analyzed with fixed factors of Group and Hemisphere and with a random factor of subject nested within Group. In order to determine the presence of SIS fall-off from center to periphery, we analyzed each hemisphere separately, including only the participants who had shown SIS in that hemisphere (as these were the only participants in which we could evaluate our hypothesis) across all experimental trials. Therefore, the left-hemisphere test included n = 11 controls and n = 14 PWA, and the right-hemisphere test included n = 11 controls and n = 13 PWA. These two ANOVAs had fixed factors of Group and Trial Type (center, periphery) and a random factor of subject nested within Group. For completeness, this analysis was followed up with an ANOVA including all participants (even those without SIS), with fixed factors of Group, Hemisphere, and Trial Type and a random factor of subject nested within Group. For each ANOVA described above, the significance level was 0.05 and the full model (all main-effect and interaction terms) was specified. The effect of subject was significant in all models; these random effects were omitted from the results for brevity.

Associations among variables were computed with Spearman correlation when the relationship involved one or more bounded variables (e.g., percent of tissue spared in an anatomical ROI). In all other cases, Pearson correlation was used. We assessed whether the control and aphasia groups differed on measures of variability, centering, hearing thresholds, and laterality indices by conducting two-tailed, two-sample t tests. All reported p values are uncorrected.

PWA and Controls Center Their Peripheral Vowels to Reduce Variability

We measured formant variability across all correct productions of “eat,” “Ed,” and “add” during the MEG experiment (e.g., Figure 2A) and found that, in contrast with prior findings (Niziolek & Kiran, 2018), PWA did not have greater initial variability than controls, neither for all trials in the experiment (t(28) = −1.59, p = 0.12) nor for the peripheral subset of trials (t(28) = −1.11, p = 0.28).

Figure 2.

Formant variability and vowel centering. (A) Formant variability at vowel onset across repeated utterances of the stimulus words “eat,” “Ed,” and “add” by a representative person with aphasia (PWA) (s09). Green dots indicate center trials (n = 125 closest to the median vowel) and red circles indicate peripheral trials (n = 125 farthest from the median vowel). (B) Centering of the vowel /i/ in peripheral trials by a representative PWA (s09). Formants tend to move toward the median F1 and F2 values, here normalized to (0,0), from the first 50 ms (open red circles) to the middle 50% of the utterance (red arrows), thereby reducing variability. The circle radii represent the mean distance to the median in these two time windows (dashed: first 50 ms; solid: middle 50%); the difference between the radii is the amount of centering. (C) Schematic of vowel centering analysis. At left, a spectrogram of a single trial with formants F1 and F2 tracked. At center, these formants are compared with the median for this speaker and vowel, with the distance between them, measured within an initial and a mid-utterance time window, highlighted in gold. At right, the distance from the median is plotted in 2D formant space. The median for both time windows is normalized to (0,0). The open circle and arrowhead indicate formant values in initial and mid-utterance time windows, respectively. Centering is calculated as the difference between the initial and mid-utterance distances. (D) Correlation between initial variability and centering. Participants with greater initial variability in peripheral trials made greater within-vowel corrective movements in those trials. The least-squares line is computed across all participants. Orange diamonds indicate control participants and purple circles indicate PWAs. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals.

Figure 2.

Formant variability and vowel centering. (A) Formant variability at vowel onset across repeated utterances of the stimulus words “eat,” “Ed,” and “add” by a representative person with aphasia (PWA) (s09). Green dots indicate center trials (n = 125 closest to the median vowel) and red circles indicate peripheral trials (n = 125 farthest from the median vowel). (B) Centering of the vowel /i/ in peripheral trials by a representative PWA (s09). Formants tend to move toward the median F1 and F2 values, here normalized to (0,0), from the first 50 ms (open red circles) to the middle 50% of the utterance (red arrows), thereby reducing variability. The circle radii represent the mean distance to the median in these two time windows (dashed: first 50 ms; solid: middle 50%); the difference between the radii is the amount of centering. (C) Schematic of vowel centering analysis. At left, a spectrogram of a single trial with formants F1 and F2 tracked. At center, these formants are compared with the median for this speaker and vowel, with the distance between them, measured within an initial and a mid-utterance time window, highlighted in gold. At right, the distance from the median is plotted in 2D formant space. The median for both time windows is normalized to (0,0). The open circle and arrowhead indicate formant values in initial and mid-utterance time windows, respectively. Centering is calculated as the difference between the initial and mid-utterance distances. (D) Correlation between initial variability and centering. Participants with greater initial variability in peripheral trials made greater within-vowel corrective movements in those trials. The least-squares line is computed across all participants. Orange diamonds indicate control participants and purple circles indicate PWAs. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals.

Close modal

Every participant in the study demonstrated centering of peripheral trials, decreasing their average deviance over the course of the syllable (e.g., Figure 2BC). Across participants, peripheral trials began, on average, 62 mels from the median vowel, a distance which dropped to 42 mels at mid-utterance. As in Niziolek and Kiran’s (2018) study, we confirmed that the movement of these peripheral trials over time was greater than would be expected by chance (main effect of time direction [see Materials and Methods]: F(1, 29) = 34.76, p < 0.01). The magnitude of this centering did not differ between control and aphasia groups (t(28) = −0.74, p = 0.47; M = 20 mels, SD = 7, range = 8–35). On average, centering reduced the initial dispersion of peripheral productions by 32% (SD = 9%, range = 14–55%).

In line with prior reports (Niziolek et al., 2013; Niziolek & Kiran, 2018), the magnitude of centering was positively correlated with the initial variability of peripheral trials (all participants: r = 0.65, p < 0.001; Figure 2D). In other words, the greater the initial spread of formant values, the greater the corrective movement. This relationship held in the control and aphasia groups separately, as well (r = 0.67, p = 0.01 and r = 0.62, p = 0.01, respectively). In Figure 2D, note that the n = 3 PWA with apraxia of speech fall in the middle of the group in terms of initial variability but are relatively low in terms of centering behavior.

Latencies of Neural Responses to Speaking and Listening

In order to evaluate the modulatory effect of an efferent prediction on auditory cortex, we first identified the peak cortical response to self-produced speech in the speak and listen conditions. An M100 evoked response was identified in the left and right hemispheres of each participant, both control and PWA. In Figure 3A, the locations of these individual peak responses (functional ROIs) are shown projected onto template brains showing the mean auditory-evoked cortical activity for each group and hemisphere, captured at the mean peak latency for that group and hemisphere in the listen condition. Response latencies were faster overall in the right hemisphere (main effect of hemisphere: F(1, 28) = 4.91, p = 0.03). By definition, the M100 should peak close to 100 ms after sound onset in each hemisphere, but there was a Hemisphere × Subject interaction (F(1, 28) = 4.48, p < 0.01), indicating that the effect of hemisphere on M100 latency differed among participants. As Figure 3B shows, this was driven by substantially longer latencies (above the unity line) in the lesioned left hemispheres of some PWA. Overall, however, latencies did not significantly differ by group (F(1, 28) = 2.67, p = 0.11). The latencies of listen- and speak-evoked responses were not significantly different (F(1, 28) = 0.02, p = 0.90), and there was no hemisphere × condition interaction (F(1, 28) = 1.41, p = 0.25).

Figure 3.

M100 neural responses identified in space and time in intact and lesioned hemispheres. (A) Location of functional regions of interest (ROIs) from which M100 source space data were extracted, projected onto a common cortical-surface template. Each color represents an individual participant. In intact hemispheres, source locations cluster around canonical auditory cortex in the superior temporal lobe. (In both control hemispheres, n = 13 ROIs fall in temporal cortex and n = 2 in parietal cortex. In both aphasic hemispheres, n = 10 ROIs fall in temporal cortex and n = 5 in parietal cortex.) In lesioned hemispheres, apparent dispersion of source locations reflects lesion heterogeneity across PWA as well as the unreliability of cortical projections not designed to account for lesion topology (Piastra et al., 2022). The cortical surface shows the average listen-evoked M100 activity at the mean peak latency for that group and hemisphere. (B) M100 peak latencies by condition and hemisphere. Lines connect the speak (red) and listen (blue) latencies for each participant. Dark markers indicate the group mean. Data points above the diagonal indicate longer M100 latencies in the left hemisphere, which characterizes some PWA.

Figure 3.

M100 neural responses identified in space and time in intact and lesioned hemispheres. (A) Location of functional regions of interest (ROIs) from which M100 source space data were extracted, projected onto a common cortical-surface template. Each color represents an individual participant. In intact hemispheres, source locations cluster around canonical auditory cortex in the superior temporal lobe. (In both control hemispheres, n = 13 ROIs fall in temporal cortex and n = 2 in parietal cortex. In both aphasic hemispheres, n = 10 ROIs fall in temporal cortex and n = 5 in parietal cortex.) In lesioned hemispheres, apparent dispersion of source locations reflects lesion heterogeneity across PWA as well as the unreliability of cortical projections not designed to account for lesion topology (Piastra et al., 2022). The cortical surface shows the average listen-evoked M100 activity at the mean peak latency for that group and hemisphere. (B) M100 peak latencies by condition and hemisphere. Lines connect the speak (red) and listen (blue) latencies for each participant. Dark markers indicate the group mean. Data points above the diagonal indicate longer M100 latencies in the left hemisphere, which characterizes some PWA.

Close modal

SIS of M100 Amplitude

Suppression of the M100 peak amplitude in the speak condition relative to the listen condition was the primary neural metric in this study. SIS was identified to a similar degree in both control and aphasia groups and in both left and right hemispheres (main effect of condition: F(1, 28) = 15.3, p < 0.01; Figure 4). Overall, amplitudes were lower in PWA (main effect of group: F(1, 28) = 6.77, p = 0.01), but surprisingly, this was not specific to the lesioned hemisphere. The effects of hemisphere and condition on amplitude varied across participants (F(1, 28) = 5.98, p < 0.01 and F(1, 28) = 7.5, p < 0.01, respectively).

Figure 4.

Speaking-induced suppression (SIS). (A) Group mean ± standard error of left-hemisphere magnetoencephalography (MEG) activity in listen (blue) and speak (red) conditions. Owing to the variation in M100 peak latencies across participants, individual traces have been aligned to acoustic onset (t = 0) and linearly scaled so that the M100 peak falls at t = 100. The full M50-M100-M200 complex is evident in intact hemispheres, but greatly diminished in the lesioned hemisphere. Note that the M50 may be obscured because of the variation in latencies. Although M100 amplitudes are larger in the control group, the magnitude of SIS does not significantly differ between control and aphasia. Error bars on the group-mean markers (dark orange, dark purple) represent 95% confidence intervals. (B) As in (A), right hemisphere. At far right, SIS in each group and hemisphere was evaluated with a one-sample t test: * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001.

Figure 4.

Speaking-induced suppression (SIS). (A) Group mean ± standard error of left-hemisphere magnetoencephalography (MEG) activity in listen (blue) and speak (red) conditions. Owing to the variation in M100 peak latencies across participants, individual traces have been aligned to acoustic onset (t = 0) and linearly scaled so that the M100 peak falls at t = 100. The full M50-M100-M200 complex is evident in intact hemispheres, but greatly diminished in the lesioned hemisphere. Note that the M50 may be obscured because of the variation in latencies. Although M100 amplitudes are larger in the control group, the magnitude of SIS does not significantly differ between control and aphasia. Error bars on the group-mean markers (dark orange, dark purple) represent 95% confidence intervals. (B) As in (A), right hemisphere. At far right, SIS in each group and hemisphere was evaluated with a one-sample t test: * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001.

Close modal

Despite group differences in amplitude, the magnitude of SIS, calculated as the difference between listen- and speak-evoked peak amplitudes, did not differ by group (F(1, 28) = 0.84, p = 0.37; Figure 4) or by hemisphere (F(1, 28) = 0.08, p = 0.78), nor was there an interaction (F(1, 28) = 0.19, p = 0.66). The lack of a group effect on SIS was evident even when we excluded individuals who showed amplitude enhancement (SIS < 0) rather than suppression in the speak condition (in both hemispheres: n = 2 control; in the left only: n = 2 control, n = 1 PWA; in the right only: n = 2 control, n = 2 PWA): with these exclusions, SIS trended larger but was not significantly different in control versus aphasia (left hemisphere: F(1, 23) = 3.75, p = 0.07; right hemisphere: F(1, 22) = 2.47, p = 0.13).

Spared left IFO predicts left SIS

We took advantage of the variation in lesion profiles among PWA to ask whether the sparing of the left IFO and/or the left STG, representing a putative source and target, respectively, of efferent suppression, was related to ipsilateral SIS. Because the sparing of an anatomical ROI predictor could be confounded with a smaller lesion overall (and therefore less severe aphasia: the smaller the lesion, the higher the WAB Aphasia Quotient, r = −0.70, p < 0.01), we additionally controlled for total lesion volume in these correlations.

Results indicated that the percent of IFO spared, ranging in this sample from 14% to 100%, was associated with the magnitude of SIS in the left hemisphere (ρ = 0.60, p = 0.02): the greater the percent spared, the greater the suppression (Figure 5A). We confirmed the specificity of the IFO–SIS relationship by controlling for total lesion volume; the correlation remained significant (partial ρ = 0.54, p = 0.0486). In contrast, the percent of STG spared, ranging in this sample from 4% to 94%, was not associated with the magnitude of left SIS (ρ = 0.17, p = 0.55; when controlling for total lesion volume: partial ρ = −0.10, p = 0.73).

Figure 5.

Inferior frontal gyrus pars opercularis (IFO) predicts neural suppression and behavioral correction of vowels; correction is associated with fewer overt errors. (A) Correlation of percent of IFO spared and magnitude of left-hemisphere speaking-induced suppression (SIS) in persons with aphasia (PWA). Circular markers containing an “×” indicate PWA with a comorbid diagnosis of apraxia of speech. (B) Correlation of percent of IFO spared and magnitude of centering of peripheral trials in PWA. (C) Correlation of centering and number of speech errors made by PWA. (D) Lesion profiles of representative PWA s08, s13, and s12.

Figure 5.

Inferior frontal gyrus pars opercularis (IFO) predicts neural suppression and behavioral correction of vowels; correction is associated with fewer overt errors. (A) Correlation of percent of IFO spared and magnitude of left-hemisphere speaking-induced suppression (SIS) in persons with aphasia (PWA). Circular markers containing an “×” indicate PWA with a comorbid diagnosis of apraxia of speech. (B) Correlation of percent of IFO spared and magnitude of centering of peripheral trials in PWA. (C) Correlation of centering and number of speech errors made by PWA. (D) Lesion profiles of representative PWA s08, s13, and s12.

Close modal

Spared left IFO predicts vowel centering but not initial variability

We next asked whether the sparing of IFO and/or STG was related to acoustic measures of motor control, namely, the formant variability measured at vowel onset and the degree to which peripheral trials were centered.

Results indicated that neither the percent of IFO spared nor the percent of STG spared were associated with initial variability, neither of peripheral trials nor of all trials (ps > 0.65).

IFO spared was marginally associated with centering (ρ = 0.51, p = 0.0508): the greater the percent spared, the greater the centering (Figure 5B). This relationship was significant (partial ρ = 0.68, p = 0.01) when jointly controlling for total lesion volume and initial peripheral variability, which we had found to predict centering (Figure 2D). In contrast, STG spared was not associated with centering (ρ = 0.16, p = 0.57).

Vowel Centering (and Spared Left IFO) Predict Speech Accuracy in PWA

Centering has been advanced as an objective acoustic marker of successful feedback processing, preventing deviant productions from veering across a phonetic boundary and being perceived as an error (Niziolek & Kiran, 2018). As such, we explored the relationship between centering and overt speech errors made by PWA. (The majority of control participants made either 0 or 1 errors during the MEG task (median = 1, M = 3.73, SD = 8.37), so we did not analyze speech errors in this group.)

We found that the greater the centering, the fewer the speech errors made during the MEG speaking task (r = −0.62, p = 0.01; Table 1; Figure 5C). This relationship remained significant when we did not include omissions (which could stem from attentional lapses or failure to initiate movement within the recording window) in the count of PWA’s speech errors (r = −0.57, p = 0.03).

Furthermore, there was a strong trend such that the number of speech errors was inversely correlated with IFO integrity (ρ = −0.50, p = 0.0572), but not with STG integrity (p = 0.64). This can be appreciated in Figure 5C, where larger marker sizes indicate more spared left IFO.

Centering was not, however, associated with speech accuracy as measured on standardized assessments of repetition (PAL7, PALPA7, PALPA8, or their average; ps > 0.49). Lesion profiles (IFO spared, STG spared, total lesion volume) also did not predict these standardized scores in our aphasia sample (ps > 0.23).

Representative Individual Data

The key findings of this study can be appreciated by considering the profiles of three representative participants with aphasia, illustrated in Figure 5D. In the full sample of PWA, lesions affecting IFO and STG were largely dissociable: The percent spared in each region was not correlated (ρ = 0.37, p = 0.18). Two participants clearly illustrate the dissociation: s08’s lesion is relatively inferior, leaving IFO 100% spared and STG 39% spared; in contrast, s12’s lesion is relatively superior, with IFO 14% spared and STG 89% spared. In another participant, s13, IFO is 44% spared and STG is 91% spared. The left-hemisphere SIS, centering, and number of speech errors for each of these three representative participants are labeled in Figure 5AC, respectively. For example, s08 has substantial left-hemisphere SIS, a large amount of centering, and few speech errors in the MEG task.

Left SIS falls off from center to periphery of the vowel distribution

Previous research using the same paradigm in typical speakers identified greater SIS in the center versus periphery of their formant distributions. This differential SIS was suggested to reflect neural sensitivity to natural variation in speech, such that peripheral trials showed a greater release from suppression—an effect that was specific to the left hemisphere (Niziolek et al., 2013). In our planned analyses, we intended to replicate and extend this finding by comparing SIS between center (n = 125) and peripheral (n = 125) trials in intact and lesioned hemispheres. However, we were limited by signal quality and in many participants could not reliably identify the M100 peak among low-level noise in the average waveforms for center and peripheral trial subsets. Instead, we took a median split and compared the magnitude of SIS in the center half of trials to that in the peripheral half, focusing on participants who, overall, showed suppression rather than enhancement in each hemisphere, and therefore in whom we could evaluate our hypothesis of differential SIS (left: n = 25 of 30; right: n = 24 of 30).

In the left hemisphere, there was significant SIS fall-off from the center half to the peripheral half (main effect of Trial Type: F(1, 23) = 5.35, p = 0.03; SIS-center M = 5.70, SD = 7.25; SIS-peripheral M = 3.28, SD = 5.09). There was no interaction with nor main effect of Group (ps > 0.13), indicating that comparable left-hemisphere modulation by prototypicality was observed in both control and aphasia (Figure 6). In contrast, the corresponding analysis in the right hemisphere found no such modulation of SIS (F(1, 22) = 0.48, p = 0.49). This relationship was dependent on the participants having SIS: that is, across all 30 participants, including those with enhancement rather than suppression in one or both hemispheres, an ANOVA testing SIS (with factors of Group, Hemisphere, Trial Type, and Subject) revealed only a Hemisphere × Subject interaction (F(1, 28) = 1.95, p = 0.04), no effect of Group (F(1, 28) = 2.53, p = 0.36), no effect of Hemisphere (F(1, 28) = 0.05, p = 0.86), and no significant effect of Trial Type (F(1, 28) = 0.33, p = 0.63).

Figure 6.

SIS is modulated by vowel typicality. Across all participants who showed suppression rather than enhancement, SIS measured in the center half of trials was greater than SIS measured in the peripheral half of trials. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals for each group mean. * p < 0.05.

Figure 6.

SIS is modulated by vowel typicality. Across all participants who showed suppression rather than enhancement, SIS measured in the center half of trials was greater than SIS measured in the peripheral half of trials. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals for each group mean. * p < 0.05.

Close modal

Although there was no group difference in the magnitude of SIS fall-off, we investigated whether, in the full aphasia group, it was related to lesion profiles. However, neither IFO spared nor STG spared were significantly associated with the magnitude of SIS fall-off, neither in the left (preserved) nor the right (potentially reorganized) hemisphere (ps > 0.28).

Our final planned analysis concerned the hypothesized link between reduced SIS in peripheral trials, indicating detection of deviant acoustics via prediction error, and centering, a corrective action. Niziolek and colleagues (2013) found that, in healthy speakers, the smaller a participant’s left SIS in peripheral trials, the greater their centering behavior, suggesting that the activity in sensory regions drives the corrective action. However, this relationship was not replicated in our full control sample (r = 0.19, p = 0.50). Across all PWA, centering was not related to peripheral SIS in either left (r = 0.40, p = 0.14) or right (r = 0.02, p = 0.94) hemispheres.

Unexplained variation in listen-evoked amplitudes

In this study, there was notably large variation in M100 amplitudes, even though they were measured as z scores with respect to participant-specific baseline (intertrial) intervals, when little cognitive, perceptual, or motor activity was expected. Listen-evoked peak amplitudes ranged from 1.82 to 45.06, with the median across groups and hemispheres falling at 7.80 (M = 11.61, SD = 10.81). As the listen-evoked M100 was the reference from which SIS was measured, we attempted to account for this variation. We considered the possibility that it could be explained by individuals’ numbers of usable MEG trials (where more trials should yield a stronger neural signal) and/or hearing thresholds (where lower thresholds at speech-related frequencies, i.e., the PTA) should yield a stronger neural signal, especially given the possibility of age-related hearing loss in our participants [M age = 56 years]). However, across all participants, the number of usable trials was not related to left or right amplitudes (ps > 0.64). Moreover, across all participants, PTAs in left and right ears were not related to peak amplitudes, neither ipsilateral nor contralateral (ps > 0.07).

Although PTAs did not differ between the control and aphasia groups in either ear (ps > 0.2206; Figure S1), we paid special attention to the right-ear PTA in the aphasia group, contralateral to the lesioned hemisphere, asking whether low auditory-evoked responses here could be attributed to peripheral hearing loss and/or cortical damage. We found that the higher the PTA, the lower the listen-evoked amplitude (without the outlier who had profound hearing loss in that ear: r = −0.74, p < 0.01; with all n = 15: r = −0.18, p = 0.52), suggesting that peripheral hearing loss could be contributing to attenuated responses in PWA. The sparing of canonical left auditory cortex (Heschl’s gyrus and STG) was not related to the right-ear PTA (ps > 0.60), so the measured thresholds likely reflected the integrity of the peripheral auditory system in these PWA. In fact, these auditory-cortex lesion profiles did not even predict the listen-evoked M100 amplitudes supposedly generated there (ps > 0.38).

Laterality of Neural Responses in PWA Compared to Controls

Although it was not the primary aim of our study, we took the opportunity to compute the lateralization of neural responses in the aphasia group and compare it to that obtained in an age-matched control sample. While we cannot infer a causal effect of left-hemisphere stroke on laterality given only a single, post-stroke time point, these data offer a snapshot of normative and patient populations. We explored whether lesion profiles were associated with the lateralization of responses, for example, spared tissue supporting typical lateralization or lesioned tissue driving atypical lateralization.

Latency

We found that listen-evoked M100 latencies did not differ by hemisphere in controls (t(14) = −0.87, p = 0.40), but they did in PWA (t(14) = 2.36, p = 0.03); relatedly, the latency laterality index, indicating a participant’s (im)balance between the hemispheres, significantly differed between the groups (t(28) = 2.60, p = 0.01), with longer latencies measured in the left hemispheres of PWA (Figure S2). In PWA, the latency laterality index was associated with IFO spared (ρ = 0.66, p = 0.01) and total lesion volume (ρ = −0.72, p < 0.01), but not with STG spared or Heschl’s gyrus spared (ps > 0.20). Consistent with disruption of left-hemisphere afferent pathways, more IFO spared was associated with a skew toward longer right-hemisphere latencies, and a greater total lesion volume was associated with a skew toward longer left-hemisphere latencies.

Amplitude

We found that listen-evoked M100 amplitudes did not differ by hemisphere, neither in controls (t(14) = −0.79, p = 0.44) nor in PWA (t(14) = −1.22, p = 0.24); relatedly, the amplitude laterality index did not differ between the groups (t(28) = −0.60, p = 0.55; Figure S2). Perhaps surprisingly, the amplitude laterality index in PWA was not associated with any lesion profile (IFO spared, STG spared, Heschl spared, total lesion volume; ps > 0.12), so it was not the case that (right-)lateralization of evoked amplitudes was explained by left-hemisphere damage.

SIS

We found that SIS magnitude did not differ by hemisphere, neither in all controls (t(14) = −0.52, p = 0.61) nor in all PWA (t(14) = 0.11, p = 0.92). The SIS laterality index did not differ between the groups (t(26) = −0.32, p = 0.75; Figure S2), and thus there was no evidence that left-hemisphere stroke had altered how SIS manifested by hemisphere. The SIS laterality index (favoring the left hemisphere) was associated with IFO spared (ρ = −0.55, p = 0.03) but not with the other lesion profiles (ps > 0.25).

Finally, for completeness, we noted that there were no significant correlations between any of the left-hemisphere lesion profiles (IFO spared, STG spared, Heschl spared, total lesion volume) and any of the right-hemisphere neural measures (latency, amplitude, SIS, SIS fall-off) (ps > 0.17) that could be related to functional reorganization after stroke.

Summary of Results

Suppression of the auditory-evoked neural response to one’s own speech is a hallmark of feedback processing. In participants with chronic aphasia and matched controls, we found that SIS was, at the group level, robust to damage to the left hemisphere. Moreover, despite overall low neural signal in the aphasia group, which we could not fully explain in terms of data quantity, hearing ability, or even lesioned tissue, we found that the magnitude of suppression in the left hemisphere was modulated by the acoustic typicality of the utterance, with less suppression measured in trials at the periphery of speakers’ vowel-formant distributions. This finding is consistent with a framework in which the efferent prediction reflects the sensory goal—the prototypical vowel—such that an atypical production generates a prediction error that is reflected in reduced suppression.

Within the aphasia group, we found that the left IFO was an important part of the feedback-processing circuit. PWA with more spared tissue in this region had greater neural suppression and greater within-syllable behavioral correction of atypical formants (vowel centering). Sparing of the left STG, conversely, was not related to either of these metrics. In turn, PWA who made greater corrective movements had fewer overt speech errors. These findings suggest that IFO is a source of the efferent signal and that its predictions underlie successful, feedback-based corrections of ongoing speech.

IFO Participates in SIS

The suppression of auditory reafference by frontal motor planning regions is consistent with a vast animal literature on corollary discharge (Crapse & Sommer, 2008). In particular, a direct motor-auditory connection is a structural feature of mammalian brains (Budinger & Scheich, 2009; Frey et al., 2008; Nelson et al., 2013; Reep et al., 1987), and, in the mouse, it is the secondary motor neurons that drive the suppression of auditory cortex before and during movement (Schneider et al., 2014). Human studies confirm that the source of the suppressive signal is upstream of primary motor cortex (Haggard & Whitford, 2004; Voss et al., 2006), thus linking the efferent signal to the motor plan rather than to the muscle command itself. Correspondingly, the putative plan-related signal precedes action onset in time: Source-localized EEG has revealed a correlate of the efference copy such that that activity in both left and right inferior frontal gyrus in the 300 ms prior to speaking is associated with suppression of the N1 response subsequently evoked in STG (Wang et al., 2014).

The ubiquitous phenomenon of motor-induced suppression has both general and plan-specific components. In the auditory system, general components attenuate response magnitudes within the central nervous system as well as at the auditory periphery, as early as the tympanic membrane (Carmel & Starr, 1963). Studies across mammalian species have reported auditory suppression related to general preparation and/or engagement (Li et al., 2020; Otazu et al., 2009; Zheng et al., 2022), and therefore, the SIS measured in our study likely encompasses a nonspecific suppressive phenomenon independent of forward prediction of linguistic content. However, an additional line of evidence suggests that another component of SIS is specific to the intended speech sound, above and beyond general attenuation during movement, and instantiates a prediction of specific acoustic consequences (Houde et al., 2002; Rummell et al., 2016; Schneider et al., 2018). First, the fact that some neurons shift their tuning in order to monitor for deviations from expected feedback (Eliades & Wang, 2008) indicates that the vocal content, not merely the act of vocalizing, is represented neurally. Second, suppression is reduced if feedback is artificially altered to create a mismatch between predicted and actual sensation (Behroozmand & Larson, 2011; Chang et al., 2013; Heinks-Maldonado et al., 2006; Whitford et al., 2017). We found that PWA with more spared tissue in IFO showed greater SIS, suggesting that regions involved in motor planning are critically involved in sending predictions of the sensory consequences of those plans and that this operation can be disrupted by left-hemisphere stroke.

We extend this framework—in which the specific prediction derives from the motor plan—to one in which the specific prediction is the speaker’s acoustic prototype for that speech sound. In this view, speakers’ peripheral productions are likened to errors and consequently show smaller suppression. We observed this effect across controls and PWA, and in the left but not the right hemisphere, replicating the pattern identified by Niziolek and colleagues (2013) and Tang and colleagues (2023) in younger typical speakers. This phenomenon of differential SIS therefore makes a strong case that the efferent prediction is specific, suitable for comparison with sensory reafference in auditory cortex as part of the detection–correction circuit.

It is notable that we did not find a relationship between the magnitude of SIS and the sparing of STG, given that the M100—the auditory-evoked field component by which we define SIS—is typically localized to this region in both hemispheres (Gage et al., 1998). One likely explanation is that the STG is heterogeneous, with only a subset of sites exhibiting SIS (Chang et al., 2013; Flinker et al., 2010); a lesion in part of the gyrus does not necessarily damage the specific neural populations that are suppressed during speaking.

IFO Participates in Vowel Centering

We found that PWA with more spared tissue in IFO showed greater vowel centering, bringing vowel formants from the periphery of the speaker-specific distribution toward the median, thereby reducing variability. This relationship held even when controlling for the amount of initial formant variability across their productions. In fact, our findings suggest that damage to IFO impairs the online control of speech variability without substantially increasing feedforward variability itself.

A relationship between IFO and corrective action is consistent with prominent models of speech production that include a cortical mechanism for incorporating feedback into online control. In the state feedback control model, this takes the form of processing state estimate corrections after sensory error in frontal premotor areas (Houde & Nagarajan, 2011). In the DIVA model, the feedback control map, also localized to premotor planning regions, transforms error into updated articulatory trajectories (Tourville & Guenther, 2011). These models are informed by neuroimaging results in which, for example, activity in superior temporal and inferior frontal areas during formant perturbations correlates with participants’ compensatory actions (Niziolek & Guenther, 2013). However, as noted above, the sparing of specific neural populations that encode auditory error (Chang et al., 2013) may help to explain why damage to STG was not associated with the magnitude of centering in our aphasia sample.

In the non-speaking mouse, homologous higher-order motor regions are recognized for their causal role in the flexible updating of motor plans subsequent to sensory feedback (Barthas & Kwan, 2017; Gremel & Costa, 2013). Causal evidence in humans is rare, but a recent study identified greater behavioral compensation and enhanced neural responses to artificially-altered pitch feedback as an after-effect of transcranial alternating current stimulation over the left inferior frontal gyrus (Li et al., 2023). Together, these lines of evidence suggest that the motor-planning regions that create the efferent prediction are integral to performing the correction when that prediction is violated.

It is important to note that several studies implicate ventral precentral gyrus more strongly than its rostral neighbor IFO in driving suppression and causing compensatory movements during speech. Specifically, connectivity analyses of human electrocorticography (ECoG) identified the ventral precentral gyrus as a source of the corollary-discharge signal, peaking prior to articulation and targeting STG (Khalilian-Gourtani et al., 2022). In that study, activity in inferior frontal gyrus preceded activity in precentral gyrus, but the stronger correlate of a motor-to-auditory discharge arose from precentral gyrus. In another ECoG study, compensation for vocal pitch perturbation was correlated with the activity of electrodes in posterior STG as well as ventral precentral gyrus, suggesting that corrective movements have premotor origins (Chang et al., 2013). In that study, most electrodes in IFO did not show enhanced activity in response to speech perturbation. However, IFO rather than precentral gyrus was our a priori frontal ROI in the present study not only because of its role in speech motor planning (Long et al., 2016; Mugler et al., 2018; Papoutsi et al., 2009; Rong et al., 2018), but also because of its anatomical specificity. In the aphasia-standard AAL parcellation used for calculating the percent spared of ROIs, precentral gyrus contains primary motor as well as premotor cortex and is therefore not suitable for interrogating specific contributions of motor planning regions to the detection–correction circuit. Given the reciprocal functional connections among frontal motor- and speech-related areas (Greenlee et al., 2004), we expect that IFO and ventral precentral gyrus both participate in updating speech trajectories during online control.

Finally, it is also worth underscoring that at the group level, PWA—a majority of whom had damage to left IFO—exhibited a significant degree of vowel centering, comparable to that of controls. One potential explanation for this result is that right-hemisphere premotor regions, which were fully spared in our sample of PWA, also support corrective behavior. Whereas speech motor programs tend to be left-lateralized (Ghosh et al., 2008) and error detection tends to be bilateral (Tourville et al., 2019), there is evidence that compensation for altered feedback is related to premotor activity in the right hemisphere, especially that which is driven by right posterior temporal cortex (Floegel et al., 2020; Liu et al., 2023; Tourville et al., 2008; Toyomura et al., 2007). Findings such as these ultimately led the developers of the DIVA model to place the feedback control map in the right hemisphere (Tourville & Guenther, 2011). Our study was designed to examine the left-hemisphere circuit because of the aphasic population we were working with, but future studies could evaluate whether right-hemisphere stroke is associated with greater impairment in the online control of speech, as the DIVA model might predict.

Vowel Centering Relates to Speech Accuracy

Vowel centering is one of a number of ways to measure speech motor control. This analysis quantifies speakers’ responses to their own natural variability and treats peripheral productions as error-like. We found that, in PWA, greater centering was associated with fewer overt speech errors, suggesting that it has functional consequences for successful production. Centering potentially prevents acoustic deviations from becoming full-blown speech errors, crossing a perceptual boundary and being perceived by listeners as a vowel misselection. This is the first demonstration of a relationship between centering, a subclinical variable, and speech accuracy, a highly salient variable. We therefore propose that centering may be a manifestation of successful feedback processing in natural speech.

Another way to assay feedback processing is to manipulate auditory feedback in real time as it is played back to the speaker. Behroozmand and colleagues (2018) found that PWA respond to external alterations of vowel pitch as controls do—by altering their true pitch to oppose the shift—but to a lesser degree. Diminished compensation was predicted by damage to left superior temporal, middle temporal, inferior frontal, and supramarginal gyri. The inferior frontal gyrus was implicated in the time window of the steepest slope of compensation, which the authors linked to the greatest need for corrective motor precision. Thus, left inferior frontal cortex has now been linked to corrective responses to both artificial pitch perturbations and naturally occuring peripheral vowels.

Like compensation for altered auditory feedback, centering theoretically relies upon the inverse model—the sensory-to-motor transformation that establishes how to move the articulators to produce the desired correction. However, there is an important distinction between the two metrics. (Indeed, in typical speakers, compensation and centering have not been shown to correlate; Niziolek & Parrell, 2021.) Altered auditory feedback paradigms (e.g., Houde & Jordan, 1998; Purcell & Munhall, 2006) manipulate auditory but not somatosensory feedback, such that speakers encounter a conflict between the two sensory modalities as well as the intended auditory prediction error. The centering analysis in the present study is used to characterize natural speech in which speakers receive veridical feedback, both auditory and somatosensory. A typical speaker might prefer to rely on one or the other (Lametti et al., 2012); a PWA might be limited by their surviving neural architecture. Whereas altered auditory feedback is very likely to drive an STG-dependent auditory error response, natural variability could be detected and corrected by means of somatosensory error. This may be one reason we did not observe a relationship between STG integrity and centering in natural speech.

Speech repetition tasks have also been used to tap the inverse model in aphasia. The ability to perceive and repeat a word, particularly a pseudoword, has been associated with larger compensation for altered feedback (Sangtian et al., 2021). We did not find a correlation between centering and standardized measures of speech repetition, which may reflect low power in our study. On the other hand, because the magnitude of centering did not differ between control and aphasia groups, it is reasonable that there would be no relationship between centering and aphasia-specific clinical assessments.

Apraxia of Speech

We paid special attention to the n = 3 PWA who had a diagnosis of apraxia of speech comorbid with aphasia because apraxia is, specifically, a motor speech disorder. Generalizability here is limited, but these individuals did not seem to drive or disrupt the relationships we found in the larger aphasia group in terms of neural suppression, motor control, or speech accuracy. Since this population tends to have lesions in left inferior frontal gyrus (Ballard et al., 2014), we might predict low neural suppression and low centering due to IFO damage. Empirical findings regarding feedforward and feedback control systems in apraxia of speech are, to date, relatively limited and somewhat inconclusive (Ballard et al., 2018; Maas et al., 2015). Yet it is commonly said that persons with apraxia “know what they want to say and how it should sound” (Ziegler, 2008, p. 276), leading them to make multiple attempts to produce intended words. This behavioral profile hints at preserved feedback-monitoring capabilities in the face of impaired motor programming. Such a dissociation—an ability to detect error but not to correct it—was not observed in the current small-scale study of a diverse aphasic population but is theoretically possible. It also underscores the important distinction between speech errors consciously perceived—or not perceived—by the affected individual (e.g., semantic errors, phonemic paraphasias) and subphonemic “errors” in vowel pronunciation that are corrected, preconsciously, by speech motor circuitry—the focus of the present study. As proposed by Niziolek and Kiran (2018), centering analysis may complement other techniques used by speech-language pathologists to evaluate disordered speech because it reveals ongoing, feedback-guided correction that is below the perceptual threshold.

Neural response profiles do not easily distinguish control and aphasic populations

Although PWA had lower auditory-evoked response magnitudes than controls, these low amplitudes were not confined to the damaged hemisphere, nor did they result in significantly smaller average magnitudes of suppression when comparing speak and listen conditions. This underscores a general pattern of nonsignificant differences at the group level, with greater variability within than across groups, and within than across hemispheres in the aphasia group. For example, we expected that listen-evoked responses would be delayed and weaker in the left versus the right hemispheres of PWA; however, while left-hemisphere responses were slower, they were not smaller in amplitude. This bilateral pattern of responsiveness ultimately made the PWA look much like controls, whose latencies, amplitudes, and SIS magnitudes, at the group level, did not differ by hemisphere (Figure S2). This contrasts with previous reports, in younger, typical speakers, of greater SIS in the left versus the right hemisphere (Curio et al., 2000; Houde et al., 2002; Niziolek et al., 2013). This heterogeneity in typical lateralization precludes attributing any lateralization observed in PWA to their stroke and/or recovery. For example, that the sparing of IFO predicted SIS laterality was interesting, but less so in light of having found no group difference in SIS laterality. All in all, there was very limited evidence for lesion effects on lateralization or right-hemisphere plasticity in these data.

Relatedly, lesioned tissue did not straightforwardly explain certain basic auditory response profiles in PWA. For example, the integrity of left STG and left Heschl’s gyrus did not predict hearing thresholds, confirming that tone detection survives substantial damage to these regions (Masterton & Berkley, 1974; see Table 1). However, the sparing of auditory cortex also showed no relationship with evoked amplitudes. While low neural signal in PWA was potentially partially explained by peripheral hearing loss, this relationship was less compelling in light of the fact that their hearing did not differ from controls’ (Figure S1). This unanticipated result could be addressed in future work providing a more comprehensive picture of surviving neural architecture in auditory cortex. Specifically, it would be desirable to have measures of sensory and perceptual acuity as a function of spared STG (e.g., Robson et al., 2012; Robson et al., 2013), as impairments in these domains could limit the resolution of the comparison between the efferent prediction and sensory reafference. In addition, tractography (e.g., Breier et al., 2008), functional connectivity (e.g., Schofield et al., 2012), and a finer anatomical parcellation of STG (e.g., the planum temporale; Hickok et al., 2009) would all help to elucidate the components of the cortical feedback circuit for speech in individuals with and without brain damage.

Caveats

The significant structure–function predictive relationships within the aphasia group (IFO–SIS and IFO–centering) are tempered by the fact that although PWA, as a group, had strictly less tissue in IFO, they did not show statistically less SIS or centering than controls. Thus, one could argue that the speech error detection–correction circuit is not necessarily impaired after left-hemisphere stroke—at least for the simple monosyllables used in the current study. Even with these simple stimuli, dysfunction relative to age-matched peers was evident in the number of errors made during a simple word-production task, but not in our subclinical acoustic measures of speech motor control (initial formant variability, a proxy for feedforward control, and centering, a proxy for feedback control), underscoring that profiles such as high speech variability and low online correction ability are not indicative of an aphasia diagnosis.

On the other hand, the lack of group difference in the magnitude of SIS, and the relationship between spared IFO and SIS, is complicated by the unexpected presence of speaking-induced enhancement in our data (i.e., SIS < 0 in Figure 4). Despite overwhelming evidence across species for neural suppression during self-vocalization (Creutzfeldt et al., 1989; Eliades & Wang, 2003; Müller-Preuss & Ploog, 1981; Suga & Shimozawa, 1974), a subset of participants in our study, both controls and PWA, demonstrated larger M100 amplitudes during speaking relative to listening. (While we expected to find SIS in all controls and did not, all PWA did show SIS in one or both hemispheres.) With only a single MEG session per participant, it is unclear whether being an enhancer versus a suppresser is a stable individual trait. Speaking-induced enhancement could potentially be attributed to individuals’ having particular cortical topography and/or a particular position in the MEG helmet that led to the recording of neural populations that are excited rather than suppressed by the act of vocalizing (Eliades & Wang, 2008). As MEG has limited spatial resolution in this regard, this is fully speculative. It is also possible that enhancement was the result of a particular cognitive or attentional state during speaking, as the M100 is obligatory but its amplitude does increase with attention (Woldorff et al., 1993). At present, it is unclear whether amplitude enhancement is functionally relevant for speaking.

We interrogated sensorimotor function in aphasia, focusing on efferent predictions of the sound of one’s own speech and the detection and correction of natural variability in vowel production. Post-stroke sparing of pars opercularis of the left inferior frontal gyrus was associated with greater speaking-induced neural suppression (an indicator of efferent prediction delivery), greater vowel centering (an indicator of feedback-guided correction), and fewer speech errors. These findings highlight the importance of pars opercularis, in speaking humans, to the efferent prediction circuit that has, to date, largely been studied in non-speaking animals. Rather than initiating them, pars opercularis appears to underlie the updating of motor plans for speech; our findings suggest that it creates the efferent prediction and also performs the correction when the prediction is violated.

We thank the staff of the Athinoula A. Martinos Imaging Center at the McGovern Institute for Brain Research (Massachusetts Institute of Technology) for technical support. We thank Isaac Falconer for assistance with the lesion analysis pipeline. Dr. Tang's work for this paper was conducted while the author was at the Waisman Center.

Caroline A. Niziolek, National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders (https://dx.doi.org/10.13039/100000055), Award ID: K99/R00DC014520.

Sara D. Beach: Formal analysis: Lead; Investigation: Equal; Methodology: Lead; Software: Equal; Visualization: Equal; Writing – original draft: Lead; Writing – review & editing: Equal. Ding-lan Tang: Methodology: Supporting; Writing – review & editing: Equal. Swathi Kiran: Conceptualization: Supporting; Resources: Equal; Supervision: Equal; Writing – review & editing: Equal. Caroline A. Niziolek: Conceptualization: Lead; Funding acquisition: Lead; Investigation: Equal; Project administration: Lead; Resources: Equal; Software: Equal; Supervision: Equal; Visualization: Equal; Writing – review & editing: Equal.

The data and code supporting this study are openly available at https://osf.io/hjwsx/.

Aphasia:

An acquired language disorder caused by brain damage.

Magnetoencephalography (MEG):

A functional brain-imaging technology that noninvasively records the magnetic fields generated by neurons’ electrical activity.

Apraxia of speech:

An acquired motor speech disorder in which the programming of the movements of the speech articulators is impaired.

Abur
,
D.
,
Subaciute
,
A.
,
Kapsner-Smith
,
M.
,
Segina
,
R. K.
,
Tracy
,
L. F.
,
Noordzij
,
J. P.
, &
Stepp
,
C. E.
(
2021
).
Impaired auditory discrimination and auditory-motor integration in hyperfunctional voice disorders
.
Scientific Reports
,
11
(
1
),
Article 13123
. ,
[PubMed]
Ballard
,
K. J.
,
Halaki
,
M.
,
Sowman
,
P.
,
Kha
,
A.
,
Daliri
,
A.
,
Robin
,
D. A.
,
Tourville
,
J. A.
, &
Guenther
,
F. H.
(
2018
).
An investigation of compensation and adaptation to auditory perturbations in individuals with acquired apraxia of speech
.
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience
,
12
,
Article 510
. ,
[PubMed]
Ballard
,
K. J.
,
Tourville
,
J. A.
, &
Robin
,
D. A.
(
2014
).
Behavioral, computational, and neuroimaging studies of acquired apraxia of speech
.
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience
,
8
,
Article 892
. ,
[PubMed]
Barthas
,
F.
, &
Kwan
,
A. C.
(
2017
).
Secondary motor cortex: Where “sensory” meets “motor” in the rodent frontal cortex
.
Trends in Neurosciences
,
40
(
3
),
181
193
. ,
[PubMed]
Behroozmand
,
R.
,
Bonilha
,
L.
,
Rorden
,
C.
,
Hickok
,
G.
, &
Fridriksson
,
J.
(
2022
).
Neural correlates of impaired vocal feedback control in post-stroke aphasia
.
NeuroImage
,
250
,
Article 118938
. ,
[PubMed]
Behroozmand
,
R.
, &
Larson
,
C. R.
(
2011
).
Error-dependent modulation of speech-induced auditory suppression for pitch-shifted voice feedback
.
BMC Neuroscience
,
12
,
Article 54
. ,
[PubMed]
Behroozmand
,
R.
,
Phillip
,
L.
,
Johari
,
K.
,
Bonilha
,
L.
,
Rorden
,
C.
,
Hickok
,
G.
, &
Fridriksson
,
J.
(
2018
).
Sensorimotor impairment of speech auditory feedback processing in aphasia
.
NeuroImage
,
165
,
102
111
. ,
[PubMed]
Bhaya-Grossman
,
I.
, &
Chang
,
E. F.
(
2022
).
Speech computations of the human superior temporal gyrus
.
Annual Review of Psychology
,
73
,
79
102
. ,
[PubMed]
Billot
,
A.
,
Lai
,
S.
,
Varkanitsa
,
M.
,
Braun
,
E. J.
,
Rapp
,
B.
,
Parrish
,
T. B.
,
Higgins
,
J.
,
Kurani
,
A. S.
,
Caplan
,
D.
,
Thompson
,
C. K.
,
Ishwar
,
P.
,
Betke
,
M.
, &
Kiran
,
S.
(
2022
).
Multimodal neural and behavioral data predict response to rehabilitation in chronic poststroke aphasia
.
Stroke
,
53
(
5
),
1606
1614
. ,
[PubMed]
Blank
,
S. C.
,
Scott
,
S. K.
,
Murphy
,
K.
,
Warburton
,
E.
, &
Wise
,
R. J.
(
2002
).
Speech production: Wernicke, Broca and beyond
.
Brain
,
125
(
8
),
1829
1838
. ,
[PubMed]
Boersma
,
P.
, &
Weenink
,
D.
(
2019
).
Praat: Doing phonetics by computer
. https://www.praat.org
Bradshaw
,
A. R.
,
Lametti
,
D. R.
, &
McGettigan
,
C.
(
2021
).
The role of sensory feedback in developmental stuttering: A review
.
Neurobiology of Language
,
2
(
2
),
308
334
. ,
[PubMed]
Breier
,
J. I.
,
Hasan
,
K. M.
,
Zhang
,
W.
,
Men
,
D.
, &
Papanicolaou
,
A. C.
(
2008
).
Language dysfunction after stroke and damage to white matter tracts evaluated using diffusion tensor imaging
.
American Journal of Neuroradiology
,
29
(
3
),
483
487
. ,
[PubMed]
Brett
,
M.
,
Anton
,
J. L.
,
Valabregue
,
R.
, &
Poline
,
J. B.
(
2002
).
Region of interest analysis using the MarsBar toolbox for SPM 99
.
NeuroImage
,
16
(
2
),
S497
.
Buckingham
,
H. W.
, &
Yule
,
G.
(
1987
).
Phonemic false evaluation: Theoretical and clinical aspects
.
Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics
,
1
(
2
),
113
125
.
Budinger
,
E.
, &
Scheich
,
H.
(
2009
).
Anatomical connections suitable for the direct processing of neuronal information of different modalities via the rodent primary auditory cortex
.
Hearing Research
,
258
(
1–2
),
16
27
. ,
[PubMed]
Cai
,
S.
,
Beal
,
D. S.
,
Ghosh
,
S. S.
,
Tiede
,
M. K.
,
Guenther
,
F. H.
, &
Perkell
,
J. S.
(
2012
).
Weak responses to auditory feedback perturbation during articulation in persons who stutter: Evidence for abnormal auditory-motor transformation
.
PLOS ONE
,
7
(
7
),
Article e41830
. ,
[PubMed]
Carmel
,
P. W.
, &
Starr
,
A.
(
1963
).
Acoustic and nonacoustic factors modifying middle-ear muscle activity in waking cats
.
Journal of Neurophysiology
,
26
(
4
),
598
616
. ,
[PubMed]
Caudrelier
,
T.
, &
Rochet-Capellan
,
A.
(
2019
).
Changes in speech production in response to formant perturbations: An overview of two decades of research
. In
S.
Fuchs
,
J.
Cleland
, &
A.
Rochet-Capellan
(Eds.),
Speech production and perception: Learning and memory
(pp.
15
75
).
Peter Lang
.
Chang
,
E. F.
,
Niziolek
,
C. A.
,
Knight
,
R. T.
,
Nagarajan
,
S. S.
, &
Houde
,
J. F.
(
2013
).
Human cortical sensorimotor network underlying feedback control of vocal pitch
.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
,
110
(
7
),
2653
2658
. ,
[PubMed]
Crapse
,
T. B.
, &
Sommer
,
M. A.
(
2008
).
Corollary discharge circuits in the primate brain
.
Current Opinion in Neurobiology
,
18
(
6
),
552
557
. ,
[PubMed]
Creutzfeldt
,
O.
,
Ojemann
,
G.
, &
Lettich
,
E.
(
1989
).
Neuronal activity in the human lateral temporal lobe: II. Responses to the subjects own voice
.
Experimental Brain Research
,
77
,
476
489
. ,
[PubMed]
Curio
,
G.
,
Neuloh
,
G.
,
Numminen
,
J.
,
Jousmäki
,
V.
, &
Hari
,
R.
(
2000
).
Speaking modifies voice-evoked activity in the human auditory cortex
.
Human Brain Mapping
,
9
(
4
),
183
191
. ,
[PubMed]
Daliri
,
A.
, &
Max
,
L.
(
2015
).
Modulation of auditory processing during speech movement planning is limited in adults who stutter
.
Brain and Language
,
143
,
59
68
. ,
[PubMed]
Eliades
,
S. J.
, &
Wang
,
X.
(
2003
).
Sensory-motor interaction in the primate auditory cortex during self-initiated vocalizations
.
Journal of Neurophysiology
,
89
(
4
),
2194
2207
. ,
[PubMed]
Eliades
,
S. J.
, &
Wang
,
X.
(
2008
).
Neural substrates of vocalization feedback monitoring in primate auditory cortex
.
Nature
,
453
(
7198
),
1102
1106
. ,
[PubMed]
Eliades
,
S. J.
, &
Wang
,
X.
(
2019
).
Corollary discharge mechanisms during vocal production in marmoset monkeys
.
Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging
,
4
(
9
),
805
812
. ,
[PubMed]
Fischl
,
B.
(
2012
).
FreeSurfer
.
NeuroImage
,
62
(
2
),
774
781
. ,
[PubMed]
Flinker
,
A.
,
Chang
,
E. F.
,
Kirsch
,
H. E.
,
Barbaro
,
N. M.
,
Crone
,
N. E.
, &
Knight
,
R. T.
(
2010
).
Single-trial speech suppression of auditory cortex activity in humans
.
Journal of Neuroscience
,
30
(
49
),
16643
16650
. ,
[PubMed]
Floegel
,
M.
,
Fuchs
,
S.
, &
Kell
,
C. A.
(
2020
).
Differential contributions of the two cerebral hemispheres to temporal and spectral speech feedback control
.
Nature Communications
,
11
(
1
),
Article 2839
. ,
[PubMed]
Frey
,
S.
,
Campbell
,
J. S.
,
Pike
,
G. B.
, &
Petrides
,
M.
(
2008
).
Dissociating the human language pathways with high angular resolution diffusion fiber tractography
.
Journal of Neuroscience
,
28
(
45
),
11435
11444
. ,
[PubMed]
Fridriksson
,
J.
,
Yourganov
,
G.
,
Bonilha
,
L.
,
Basilakos
,
A.
,
Den Ouden
,
D. B.
, &
Rorden
,
C.
(
2016
).
Revealing the dual streams of speech processing
.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
,
113
(
52
),
15108
15113
. ,
[PubMed]
Gage
,
N.
,
Poeppel
,
D.
,
Roberts
,
T. P.
, &
Hickok
,
G.
(
1998
).
Auditory evoked M100 reflects onset acoustics of speech sounds
.
Brain Research
,
814
(
1–2
),
236
239
. ,
[PubMed]
Ghosh
,
S. S.
,
Tourville
,
J. A.
, &
Guenther
,
F. H.
(
2008
).
A neuroimaging study of premotor lateralization and cerebellar involvement in the production of phonemes and syllables
.
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research
,
51
(
5
),
1183
1202
. ,
[PubMed]
Greenlee
,
J. D.
,
Oya
,
H.
,
Kawasaki
,
H.
,
Volkov
,
I. O.
,
Kaufman
,
O. P.
,
Kovach
,
C.
,
Howard
,
M. A.
, &
Brugge
,
J. F.
(
2004
).
A functional connection between inferior frontal gyrus and orofacial motor cortex in human
.
Journal of Neurophysiology
,
92
(
2
),
1153
1164
. ,
[PubMed]
Gremel
,
C. M.
, &
Costa
,
R. M.
(
2013
).
Premotor cortex is critical for goal-directed actions
.
Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience
,
7
,
Article 110
. ,
[PubMed]
Guenther
,
F. H.
(
2006
).
Cortical interactions underlying the production of speech sounds
.
Journal of Communication Disorders
,
39
(
5
),
350
365
. ,
[PubMed]
Guenther
,
F. H.
, &
Vladusich
,
T.
(
2012
).
A neural theory of speech acquisition and production
.
Journal of Neurolinguistics
,
25
(
5
),
408
422
. ,
[PubMed]
Haggard
,
P.
, &
Whitford
,
B.
(
2004
).
Supplementary motor area provides an efferent signal for sensory suppression
.
Cognitive Brain Research
,
19
(
1
),
52
58
. ,
[PubMed]
Haley
,
K. L.
,
Ohde
,
R. N.
, &
Wertz
,
R. T.
(
2001
).
Vowel quality in aphasia and apraxia of speech: Phonetic transcription and formant analyses
.
Aphasiology
,
15
(
12
),
1107
1123
.
Heinks-Maldonado
,
T. H.
,
Nagarajan
,
S. S.
, &
Houde
,
J. F.
(
2006
).
Magnetoencephalographic evidence for a precise forward model in speech production
.
NeuroReport
,
17
(
13
),
1375
1379
. ,
[PubMed]
Hickok
,
G.
(
2012
).
Computational neuroanatomy of speech production
.
Nature Reviews Neuroscience
,
13
(
2
),
135
145
. ,
[PubMed]
Hickok
,
G.
,
Okada
,
K.
, &
Serences
,
J. T.
(
2009
).
Area Spt in the human planum temporale supports sensory-motor integration for speech processing
.
Journal of Neurophysiology
,
101
(
5
),
2725
2732
. ,
[PubMed]
Hickok
,
G.
, &
Poeppel
,
D.
(
2004
).
Dorsal and ventral streams: A framework for understanding aspects of the functional anatomy of language
.
Cognition
,
92
(
1–2
),
67
99
. ,
[PubMed]
Hickok
,
G.
, &
Poeppel
,
D.
(
2007
).
The cortical organization of speech processing
.
Nature Reviews Neuroscience
,
8
(
5
),
393
402
. ,
[PubMed]
Houde
,
J. F.
, &
Jordan
,
M. I.
(
1998
).
Sensorimotor adaptation in speech production
.
Science
,
279
(
5354
),
1213
1216
. ,
[PubMed]
Houde
,
J. F.
, &
Nagarajan
,
S. S.
(
2011
).
Speech production as state feedback control
.
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience
,
5
,
Article 82
. ,
[PubMed]
Houde
,
J. F.
,
Nagarajan
,
S. S.
,
Sekihara
,
K.
, &
Merzenich
,
M. M.
(
2002
).
Modulation of the auditory cortex during speech: An MEG study
.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
,
14
(
8
),
1125
1138
. ,
[PubMed]
Huang
,
X.
,
Chen
,
X.
,
Yan
,
N.
,
Jones
,
J. A.
,
Wang
,
E. Q.
,
Chen
,
L.
,
Guo
,
Z.
,
Li
,
W.
,
Liu
,
P.
, &
Liu
,
H.
(
2016
).
The impact of Parkinson’s disease on the cortical mechanisms that support auditory-motor integration for voice control
.
Human Brain Mapping
,
37
(
12
),
4248
4261
. ,
[PubMed]
Jansen
,
A.
,
Menke
,
R.
,
Sommer
,
J.
,
Förster
,
A. F.
,
Bruchmann
,
S.
,
Hempleman
,
J.
,
Weber
,
B.
, &
Knecht
,
S.
(
2006
).
The assessment of hemispheric lateralization in functional MRI—Robustness and reproducibility
.
NeuroImage
,
33
(
1
),
204
217
. ,
[PubMed]
Johnson
,
J. P.
,
Meier
,
E. L.
,
Pan
,
Y.
, &
Kiran
,
S.
(
2019
).
Treatment-related changes in neural activation vary according to treatment response and extent of spared tissue in patients with chronic aphasia
.
Cortex
,
121
,
147
168
. ,
[PubMed]
Johnson
,
L. P.
,
Sangtian
,
S.
,
Johari
,
K.
,
Behroozmand
,
R.
, &
Fridriksson
,
J.
(
2020
).
Slowed compensation responses to altered auditory feedback in post-stroke aphasia: Implications for speech sensorimotor integration
.
Journal of Communication Disorders
,
88
,
Article 106034
. ,
[PubMed]
Kay
,
J.
,
Lesser
,
R.
, &
Coltheart
,
M.
(
1992
).
PALPA: Psycholinguistic assessments of language processing in aphasia
.
Lawrence Erlbaum
.
Kertesz
,
A.
(
2007
).
Western Aphasia Battery–Revised
.
Psych Corp
.
Khalilian-Gourtani
,
A.
,
Wang
,
R.
,
Chen
,
X.
,
Yu
,
L.
,
Dugan
,
P.
,
Friedman
,
D.
,
Doyle
,
W.
,
Devinsky
,
O.
,
Wang
,
Y.
, &
Flinker
,
A.
(
2022
).
A corollary discharge circuit in human speech
.
BioRxiv
.
Kim
,
K. S.
,
Hinkley
,
L. B.
,
Dale
,
C. L.
,
Nagarajan
,
S. S.
, &
Houde
,
J. F.
(
2023
).
Neurophysiological evidence of sensory prediction errors driving speech sensorimotor adaptation
.
BioRxiv
.
Kurowski
,
K.
, &
Blumstein
,
S. E.
(
2016
).
Phonetic basis of phonemic paraphasias in aphasia: Evidence for cascading activation
.
Cortex
,
75
,
193
203
. ,
[PubMed]
Lametti
,
D. R.
,
Nasir
,
S. M.
, &
Ostry
,
D. J.
(
2012
).
Sensory preference in speech production revealed by simultaneous alteration of auditory and somatosensory feedback
.
Journal of Neuroscience
,
32
(
27
),
9351
9358
. ,
[PubMed]
Li
,
S.
,
Zhu
,
H.
, &
Tian
,
X.
(
2020
).
Corollary discharge versus efference copy: Distinct neural signals in speech preparation differentially modulate auditory responses
.
Cerebral Cortex
,
30
(
11
),
5806
5820
. ,
[PubMed]
Li
,
T.
,
Chang
,
Y.
,
Zhao
,
S.
,
Jones
,
J. A.
,
Chen
,
X.
,
Gan
,
C.
,
Wu
,
X.
,
Dai
,
G.
,
Li
,
J.
,
Shen
,
Y.
,
Liu
,
P.
, &
Liu
,
H.
(
2023
).
The left inferior frontal gyrus is causally linked to vocal feedback control: Evidence from high-definition transcranial alternating current stimulation
.
Cerebral Cortex
,
33
(
9
),
5625
5635
. ,
[PubMed]
Liu
,
D.
,
Chang
,
Y.
,
Dai
,
G.
,
Guo
,
Z.
,
Jones
,
J. A.
,
Li
,
T.
,
Chen
,
X.
,
Chen
,
M.
,
Li
,
J.
,
Wu
,
X.
,
Liu
,
P.
, &
Liu
,
H.
(
2023
).
Right, but not left, posterior superior temporal gyrus is causally involved in vocal feedback control
.
NeuroImage
,
278
,
Article 120282
. ,
[PubMed]
Long
,
M. A.
,
Katlowitz
,
K. A.
,
Svirsky
,
M. A.
,
Clary
,
R. C.
,
Byun
,
T. M.
,
Majaj
,
N.
,
Oya
,
H.
,
Howard
,
M. A.
, &
Greenlee
,
J. D.
(
2016
).
Functional segregation of cortical regions underlying speech timing and articulation
.
Neuron
,
89
(
6
),
1187
1193
. ,
[PubMed]
Maas
,
E.
,
Mailend
,
M. L.
, &
Guenther
,
F. H.
(
2015
).
Feedforward and feedback control in apraxia of speech: Effects of noise masking on vowel production
.
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research
,
58
(
2
),
185
200
. ,
[PubMed]
Masterton
,
R. B.
, &
Berkley
,
M. A.
(
1974
).
Brain function: Changing ideas on the role of sensory, motor, and association cortex in behavior
.
Annual Review of Psychology
,
25
,
277
312
. ,
[PubMed]
Meekings
,
S.
, &
Scott
,
S. K.
(
2021
).
Error in the superior temporal gyrus? A systematic review and activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of speech production studies
.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
,
33
(
3
),
422
444
. ,
[PubMed]
Meier
,
E. L.
,
Johnson
,
J. P.
,
Pan
,
Y.
, &
Kiran
,
S.
(
2019
).
The utility of lesion classification in predicting language and treatment outcomes in chronic stroke-induced aphasia
.
Brain Imaging and Behavior
,
13
(
6
),
1510
1525
. ,
[PubMed]
Miller
,
H. E.
, &
Guenther
,
F. H.
(
2021
).
Modelling speech motor programming and apraxia of speech in the DIVA/GODIVA neurocomputational framework
.
Aphasiology
,
35
(
4
),
424
441
. ,
[PubMed]
Mollaei
,
F.
,
Shiller
,
D. M.
, &
Gracco
,
V. L.
(
2013
).
Sensorimotor adaptation of speech in Parkinson’s disease
.
Movement Disorders
,
28
(
12
),
1668
1674
. ,
[PubMed]
Mugler
,
E. M.
,
Tate
,
M. C.
,
Livescu
,
K.
,
Templer
,
J. W.
,
Goldrick
,
M. A.
, &
Slutzky
,
M. W.
(
2018
).
Differential representation of articulatory gestures and phonemes in precentral and inferior frontal gyri
.
Journal of Neuroscience
,
38
(
46
),
9803
9813
. ,
[PubMed]
Müller-Preuss
,
P.
, &
Ploog
,
D.
(
1981
).
Inhibition of auditory cortical neurons during phonation
.
Brain Research
,
215
(
1–2
),
61
76
. ,
[PubMed]
Munhall
,
K. G.
,
MacDonald
,
E. N.
,
Byrne
,
S. K.
, &
Johnsrude
,
I.
(
2009
).
Talkers alter vowel production in response to real-time formant perturbation even when instructed not to compensate
.
Journal of the Acoustical Society of America
,
125
(
1
),
384
390
. ,
[PubMed]
Nelson
,
A.
,
Schneider
,
D. M.
,
Takatoh
,
J.
,
Sakurai
,
K.
,
Wang
,
F.
, &
Mooney
,
R.
(
2013
).
A circuit for motor cortical modulation of auditory cortical activity
.
Journal of Neuroscience
,
33
(
36
),
14342
14353
. ,
[PubMed]
Niziolek
,
C. A.
, &
Guenther
,
F. H.
(
2013
).
Vowel category boundaries enhance cortical and behavioral responses to speech feedback alterations
.
Journal of Neuroscience
,
33
(
29
),
12090
12098
. ,
[PubMed]
Niziolek
,
C. A.
, &
Houde
,
J. F.
(
2015
).
wave_viewer: First release
.
Zenodo
.
Niziolek
,
C. A.
, &
Kiran
,
S.
(
2018
).
Assessing speech correction abilities with acoustic analyses: Evidence of preserved online correction in persons with aphasia
.
International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology
,
20
(
6
),
659
668
. ,
[PubMed]
Niziolek
,
C. A.
,
Nagarajan
,
S. S.
, &
Houde
,
J. F.
(
2013
).
What does motor efference copy represent? Evidence from speech production
.
Journal of Neuroscience
,
33
(
41
),
16110
16116
. ,
[PubMed]
Niziolek
,
C. A.
,
Nagarajan
,
S. S.
, &
Houde
,
J. F.
(
2015
).
The contribution of auditory feedback to corrective movements in vowel formant trajectories
. In
Scottish Consortium for ICPhS 2015
(Ed.),
Proceedings of the 18th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences
.
University of Glasgow
.
Niziolek
,
C. A.
, &
Parrell
,
B.
(
2021
).
Responses to auditory feedback manipulations in speech may be affected by previous exposure to auditory errors
.
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research
,
64
(
6S
),
2169
2181
. ,
[PubMed]
Otazu
,
G. H.
,
Tai
,
L. H.
,
Yang
,
Y.
, &
Zador
,
A. M.
(
2009
).
Engaging in an auditory task suppresses responses in auditory cortex
.
Nature Neuroscience
,
12
(
5
),
646
654
. ,
[PubMed]
Papoutsi
,
M.
,
de Zwart
,
J. A.
,
Jansma
,
J. M.
,
Pickering
,
M. J.
,
Bednar
,
J. A.
, &
Horwitz
,
B.
(
2009
).
From phonemes to articulatory codes: An fMRI study of the role of Broca’s area in speech production
.
Cerebral Cortex
,
19
(
9
),
2156
2165
. ,
[PubMed]
Parrell
,
B.
,
Ivry
,
R. B.
,
Nagarajan
,
S. S.
, &
Houde
,
J. F.
(
2021
).
Intact correction for self-produced vowel formant variability in individuals with cerebellar ataxia regardless of auditory feedback availability
.
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research
,
64
(
6S
),
2234
2247
. ,
[PubMed]
Parsons
,
C. L.
,
Lambier
,
J. D.
, &
Miller
,
A.
(
1988
).
Phonological processes and phonemic paraphasias
.
Aphasiology
,
2
(
1
),
45
54
.
Perkell
,
J. S.
(
2012
).
Movement goals and feedback and feedforward control mechanisms in speech production
.
Journal of Neurolinguistics
,
25
(
5
),
382
407
. ,
[PubMed]
Piastra
,
M. C.
,
Oostenveld
,
R.
,
Schoffelen
,
J. M.
, &
Piai
,
V.
(
2022
).
Estimating the influence of stroke lesions on MEG source reconstruction
.
NeuroImage
,
260
,
Article 119422
. ,
[PubMed]
Purcell
,
D. W.
, &
Munhall
,
K. G.
(
2006
).
Compensation following real-time manipulation of formants in isolated vowels
.
Journal of the Acoustical Society of America
,
119
(
4
),
2288
2297
. ,
[PubMed]
Railo
,
H.
,
Nokelainen
,
N.
,
Savolainen
,
S.
, &
Kaasinen
,
V.
(
2020
).
Deficits in monitoring self-produced speech in Parkinson’s disease
.
Clinical Neurophysiology
,
131
(
9
),
2140
2147
. ,
[PubMed]
Rauschecker
,
J. P.
, &
Scott
,
S. K.
(
2009
).
Maps and streams in the auditory cortex: Nonhuman primates illuminate human speech processing
.
Nature Neuroscience
,
12
(
6
),
718
724
. ,
[PubMed]
Reep
,
R. L.
,
Corwin
,
J. V.
,
Hashimoto
,
A.
, &
Watson
,
R. T.
(
1987
).
Efferent connections of the rostral portion of medial agranular cortex in rats
.
Brain Research Bulletin
,
19
(
2
),
203
221
. ,
[PubMed]
Robson
,
H.
,
Grube
,
M.
,
Lambon Ralph
,
M. A.
,
Griffiths
,
T. D.
, &
Sage
,
K.
(
2013
).
Fundamental deficits of auditory perception in Wernicke’s aphasia
.
Cortex
,
49
(
7
),
1808
1822
. ,
[PubMed]
Robson
,
H.
,
Keidel
,
J. L.
,
Lambon Ralph
,
M. A.
, &
Sage
,
K.
(
2012
).
Revealing and quantifying the impaired phonological analysis underpinning impaired comprehension in Wernicke’s aphasia
.
Neuropsychologia
,
50
(
2
),
276
288
. ,
[PubMed]
Rong
,
F.
,
Isenberg
,
A. L.
,
Sun
,
E.
, &
Hickok
,
G.
(
2018
).
The neuroanatomy of speech sequencing at the syllable level
.
PLOS ONE
,
13
(
10
),
Article e0196381
. ,
[PubMed]
Rummell
,
B. P.
,
Klee
,
J. L.
, &
Sigurdsson
,
T.
(
2016
).
Attenuation of responses to self-generated sounds in auditory cortical neurons
.
Journal of Neuroscience
,
36
(
47
),
12010
12026
. ,
[PubMed]
Ryalls
,
J. H.
(
1986
).
An acoustic study of vowel production in aphasia
.
Brain and Language
,
29
(
1
),
48
67
. ,
[PubMed]
Sangtian
,
S.
,
Wang
,
Y.
,
Fridriksson
,
J.
, &
Behroozmand
,
R.
(
2021
).
Impairment of speech auditory feedback error detection and motor correction in post-stroke aphasia
.
Journal of Communication Disorders
,
94
,
Article 106163
. ,
[PubMed]
Schneider
,
D. M.
,
Nelson
,
A.
, &
Mooney
,
R.
(
2014
).
A synaptic and circuit basis for corollary discharge in the auditory cortex
.
Nature
,
513
(
7517
),
189
194
. ,
[PubMed]
Schneider
,
D. M.
,
Sundararajan
,
J.
, &
Mooney
,
R.
(
2018
).
A cortical filter that learns to suppress the acoustic consequences of movement
.
Nature
,
561
,
391
395
. ,
[PubMed]
Schofield
,
T. M.
,
Penny
,
W. D.
,
Stephan
,
K. E.
,
Crinion
,
J. T.
,
Thompson
,
A. J.
,
Price
,
C. J.
, &
Leff
,
A. P.
(
2012
).
Changes in auditory feedback connections determine the severity of speech processing deficits after stroke
.
Journal of Neuroscience
,
32
(
12
),
4260
4270
. ,
[PubMed]
Shah-Basak
,
P. P.
,
Sivaratnam
,
G.
,
Teti
,
S.
,
Francois-Nienaber
,
A.
,
Yossofzai
,
M.
,
Armstrong
,
S.
,
Nayar
,
S.
,
Jokel
,
R.
, &
Meltzer
,
J.
(
2020
).
High definition transcranial direct current stimulation modulates abnormal neurophysiological activity in post-stroke aphasia
.
Scientific Reports
,
10
(
1
),
Article 19625
. ,
[PubMed]
Simonyan
,
K.
,
Ackermann
,
H.
,
Chang
,
E. F.
, &
Greenlee
,
J. D.
(
2016
).
New developments in understanding the complexity of human speech production
.
Journal of Neuroscience
,
36
(
45
),
11440
11448
. ,
[PubMed]
Sims
,
J. A.
,
Kapse
,
K.
,
Glynn
,
P.
,
Sandberg
,
C.
,
Tripodis
,
Y.
, &
Kiran
,
S.
(
2016
).
The relationships between the amount of spared tissue, percent signal change, and accuracy in semantic processing in aphasia
.
Neuropsychologia
,
84
,
113
126
. ,
[PubMed]
Sperry
,
R. W.
(
1950
).
Neural basis of the spontaneous optokinetic response produced by visual inversion
.
Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology
,
43
(
6
),
482
489
. ,
[PubMed]
Suga
,
N.
, &
Shimozawa
,
T.
(
1974
).
Site of neural attenuation of responses to self-vocalized sounds in echolocating bats
.
Science
,
183
(
4130
),
1211
1213
. ,
[PubMed]
Tadel
,
F.
,
Baillet
,
S.
,
Mosher
,
J. C.
,
Pantazis
,
D.
, &
Leahy
,
R. M.
(
2011
).
Brainstorm: A user-friendly application for MEG/EEG analysis
.
Computational Intelligence and Neuroscience
,
2011
,
1
13
. ,
[PubMed]
Tang
,
D.-L.
,
Parrell
,
B.
,
Beach
,
S. D.
, &
Niziolek
,
C. A.
(
2023
).
The brain’s sensitivity to sensory error can be modulated by altering perceived variability
.
BioRxiv
.
Tatu
,
L.
,
Moulin
,
T.
,
Bogousslavsky
,
J.
, &
Duvernoy
,
H.
(
1998
).
Arterial territories of the human brain: Cerebral hemispheres
.
Neurology
,
50
(
6
),
1699
1708
. ,
[PubMed]
Tourville
,
J. A.
, &
Guenther
,
F. H.
(
2011
).
The DIVA model: A neural theory of speech acquisition and production
.
Language and Cognitive Processes
,
26
(
7
),
952
981
. ,
[PubMed]
Tourville
,
J. A.
,
Nieto-Castañón
,
A.
,
Heyne
,
M.
, &
Guenther
,
F. H.
(
2019
).
Functional parcellation of the speech production cortex
.
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research
,
62
(
8S
),
3055
3070
. ,
[PubMed]
Tourville
,
J. A.
,
Reilly
,
K. J.
, &
Guenther
,
F. H.
(
2008
).
Neural mechanisms underlying auditory feedback control of speech
.
NeuroImage
,
39
(
3
),
1429
1443
. ,
[PubMed]
Toyomura
,
A.
,
Koyama
,
S.
,
Miyamaoto
,
T.
,
Terao
,
A.
,
Omori
,
T.
,
Murohashi
,
H.
, &
Kuriki
,
S.
(
2007
).
Neural correlates of auditory feedback control in human
.
Neuroscience
,
146
(
2
),
499
503
. ,
[PubMed]
Tremblay
,
P.
,
Deschamps
,
I.
, &
Gracco
,
V. L.
(
2016
).
Neurobiology of speech production: A motor control perspective
. In
Neurobiology of language
(pp.
741
750
).
Academic Press
.
Tzourio-Mazoyer
,
N.
,
Landeau
,
B.
,
Papathanassiou
,
D.
,
Crivello
,
F.
,
Etard
,
O.
,
Delcroix
,
N.
,
Mazoyer
,
B.
, &
Joliot
,
M.
(
2002
).
Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain
.
NeuroImage
,
15
(
1
),
273
289
. ,
[PubMed]
von Holst
,
E.
, &
Mittelstaedt
,
H.
(
1950
).
Das reafferenzprinzip: Wechselwirkungen zwischen zentralnervensystem und peripherie
.
Naturwissenschaften
,
37
(
20
),
464
476
.
Voss
,
M.
,
Ingram
,
J. N.
,
Haggard
,
P.
, &
Wolpert
,
D. M.
(
2006
).
Sensorimotor attenuation by central motor command signals in the absence of movement
.
Nature Neuroscience
,
9
(
1
),
26
27
. ,
[PubMed]
Wang
,
J.
,
Mathalon
,
D. H.
,
Roach
,
B. J.
,
Reilly
,
J.
,
Keedy
,
S. K.
,
Sweeney
,
J. A.
, &
Ford
,
J. M.
(
2014
).
Action planning and predictive coding when speaking
.
NeuroImage
,
91
,
91
98
. ,
[PubMed]
Waters
,
G.
, &
Caplan
,
D.
(
1995
).
The psycholinguistic assessment of language battery: Preliminary data
.
Presented at The British Aphasiological Society
,
York, England
.
Whitford
,
T. J.
,
Jack
,
B. N.
,
Pearson
,
D.
,
Griffiths
,
O.
,
Luque
,
D.
,
Harris
,
A. W.
,
Spencer
,
K. M.
, &
Le Pelley
,
M. E.
(
2017
).
Neurophysiological evidence of efference copies to inner speech
.
Elife
,
6
,
Article e28197
. ,
[PubMed]
Woldorff
,
M. G.
,
Gallen
,
C. C.
,
Hampson
,
S. A.
,
Hillyard
,
S. A.
,
Pantev
,
C.
,
Sobel
,
D.
, &
Bloom
,
F. E.
(
1993
).
Modulation of early sensory processing in human auditory cortex during auditory selective attention
.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
,
90
(
18
),
8722
8726
. ,
[PubMed]
Yourganov
,
G.
,
Smith
,
K. G.
,
Fridriksson
,
J.
, &
Rorden
,
C.
(
2015
).
Predicting aphasia type from brain damage measured with structural MRI
.
Cortex
,
73
,
203
215
. ,
[PubMed]
Yushkevich
,
P. A.
,
Gao
,
Y.
, &
Gerig
,
G.
(
2016
).
ITK-SNAP: An interactive tool for semi-automatic segmentation of multi-modality biomedical images
. In
2016 38th annual international conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC)
(pp.
3342
3345
).
IEEE
.
Yushkevich
,
P. A.
,
Piven
,
J.
,
Hazlett
,
H. C.
,
Smith
,
R. G.
,
Ho
,
S.
,
Gee
,
J. C.
, &
Gerig
,
G.
(
2006
).
User-guided 3D active contour segmentation of anatomical structures: Significantly improved efficiency and reliability
.
NeuroImage
,
31
(
3
),
1116
1128
. ,
[PubMed]
Zhang
,
W.
,
Yang
,
F.
, &
Tian
,
X.
(
2023
).
Functional connectivity between parietal and temporal lobes mediates internal forward models during speech production
.
Brain and Language
,
240
,
Article 105266
. ,
[PubMed]
Zheng
,
X.
,
Zhu
,
H.
,
Li
,
S.
, &
Tian
,
X.
(
2022
).
The generic inhibitory function of corollary discharge in motor intention: Evidence from the modulation effects of speech preparation on the late components of auditory neural responses
.
eNeuro
,
9
(
6
). .
[PubMed]
Ziegler
,
W.
(
2008
).
Apraxia of speech
. In
G.
Goldenberg
&
B. L.
Miller
(Eds.),
Handbook of clinical neurology
(
Vol. 88
,
3rd ser.
, pp.
269
285
).
Scribd
.

Competing Interests

Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Author notes

Handling Editor: Stephen M. Wilson

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. For a full description of the license, please visit https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/legalcode.

Supplementary data