Many studies argue that synchronized movement increases prosocial attitudes and behavior. We reviewed meta-analytic evidence that reported effects of synchrony may be driven by experimenter expectancy, leading to experimenter bias; and participant expectancy, otherwise known as placebo effects. We found that a majority of published studies do not adequately control for experimenter bias and that multiple independent replication attempts with added controls have failed to find the original effects. In a preregistered experiment, we measured participant expectancy directly, asking whether participants have a priori expectations about synchrony and prosociality that match the findings in published literature. Expectations about the effects of synchrony on prosocial attitudes directly mirrored previous experimental findings (including both positive and null effects)—despite the participants not actually engaging in synchrony. On the basis of this evidence, we propose an alternative account of the reported bottom-up effects of synchrony on prosociality: the effects of synchrony on prosociality may be explicable as the result of top-down expectations invoked by placebo and experimenter effects.

From the sympathetic oscillation of clocks (Huygens, 1665) to the flight patterns of migratory birds (Gunnarsson et al., 2004), synchrony is a widespread element of our physical, biological, and social worlds (Arenas et al., 2008; Bernieri & Rosenthal, 1991; Osipov et al., 2007; Phillips-Silver et al., 2010; Pikovsky et al., 2003). In humans, synchronous behavior is universal (in the form of dance; Mehr et al., 2018, 2019) and sweeping claims about the importance of synchrony to human social life, human cognition, and human evolution are common (e.g., Clarke et al., 2015; Launay et al., 2016; McNeill, 1995 and many others; for discussion, see Mehr et al., 2021).

Many have argued that synchronized movement facilitates human social interaction, aligning emotional and mental states and promoting social cohesion (Dissanayake, 2000; Durkheim, 1912; McNeill, 1995; Tarr et al., 2016; Turner, 1995). More recently, studies have found empirical evidence of synchrony’s positive effects on prosociality: Synchrony is reported to promote cooperation (Reddish et al., 2013; Valdesolo et al., 2010; Wiltermuth & Heath, 2009), affiliative behaviors (Macrae et al., 2008; Paladino et al., 2010), positive emotions (Hove & Risen, 2009; Launay et al., 2014), connection (Hove, 2008; Lakens, 2010; Wiltermuth & Heath, 2009), trust (Launay et al., 2013), and compassion for others (Valdesolo & DeSteno, 2011).

These findings support theoretical arguments that synchrony plays a causal role in the evolution of human social groups and the structure of human society (Clarke et al., 2015; Jackson et al., 2018; Lakens, 2010; Launay et al., 2016; Loersch & Arbuckle, 2013; Pearce et al., 2015; Savage et al., 2021; Weinstein et al., 2016). These in turn have been applied in real-world settings to ameliorate intergroup conflict (Tunçgenç & Cohen, 2016) and to design social robots (Delaherche et al., 2012).

Importantly, these claims go beyond the related idea that imitation, coordinated movements, and shared goals—independent of synchrony—increase prosociality, for which other literatures provide evidence (e.g., Carpenter et al., 2013; Chartrand & van Baaren, 2009; Over & Carpenter, 2015; Sherif, 1958). Indeed, recent work on synchrony aimed to control for these factors as potential confounds (e.g., Reddish et al., 2013; Tarr et al., 2018). The key claim is that synchronized movement is reliably more effective at promoting prosociality than is movement that is not synchronized (even if it is coordinated), as a result of bottom-up alignment of motoric/visual input, with aligned sensory input increasing merging of representations of self and other (e.g., Clarke et al., 2015; Tarr et al., 2014).

Here, we examine an alternative account for such claims. Meta-analytic evidence suggests that the effects of synchrony on prosociality may be driven by expectancy effects generated by a combination of (1) experimenter expectancy, leading to experimenter bias; and (2) participant expectancy (i.e., placebo effects). Rather than a result of bottom-up alignment of motoric and visual input, or “self-other merging” (e.g., Clarke et al., 2015; Tarr et al., 2014), we argue that the link between synchrony and prosociality is more parsimoniously explained as the result of top-down expectations. We evaluate the evidence for this claim in published studies, report new evidence demonstrating expectancy in real participants, and suggest ways future work can tease apart effects of synchrony from effects of expectancy.

Evidence of Experimenter Bias in the Synchrony Literature

Experimenter bias occurs when differences in an experimenter’s behavior shape participant response to align with experimenters’ expectations (Harris & Rosenthal, 1985; Rosenthal, 1976). These are most evident in study designs where both participants and experimenters are aware of their assigned experimental condition (Rosenthal, 1976). When experimenters are not naïve to experimental condition, biases can be large and are common across many study situations, even if experimenter-participant contact is limited. Rosenthal and Rubin (1978) estimated the overall size of interpersonal expectancy effects (including those caused by experimenter biases) across 345 studies in eight domains at d = .70.

Most published studies on synchrony and prosocial behavior involve experimenters who were aware of participants’ condition during the study (estimated at 75%; where experimenters were categorized as naïve if the authors stated explicitly that the experimenter was not aware of the hypotheses or condition, or was not present during the study; Rennung & Göritz, 2016), raising the possibility that differences in the ways experimenters interacted with participants unconsciously shaped their responses to align with experimenters’ expectations (Rosenthal, 1976). Two meta-analyses found weak-to-moderate effects of interpersonal synchrony on prosociality (Mogan et al., 2017; Rennung & Göritz, 2016), but when limiting the meta-analysis to experiments where the experimenter was naïve to condition, the effect of synchrony on prosocial behavior was not distinguishable from zero (Rennung & Göritz, 2016). Thus, methodological choices, such as allowing experimenters to be aware of participants’ condition assignment, or unequal degrees of participant expectancy, may leave synchrony-prosociality literature vulnerable to the role of bias via experimenter demand effects (Rennung & Göritz, 2016).

In line with this meta-analytic finding, multiple attempts to replicate the effects of highly-cited studies in the synchrony literature with added controls for experimenter bias have failed (Kirschner & Ilari, 2013; Schachner & Garvin, 2010). For example, Schachner and Garvin (2010) conducted two replications of the synchrony manipulation used in Wiltermuth and Heath (2009; Exp. 3) with experimenters naïve to condition and found that, unlike the original study, synchrony did not increase interpersonal cooperation, conformity, trust, similarity, or feelings of being in the same team. Additionally, other studies found no prosocial effects of synchrony in young children (Kirschner & Ilari, 2013).

The Potential for Placebo Effects Caused by Participant Expectancy

A placebo effect occurs when a response is misattributed to an inert treatment or experimental condition, but is actually due to contextual factors such as pre-existing beliefs (Benedetti et al., 2005). Placebo effects exist across many clinical studies (see reviews: Finniss et al., 2010; Kaptchuk & Miller, 2015; Price et al., 2008), including those in psychology (e.g., Kirsch & Sapirstein, 1998; Leuchter et al., 2002). While the psychological mechanisms underlying placebo effects are controversial (e.g., Moerman, 2002; Moerman & Jonas, 2002; Stewart-Williams & Podd, 2004), their results are clear: When people expect that a particular intervention will lead to a particular effect, this expectation alone can cause them to experience that effect (Hahn, 1997; Kirsch, 1985, 1990; Montgomery & Kirsch, 1997; Peck & Coleman, 1991).

Participant expectancy effects are a well-documented form of placebo effects in the clinical and experimental psychology literature (Chan & Lovibond, 1996; Colagiuri, 2010; Goldstein, 2013; Oettingen, 2000; Younger et al., 2012). For example, participant expectancy affects memory for previous experiences, biasing subjective outcome measures in favor of an intervention (Price et al., 1999). Moreover, randomized-controlled trials investigating the role of placebo effects find that participants in pain studies who expect a particular treatment to be effective (e.g., acupuncture) report significantly less pain, irrespective of whether they experienced actual acupuncture, or a control treatment (Linde et al., 2007). Recent work has also found that highly structured interventions lead to stronger placebo effects than less-structured interventions (Shen et al., 2020).

The elimination of bias introduced by expectation-based placebo effects is an essential step to ensure the validity of findings in psychological science—yet is often overlooked. In many studies, intervention groups are compared to control groups without accounting for differences in expectations (Boot et al., 2013). In the case of motor synchrony, expectation-based effects may arise due to explicit instruction (e.g., from an experimenter, to “walk in step” as in Wiltermuth & Heath, 2009). However, they may also arise incidentally: By observing their own participation in the task, participants become aware of the level of synchrony involved, and rate the level of synchrony as different across conditions (e.g., Tarr et al., 2018). Placebo effects therefore remain a potential concern even in tasks where synchrony arises without explicit instruction. Regardless of origin, participants’ a priori expectations about the heightened effects of an experimental manipulation on a dependent variable may be attributable to placebo rather than to the manipulation itself. Thus, comparison to an active control that is matched for participant expectations is necessary to fully attribute effects to the intervention, rather than participant expectancy (Boot et al., 2013).

Do participants in synchrony experiments enter with a priori expectations about the relationship between synchrony and prosociality? If so, this would leave the synchrony literature vulnerable to placebo effects, and imply that participant expectancy, rather than the experience of synchrony itself, may be responsible for increases in prosociality after synchrony. If true, the bottom-up effects of synchronized behavior may be small or nonexistent, and top-down expectations would the primary driver of synchrony’s prosocial effects.

Do Participants Have A Priori Expectations about Synchrony and Prosociality?

To test this question, we conducted a preregistered experiment (see https://osf.io/6g2vy) to measure the degree to which participants have a priori expectations about synchrony and prosociality, and whether these expectations match the findings reported in the literature (see full methods details in Supplementary Information Text S1S3). Participants read about a hypothetical experiment (Exp. 1 in Wiltermuth and Heath (2009), among the most-cited synchrony papers in the literature), and were asked to predict the attitudes and emotions others would have after participating. In the original experiment, participants took a 7-minute walk around a college campus together with two other participants and an experimenter. Participants in the experimental condition were instructed to walk in synchrony, while participants in the control condition were not (Wiltermuth & Heath, 2009; S. Wiltermuth, personal communication). Afterward, participants were surveyed regarding their attitudes toward the other participants and their emotions, including the extent of interpersonal coordination, connection, trust, similarity, synchrony, feeling of being on the same team, happiness, and frustration. Participants in the synchrony condition showed greater levels of connection, trust, same team feeling, and feelings of similarity to their partner in comparison to participants in the control condition; but did not differ on levels of happiness (Wiltermuth & Heath, 2009).

We asked participants (N = 216) to imagine this hypothetical experiment, and probed their expectations by asking them to predict how other people would feel and act after participating in such a task, using measures that mirrored those of the original study (provided by the authors; S. Wiltermuth, personal communication). We then compared our participants’ expectations to the real effects (both positive and null) reported by Wiltermuth and Heath (2009). We reasoned that if participants in our hypothetical scenario study showed the same distinct pattern of positive and null expectations as participants in the original study showed after actually experiencing synchrony or asynchrony, then participant expectancy is likely to play a role in the original experimental findings. To isolate participant expectancy from the effects of first-person mental simulation of synchrony (see Atherton et al., 2019; Stupacher et al., 2017), we asked participants to make predictions about other people, not themselves; and minimized vivid imagery by keeping vignettes brief and not prompting participants to take any additional time elaborating on what they read (as done in Atherton et al., 2019). We note that while such measures do not preclude the possibility of all mental simulation, it is likely that participants in our study engaged in minimal mental simulation as a result of these aspects of the study design.

Participants’ expectations about the effects of synchrony tightly matched the effects of the first-person experience of synchrony (see Figure 1): Participants predicting people’s attitudes after walking in synchrony (versus the control condition) predicted that they would feel more connected, trusting, more on the same team (ps < .00625, the Bonferroni-adjusted alpha for 8 comparisons), and more similar to their group members (p < .05; this test did not survive the correction for multiple comparisons). Notably, participants did not expect differences in levels of happiness (p = .17), mirroring the null result reported in Wiltermuth and Heath (2009; Exp. 1). Participants in our study also expected people to feel more coordinated and synchronized (p’s < .00625), but not more frustrated (p > .78) after walking in synchrony (see Supplement Table S1 and Figure S2 for means, standard deviations, and visualization of all comparisons).

Figure 1.

Participants’ expectations about synchrony match reported effects of the experience of synchrony, from a previous study. Each set of bars represents participants’ ratings of emotions and attitudes toward other people in one of five different categories (x-axis) after actually walking with them in synchrony (Experienced Synchrony; data from Wiltermuth & Heath, 2009; Exp. 1); actually walking normally/out of synchrony (Experienced Control; data from Wiltermuth & Heath, 2009; Exp. 1); imagining how other people would expect to feel after walking in synchrony (Imagined Synchrony); or imagining how others would feel after walking normally (Imagined Control). Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals. **p < 0.00625, ˆp < 0.05.

Figure 1.

Participants’ expectations about synchrony match reported effects of the experience of synchrony, from a previous study. Each set of bars represents participants’ ratings of emotions and attitudes toward other people in one of five different categories (x-axis) after actually walking with them in synchrony (Experienced Synchrony; data from Wiltermuth & Heath, 2009; Exp. 1); actually walking normally/out of synchrony (Experienced Control; data from Wiltermuth & Heath, 2009; Exp. 1); imagining how other people would expect to feel after walking in synchrony (Imagined Synchrony); or imagining how others would feel after walking normally (Imagined Control). Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals. **p < 0.00625, ˆp < 0.05.

Close modal

On the suggestion of an anonymous reviewer, we continued by testing whether the expectancy effects reported above differ in magnitude from the effects in Exp. 1 of Wiltermuth and Heath (2009; many thanks to Scott Wiltermuth for providing the original data to enable this analysis). We ran linear regressions predicting each variable tested in both samples (connection, trust, similarity, synchrony, feeling of being on the same team, and happiness) from the predictors of study type (imagined, in the current study; vs. experienced, in the Wiltermuth & Heath study), condition (synchrony vs. control/neutral), and their interaction.

We found no evidence of differences in magnitude of the main effects across the two study types for connection, trust, similarity, same team, or happiness; and interactions between study type and condition were also not significant (all ps > .1). There was a difference by study type only for the feeling of being in synchrony, such that people who experienced synchrony (relative to those who imagined it in others) showed a greater difference between synchrony vs. control condition in how synchronized they felt (or imagined others felt) with their partner (β = 1.52, t(242) = 2.75, p = 0.006. The full regression results are reported in Appendix S3 in the Supplementary Information.

Effects of synchrony on prosociality are typically attributed to the experience of synchrony, with first-person sensory experience of moving in synchrony with another person causing increased prosociality. Based on patterns of reproducibility and the results outlined above, we suspect an alternative: Synchrony may lead to prosocial effects not because of experiencing the content of the manipulation, but rather, due to systematic a priori expectations about the effects of synchrony, held by both participants and experimenters.

We are not the first to address the role of expectancy in the synchrony literature. Past work raised the possibility of a moderating role for experimenter effects, in that experimental designs that did control for experimenter effects (of which there were fairly few) showed small to null effects of interpersonal synchrony on prosocial attitudes and behaviors (see Rennung & Göritz, 2016). While this conclusion is not necessarily reflective of Rennung & Goritz’s interpretation, these data are consistent with the idea that expectancy in general inflates effect sizes (Price et al., 2008; Rosenthal & Rubin, 1978). The data presented here take these ideas one step further: we provide an existence proof that expectancy regarding prosocial attitudes can be elicited in studies of synchrony, supporting the idea of a generalized, top-down causal pathway for effects of synchrony on prosociality that may be overlooked in favor of bottom-up perceptual explanations.

Neither the existing literature nor our survey demonstrate an absence of a link between synchrony and prosociality. Experimenter bias and placebo effects may only account for a fraction of a true effect of synchrony on prosociality, and may apply differently depending on the type of movement (finger tapping vs. walking) or social context (in virtual reality vs. walking around campus). Still, given the propensity of expectations to produce large effects, their effects are likely to be substantial. Understanding the role of expectancy is therefore essential to understanding the psychological mechanisms underlying synchrony’s effects. Coordinated behaviors such as imitation or having a shared goal are also thought to increase prosociality, whether or not they involve synchronized movement (e.g., Carpenter et al., 2013; Chartrand & van Baaren, 2009; Over & Carpenter, 2015; Sherif, 1958). Thus, even if differences between synchrony and asynchrony are attributable to expectancy, activities that promote coordinated behavior (such as music and dance) could be expected to promote prosociality, independent of putative effects of synchrony.

Where might people’s expectations about synchrony come from? One possibility is that they reflect accurate metacognition: People may be aware of the impact of synchrony on their attitudes, emotions, and behavior. We find such nuanced metacognition unlikely; however, even accurate expectations would create placebo effects. In this case, placebo effects would systematically inflate the effect size of the experience of synchrony in the literature.

An alternative possibility is that people have intuitive beliefs about synchrony as a part of their folk theories of social behavior. From childhood, humans develop intuitive mental theories of how the social and physical worlds work. These are shaped both by experience and by innate biases about how people learn and think (Gopnik & Meltzoff, 1997; Spelke & Kinzler, 2007). While rich, these intuitive theories are often wrong (Gelman, 2003; Shtulman, 2017). Mistaken beliefs about synchrony’s impact on prosociality might result from biases in attention and memory: For example, synchrony may be a particularly salient form of coordinated movement, such that people notice and remember interactions involving synchronized movement more than others types of coordinated action. This would make it easier to recall instances when synchrony led to prosociality than instances when non-synchronized coordinated interactions led to prosociality, via an availability bias (e.g., Gilovich et al., 2002), creating the belief that synchronized actions drive prosocial behavior more than coordinated actions do.

We suspect some middle ground between these accounts to be most parsimonious. Synchrony has been reported to impact a variety of attitudinal and behavioral measures, including perceived entativity (Lakens, 2010), likeability (Hove & Risen, 2009), conformity (Wiltermuth, 2012), trust (Tamborini et al., 2018), inclusion of other in the self (Tarr et al., 2016), prejudice reduction (Atherton et al., 2019), and cooperative ability (Valdesolo et al., 2010). We hypothesize that the experience of synchrony likely impacts a smaller, more restricted range of social variables, with reported effects on some variables in the literature attributable to placebo and experimenter expectancy effects. Ultimately, identifying which (if any) of these variables are most susceptible and to what extent they are impacted by placebo and/or expectancy effects remains an open question and an important direction for future work.

In particular, the current study provided evidence that participants have expectations about prosocial attitudes – and did not measure whether these expectations extended to prosocial behaviors, like cooperation or helping. There is considerable evidence in the synchrony literature to suggest that attitudes and behaviors are linked (Jackson et al., 2018; Lang et al., 2017; Reddish et al., 2013; Tunçgenç & Cohen, 2016; Wiltermuth & Heath, 2009). The extent to which expectancy effects regarding synchrony impact prosocial behaviors is an open question, and an important direction for future work.

To determine which dependent measures are directly impacted by the experience of synchrony (versus those which reflect participants’ expectations), future work should attempt to eliminate or manipulate expectations directly. Ideally, this can be done using experimental designs where experimenters and participants are both unaware of condition. Such methods may be difficult to implement in synchrony experiments, as keeping participants naïve to the level of synchrony in their own movements while maintaining a social context may be impossible (or, at least, would require the creative use of one-way mirrors and similar experimental hijinks).

Synchrony may also be made less conspicuous by using a cover story regarding the purpose of the study, reducing the saliency of motor synchrony in the manipulation. This would require deception of both participants and experimenters, and all parties would need to be kept unaware of the motivation of the study until data collection is finished. This means that for experimenters, the cover story must be maintained over the course of data collection and be robust against experimenters’ expectations about synchrony and the true hypothesis of the study over time. If this method is used, researchers should provide periodic checks (e.g., open-ended questions) throughout data collection to ensure that the participants and experimenters have not inferred the true hypothesis. Tarr et al. (2018) used this approach, taking steps to decrease expectancy, including a cover story, a experimenter who was unaware of the hypothesis, and conducting the synchronous/asynchronous interaction in a virtual reality environment with tight control over simulated movement. Results were mixed: predicted effects of synchrony were found on only two of six measures, and most participants also did not believe their virtual partners were real, complicating interpretation.

Past work has demonstrated that the magnitude of expectancy effects may be related to the strength of participants’ expectations (i.e., a dose-dependent relationship; Sterzer et al., 2008). Thus, another potential way to account for expectancy effects is to use a control manipulation that is matched for participant expectations (Boot et al., 2013). In many cases, including the study we examine (Wiltermuth & Heath, 2009; Exp. 1), the control condition is active, and matched on many salient features (e.g., it involves taking a walk that is similar to the manipulation condition), but is not matched on participant expectations (as the results of our study show). To tease apart the effect of the intervention from the effect of expectations, a new control condition could be created that matches the experimental condition in the degree to which participants expect it will impact their response to the dependent variable or variables. Alternatively, conditions could be matched by choosing a different dependent variable—one that does not evoke differential participant expectations across conditions, but that theory predicts should be impacted by the experience of synchrony.

Achieving neutral expectations through these methods is likely to be challenging as commonly used manipulations like walking, bouncing, or hand motions may incidentally or intentionally engender participants’ expectations via awareness of synchronous movement. Fortunately, there are ways to counteract this. For instance participants’ expectations may also be experimentally manipulated, by presenting participants with evidence to change their a priori expectations before the task. By these methods, neutral expectancy can be achieved by presenting participants with different evidence, leading half the participants in each condition to believe that the intervention (e.g., synchronous movement) will influence their response to the dependent variable, while the other half is led to have neutral expectations (Clifasefi et al., 2006). It is also possible to explore the contributions of expectations to the effects of synchrony without creating neutral expectancy. For example, using the counterdemand design, participants could be led to believe that synchrony will only change their response to the dependent variable after a particular amount of time or exposure, and then be tested before and after this period (Boot et al., 2013).

Finally, the role of expectancy could be further tested by inducing the opposite expectation: leading participants to believe that motor synchrony decreases prosociality. Past research has demonstrated that conscious processes are vulnerable to expectations regardless of directionality (Benedetti et al., 2003). Thus, we believe that manipulating expectancy in the opposite direction would yield decreases in self-reported prosociality following synchrony or, at least, attenuated positive effects of synchrony. Attenuated positive effects would be expected if the expectation-change manipulation was not effective in every participant, or if expectation effects are one of multiple factors that drive synchrony’s prosocial impact. Any or all of these methods would help to determine the effects of the experience of synchrony on prosociality, over and above placebo effects, the effects of participants’ expectations.

The issues we outline here threaten the validity of causal inferences in the synchrony literature, and provide an alternative causal account of how synchrony may promote prosociality (i.e., top-down expectations vs. the experience of synchrony itself). While we have not shown that no link exists between synchrony and prosociality, the existence of nuanced top-down expectations about the effect of synchrony in naïve participants suggests that experimenter bias, participant expectancy, and placebo effects likely account for some portion of a putative true effect of synchrony on prosociality. Thus, future work addressing the role of expectancy is essential for understanding the mechanism and extent to which synchrony impacts interpersonal attitudes and social behavior.

The authors thank Bronwyn Tarr and Joshua S. Bamford for valuable discussion and feedback, Michelle Lee and Jordan Legaspi for assistance with data collection, and Scott Wiltermuth for his timely assistance clarifying the method and providing original data gathered in his original publication (Wiltermuth & Heath, 2009).

  • Contributed to conception and design: AS, SA, & SAM

  • Contributed to acquisition of data: AS, SA, & SAM

  • Contributed to analysis and interpretation of data: AS, SA, & SAM

  • Drafted and/or revised the article: AS, SA, & SAM

  • Approved the submitted version for publication: AS, SA, & SAM

This research was funded by the Harvard Data Science Initiative (S.A.M.), the National Institutes of Health Director’s Early Independence Award DP5OD024566 (S.A.M.), and a National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship (S.A.).

Arenas
,
A.
,
Diaz-Guilera
,
A.
,
Kurths
,
J.
,
Moreno
,
Y.
, &
Zhou
,
C.
(
2008
).
Synchronization in complex networks
.
Physics Reports
,
469
(
3
),
93
153
.
Atherton
,
G.
,
Sebanz
,
N.
, &
Cross
,
L.
(
2019
).
Imagine all the synchrony: The effects of actual and imagined synchronous walking on attitudes towards marginalised groups
.
PLOS ONE
,
14
(
5
),
Article e0216585
. ,
[PubMed]
Benedetti
,
F.
,
Mayberg
,
H. S.
,
Wager
,
T. D.
,
Stohler
,
C. S.
, &
Zubieta
,
J.-K.
(
2005
).
Neurobiological mechanisms of the placebo effect
.
Journal of Neuroscience
,
25
(
45
),
10390
10402
. ,
[PubMed]
Benedetti
,
F.
,
Pollo
,
A.
,
Lopiano
,
L.
,
Lanotte
,
M.
,
Vighetti
,
S.
, &
Rainero
,
I.
(
2003
).
Conscious expectation and unconscious conditioning in analgesic, motor, and hormonal placebo/nocebo responses
.
Journal of Neuroscience
,
23
(
10
),
4315
4323
. ,
[PubMed]
Bernieri
,
F. J.
, &
Rosenthal
,
R.
(
1991
).
Interpersonal coordination: Behavior matching and interactional synchrony
. In
R. S.
Feldman
&
B.
Rimé
(Eds.),
Fundamentals of nonverbal behavior
(pp.
401
432
).
Cambridge University Press; Editions de la Maison des Sciences de l’Homme
.
Boot
,
W. R.
,
Simons
,
D. J.
,
Stothart
,
C.
, &
Stutts
,
C.
(
2013
).
The pervasive problem with placebos in psychology: Why active control groups are not sufficient to rule out placebo effects
.
Perspectives on Psychological Science
,
8
(
4
),
445
454
. ,
[PubMed]
Carpenter
,
M.
,
Uebel
,
J.
, &
Tomasello
,
M.
(
2013
).
Being mimicked increases prosocial behavior in 18-month-old infants
.
Child Deveopment
,
84
(
5
),
1511
1518
. ,
[PubMed]
Chan
,
C. K. Y.
, &
Lovibond
,
P. F.
(
1996
).
Expectancy bias in trait anxiety
.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology
,
105
(
4
),
637
647
. ,
[PubMed]
Chartrand
,
T. L.
, & van
Baaren
,
R.
(
2009
).
Human mimicry
. In
Advances in experimental social psychology
(
Vol. 41
, pp.
219
274
).
Elsevier
.
Clarke
,
E.
,
DeNora
,
T.
, &
Vuoskoski
,
J.
(
2015
).
Music, empathy and cultural understanding
.
Physics of Life Reviews
,
15
,
61
88
. ,
[PubMed]
Clifasefi
,
S. L.
,
Takarangi
,
M. K. T.
, &
Bergman
,
J. S.
(
2006
).
Blind drunk: The effects of alcohol on inattentional blindness
.
Applied Cognitive Psychology
,
20
(
5
),
697
704
.
Colagiuri
,
B.
(
2010
).
Participant expectancies in double-blind randomized placebo-controlled trials: Potential limitations to trial validity
.
Clinical Trials
,
7
(
3
),
246
255
. ,
[PubMed]
Delaherche
,
E.
,
Chetouani
,
M.
,
Mahdhaoui
,
A.
,
Saint-Georges
,
C.
,
Viaux
,
S.
, &
Cohen
,
D.
(
2012
).
Interpersonal synchrony: A survey of evaluation methods across disciplines
.
IEEE Transactions on Affective Computing
,
3
(
3
),
349
365
.
Dissanayake
,
E.
(
2000
).
Antecedents of the temporal arts in early mother-infant interaction
. In
N. L.
Wallin
,
B.
Merker
, &
B.
Steven
(Eds.),
The origins of music
.
MIT Press
.
Durkheim
,
É.
(
1912
).
The elementary forms of the religious life
.
Free Press
.
Finniss
,
D. G.
,
Kaptchuk
,
T. J.
,
Miller
,
F.
, &
Benedetti
,
F.
(
2010
).
Biological, clinical, and ethical advances of placebo effects
.
The Lancet
,
375
(
9715
),
686
695
. ,
[PubMed]
Gelman
,
S. A.
(
2003
).
The essential child: Origins of essentialism in everyday thought
.
Oxford University Press
.
Gilovich
,
T.
,
Griffin
,
D. W.
, &
Kahneman
,
D.
(Eds.). (
2002
).
Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment
.
Cambridge University Press
.
Goldstein
,
A.
(
2013
).
Evaluating expectancy effects in cross-cultural counseling and psychotherapy
. In
A.
Marsella
&
P.
Pendersen
(Eds.),
Cross-cultural counseling and psychotherapy
.
Pergamon Press
.
Gopnik
,
A.
, &
Meltzoff
,
A. N.
(
1997
).
Words, thoughts, and theories
.
MIT Press
.
Gunnarsson
,
T. G.
,
Gill
,
J. A.
,
Sigurbjörnsson
,
T.
, &
Sutherland
,
W. J.
(
2004
).
Pair bonds: Arrival synchrony in migratory birds
.
Nature
,
431
(
7009
),
646
. ,
[PubMed]
Hahn
,
R.
(
1997
).
The nocebo phenomenon: Scope and foundations
. In
A.
Harrington
(Ed.),
The placebo effect: An interdisciplinary exploration
(pp.
56
76
).
Harvard University Press
.
Harris
,
M. J.
, &
Rosenthal
,
R.
(
1985
).
Mediation of interpersonal expectancy effects: 31 meta-analyses
.
Psychological Bulletin
,
97
(
3
),
363
386
.
Hove
,
M. J.
(
2008
).
Shared circuits, shared time, and interpersonal synchrony
.
Behavioral and Brain Sciences
,
31
(
1
),
29
30
.
Hove
,
M. J.
, &
Risen
,
J. L.
(
2009
).
It’s all in the timing: Interpersonal synchrony increases affiliation
.
Social Cognition
,
27
(
6
),
949
960
.
Huygens
,
C.
(
1665
).
Letter to de Sluse: Letter no. 1333 of February 24, 1665, no. 1335 of 26 February 1665, no. 1345 of 6 March 1665
. In
Oeuvres Complètes de Christiaan Huygens
(pp.
1664
1665
).
Societe Hollandaise Des Sciences
.
Jackson
,
J. C.
,
Jong
,
J.
,
Bilkey
,
D.
,
Whitehouse
,
H.
,
Zollmann
,
S.
,
McNaughton
,
C.
, &
Halberstadt
,
J.
(
2018
).
Synchrony and physiological arousal increase cohesion and cooperation in large naturalistic groups
.
Scientific Reports
,
8
(
1
),
127
. ,
[PubMed]
Kaptchuk
,
T. J.
, &
Miller
,
F. G.
(
2015
).
Placebo effects in medicine
.
New England Journal of Medicine
,
373
(
1
),
8
9
. ,
[PubMed]
Kirsch
,
I.
(
1985
).
Response expectancy as a determinant of experience and behavior
.
American Psychologist
,
40
(
11
),
1189
1202
.
Kirsch
,
I.
(
1990
).
Changing expectations: A key to effective psychotherapy
.
American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis
,
34
(
2
),
138
140
.
Kirsch
,
I.
, &
Sapirstein
,
G.
(
1998
).
Listening to Prozac but hearing placebo: A meta-analysis of antidepressant medication
.
Prevention & Treatment
,
1
(
2
),
Article 2a
.
Kirschner
,
S.
, &
Ilari
,
B.
(
2013
).
Joint drumming in Brazilian and German preschool children: Cultural differences in rhythmic entrainment, but no prosocial effects
.
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology
,
45
(
1
),
137
166
.
Lakens
,
D.
(
2010
).
Movement synchrony and perceived entitativity
.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology
,
46
(
5
),
701
708
.
Lang
,
M.
,
Bahna
,
V.
,
Shaver
,
J. H.
,
Reddish
,
P.
, &
Xygalatas
,
D.
(
2017
).
Sync to link: Endorphin-mediated synchrony effects on cooperation
.
Biological Psychology
,
127
,
191
197
. ,
[PubMed]
Launay
,
J.
,
Dean
,
R. T.
, &
Bailes
,
F.
(
2013
).
Synchronization can influence trust following virtual interaction
.
Experimental Psychology
,
60
(
1
),
53
63
. ,
[PubMed]
Launay
,
J.
,
Dean
,
R. T.
, &
Bailes
,
F.
(
2014
).
Synchronising movements with the sounds of a virtual partner enhances partner likeability
.
Cognitive Processing
,
15
(
4
),
491
501
. ,
[PubMed]
Launay
,
J.
,
Tarr
,
B.
, &
Dunbar
,
R. I. M.
(
2016
).
Synchrony as an adaptive mechanism for large-scale human social bonding
.
Ethology
,
122
(
10
),
779
789
.
Leuchter
,
A. F.
,
Cook
,
I. A.
,
Witte
,
E. A.
,
Morgan
,
M.
, &
Abrams
,
M.
(
2002
).
Changes in brain function of depressed subjects during treatment with placebo
.
American Journal of Psychiatry
,
159
(
1
),
122
129
. ,
[PubMed]
Linde
,
K.
,
Witt
,
C. M.
,
Streng
,
A.
,
Weidenhammer
,
W.
,
Wagenpfeil
,
S.
,
Brinkhaus
,
B.
,
Willich
,
S. N.
, &
Melchart
,
D.
(
2007
).
The impact of patient expectations on outcomes in four randomized controlled trials of acupuncture in patients with chronic pain
.
Pain
,
128
(
3
),
264
271
. ,
[PubMed]
Loersch
,
C.
, &
Arbuckle
,
N. L.
(
2013
).
Unraveling the mystery of music: Music as an evolved group process
.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
,
105
(
5
),
777
798
. ,
[PubMed]
Macrae
,
C. N.
,
Duffy
,
O. K.
,
Miles
,
L. K.
, &
Lawrence
,
J.
(
2008
).
A case of hand waving: Action synchrony and person perception
.
Cognition
,
109
(
1
),
152
156
. ,
[PubMed]
McNeill
,
W.
(
1995
).
Keeping together in time: Dance and drill in human history
.
Harvard University Press
.
Mehr
,
S. A.
,
Krasnow
,
M. M.
,
Bryant
,
G. A.
, &
Hagen
,
E. H.
(
2021
).
Origins of music in credible signaling
.
Behavioral and Brain Sciences
,
44
,
e60
. ,
[PubMed]
Mehr
,
S. A.
,
Singh
,
M.
,
Knox
,
D.
,
Ketter
,
D. M.
,
Pickens-Jones
,
D.
,
Atwood
,
S.
,
Lucas
,
C.
,
Jacoby
,
N.
,
Egner
,
A. A.
,
Hopkins
,
E. J.
,
Howard
,
R. M.
,
Hartshorne
,
J. K.
,
Jennings
,
M. V.
,
Simson
,
J.
,
Bainbridge
,
C. M.
,
Pinker
,
S.
,
O’Donnell
,
T. J.
,
Krasnow
,
M. M.
, &
Glowacki
,
L.
(
2019
).
Universality and diversity in human song
.
Science
,
366
(
6468
),
eaax0868
. ,
[PubMed]
Mehr
,
S. A.
,
Singh
,
M.
,
York
,
H.
,
Glowacki
,
L.
, &
Krasnow
,
M. M.
(
2018
).
Form and function in human song
.
Current Biology
,
28
(
3
),
356
368
. ,
[PubMed]
Moerman
,
D. E.
(
2002
).
Meaning, medicine, and the “placebo effect”
.
Cambridge University Press
.
Moerman
,
D. E.
, &
Jonas
,
W. B.
(
2002
).
Deconstructing the placebo effect and finding the meaning response
.
Annals of Internal Medicine
,
136
(
6
),
471
476
. ,
[PubMed]
Mogan
,
R.
,
Fischer
,
R.
, &
Bulbulia
,
J. A.
(
2017
).
To be in synchrony or not? A meta-analysis of synchrony’s effects on behavior, perception, cognition and affect
.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology
,
72
,
13
20
.
Montgomery
,
G. H.
, &
Kirsch
,
I.
(
1997
).
Classical conditioning and the placebo effect
.
Pain
,
72
(
1–2
),
107
113
. ,
[PubMed]
Oettingen
,
G.
(
2000
).
Expectancy effects on behavior depend on self-regulatory thought
.
Social Cognition
,
18
(
2
),
101
129
.
Osipov
,
G. V.
,
Kurths
,
J.
, &
Zhou
,
C.
(
2007
).
Synchronization in oscillatory networks
.
Springer
.
Over
,
H.
, &
Carpenter
,
M.
(
2015
).
Children infer affiliative and status relations from watching others imitate
.
Developmental Science
,
18
(
6
),
917
925
. ,
[PubMed]
Paladino
,
M.-P.
,
Mazzurega
,
M.
,
Pavani
,
F.
, &
Schubert
,
T. W.
(
2010
).
Synchronous multisensory stimulation blurs self-other boundaries
.
Psychological Science
,
21
(
9
),
1202
1207
. ,
[PubMed]
Pearce
,
E.
,
Launay
,
J.
, &
Dunbar
,
R. I. M.
(
2015
).
The ice-breaker effect: Singing mediates fast social bonding
.
Royal Society Open Science
,
2
(
10
),
150221
. ,
[PubMed]
Peck
,
C.
, &
Coleman
,
G.
(
1991
).
Implications of placebo theory for clinical research and practice in pain management
.
Theoretical Medicine
,
12
(
3
),
247
270
. ,
[PubMed]
Phillips-Silver
,
J.
,
Aktipis
,
C. A.
, &
Bryant
,
G. A.
(
2010
).
The ecology of entrainment: Foundations of coordinated rhythmic movement
.
Music Perception
,
28
(
1
),
3
14
. ,
[PubMed]
Pikovsky
,
A.
,
Rosenblum
,
M.
, &
Kurths
,
J.
(
2003
).
Synchronization: A universal concept in nonlinear sciences
(1st paperback ed.).
Cambridge University Press
.
Price
,
D. D.
,
Finniss
,
D. G.
, &
Benedetti
,
F.
(
2008
).
A comprehensive review of the placebo effect: Recent advances and current thought
.
Annual Review of Psychology
,
59
(
1
),
565
590
. ,
[PubMed]
Price
,
D. D.
,
Milling
,
L. S.
,
Kirsch
,
I.
,
Duff
,
A.
,
Montgomery
,
G. H.
, &
Nicholls
,
S. S.
(
1999
).
An analysis of factors that contribute to the magnitude of placebo analgesia in an experimental paradigm
:
Pain
,
83
(
2
),
147
156
. ,
[PubMed]
Reddish
,
P.
,
Fischer
,
R.
, &
Bulbulia
,
J.
(
2013
).
Let’s dance together: Synchrony, shared intentionality and cooperation
.
PLOS ONE
,
8
(
8
),
Article e71182
. ,
[PubMed]
Rennung
,
M.
, &
Göritz
,
A. S.
(
2016
).
Prosocial consequences of interpersonal synchrony: A meta-analysis
.
Zeitschrift für Psychologie
,
224
(
3
),
168
189
. ,
[PubMed]
Rosenthal
,
R.
(
1976
).
Experimenter effects in behavioral research
(Enlarged ed.).
Irvington
.
Rosenthal
,
R.
, &
Rubin
,
D. B.
(
1978
).
Interpersonal expectancy effects: The first 345 studies
.
Behavioral and Brain Sciences
,
1
(
3
),
377
386
.
Savage
,
P. E.
,
Loui
,
P.
,
Tarr
,
B.
,
Schachner
,
A.
,
Glowacki
,
L.
,
Mithen
,
S.
, &
Fitch
,
W. T.
(
2021
).
Music as a coevolved system for social bonding
.
Behavioral and Brain Sciences
,
44
,
e59
. ,
[PubMed]
Schachner
,
A.
, &
Garvin
,
L.
(
2010
).
Does synchrony really affect social variables? Effects on cooperation, conformity may not be robust
.
Poster presented at the International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition, Seattle, WA
.
Shen
,
Z.
,
Xu
,
Q.
, &
Jin
,
L.
(
2020
).
Structured procedures promote placebo effects
.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology
,
91
,
104029
.
Sherif
,
M.
(
1958
).
Superordinate goals in the reduction of intergroup conflict
.
American Journal of Sociology
,
63
(
4
),
349
356
.
Shtulman
,
A.
(
2017
).
Scienceblind: Why our intuitive theories about the world are so often wrong
.
Basic Books
.
Spelke
,
E. S.
, &
Kinzler
,
K. D.
(
2007
).
Core knowledge
.
Developmental Science
,
10
(
1
),
89
96
. ,
[PubMed]
Sterzer
,
P.
,
Frith
,
C.
, &
Petrovic
,
P.
(
2008
).
Believing is seeing: Expectations alter visual awareness
.
Current Biology
,
18
(
16
),
R697
R698
. ,
[PubMed]
Stewart-Williams
,
S.
, &
Podd
,
J.
(
2004
).
The placebo effect: Dissolving the expectancy versus conditioning debate
.
Psychological Bulletin
,
130
(
2
),
324
340
. ,
[PubMed]
Stupacher
,
J.
,
Maes
,
P.-J.
,
Witte
,
M.
, &
Wood
,
G.
(
2017
).
Music strengthens prosocial effects of interpersonal synchronization – If you move in time with the beat
.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology
,
72
,
39
44
.
Tamborini
,
R.
,
Novotny
,
E.
,
Prabhu
,
S.
,
Hofer
,
M.
,
Grall
,
C.
,
Klebig
,
B.
,
Hahn
,
L. S.
,
Slaker
,
J.
,
Ratan
,
R. A.
, &
Bente
,
G.
(
2018
).
The effect of behavioral synchrony with black or white virtual agents on outgroup trust
.
Computers in Human Behavior
,
83
,
176
183
.
Tarr
,
B.
,
Launay
,
J.
, &
Dunbar
,
R. I. M.
(
2014
).
Music and social bonding: “Self-other” merging and neurohormonal mechanisms
.
Frontiers in Psychology
,
5
,
1096
. ,
[PubMed]
Tarr
,
B.
,
Launay
,
J.
, &
Dunbar
,
R. I. M.
(
2016
).
Silent disco: Dancing in synchrony leads to elevated pain thresholds and social closeness
.
Evolution and Human Behavior
,
37
(
5
),
343
349
. ,
[PubMed]
Tarr
,
B.
,
Slater
,
M.
, &
Cohen
,
E.
(
2018
).
Synchrony and social connection in immersive Virtual Reality
.
Scientific Reports
,
8
(
1
),
3693
. ,
[PubMed]
Tunçgenç
,
B.
, &
Cohen
,
E.
(
2016
).
Movement synchrony forges social bonds across group divides
.
Frontiers in Psychology
,
7
,
782
. ,
[PubMed]
Turner
,
V.
(
1995
).
The ritual process
.
Aldine De Gruyter
.
Valdesolo
,
P.
, &
DeSteno
,
D.
(
2011
).
Synchrony and the social tuning of compassion
.
Emotion
,
11
(
2
),
262
266
. ,
[PubMed]
Valdesolo
,
P.
,
Ouyang
,
J.
, &
DeSteno
,
D.
(
2010
).
The rhythm of joint action: Synchrony promotes cooperative ability
.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology
,
46
(
4
),
693
695
.
Weinstein
,
D.
,
Launay
,
J.
,
Pearce
,
E.
,
Dunbar
,
R. I. M.
, &
Stewart
,
L.
(
2016
).
Singing and social bonding: Changes in connectivity and pain threshold as a function of group size
.
Evolution and Human Behavior
,
37
(
2
),
152
158
. ,
[PubMed]
Wiltermuth
,
S. S.
(
2012
).
Synchronous activity boosts compliance with requests to aggress
.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology
,
48
(
1
),
453
456
.
Wiltermuth
,
S. S.
, &
Heath
,
C.
(
2009
).
Synchrony and cooperation
.
Psychological Science
,
20
(
1
),
1
5
. ,
[PubMed]
Younger
,
J.
,
Gandhi
,
V.
,
Hubbard
,
E.
, &
Mackey
,
S.
(
2012
).
Development of the Stanford Expectations of Treatment Scale (SETS): A tool for measuring patient outcome expectancy in clinical trials
.
Clinical Trials
,
9
(
6
),
767
776
. ,
[PubMed]

Author notes

Competing Interests: The authors declare no conflict of interest.

These authors contributed equally.

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. For a full description of the license, please visit https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/legalcode.

Supplementary data