We use exogenously assigned general practitioners to study the effects of female role models on girls' educational outcomes. Girls who are exposed to female general practitioners are more likely to sort into male-dominated education programs in high school, most notably science, technology, engineering, mathematics, and medicine (STEMM). These effects persist as they enter college and select majors. The effects are larger for high-ability girls with low-educated mothers, suggesting that female role models improve intergenerational mobility and narrow the gifted gap. This demonstrates that role model effects in education need not involve individuals in the classroom but can arise due to everyday interactions with medical professionals.

WOMEN outperform men in educational attainment but remain underrepresented in fields with high financial returns, most notably STEMM (science, technology, engineering, mathematics, and medicine). This gender imbalance in education can explain a large part of the gender wage gap (Carrell, Page, & West, 2010; Lavy & Sand, 2018; Weinberger, 1999), and a better understanding of the mechanisms underlying this phenomenon is imperative. As the conventional explanations of discrimination and differences in aptitude have largely been ruled out (Card & Payne, 2017; Ceci et al., 2014; Hyde, 2005), increasing attention has been placed on alternative mechanisms, such as same-gender role models, influencers, and mentors.

Exposure to same-gender role models may affect educational decisions through several channels. Role models may fuel higher aspirations, reduce “stereotype threats,” and convey important information (Breda et al., 2018).1 A growing body of work within education economics supports the role model hypothesis, showing that females who are exposed to female role models in high school and college perform better in school and are more likely to select into male-dominated fields.2 However, several questions remain: Are these effects temporary or permanent? Do they extend to more general settings outside the classroom? Can exposure in childhood, before important education investments have been made, produce similar effects?

This paper aims to move beyond the existing role model literature by addressing these questions, exploiting exogenous variation in general practitioner (GP) assignment for Norwegian children. Norwegian GPs act as gatekeepers to the country's health care system and are responsible for diagnosing and treating patients and referring them to hospitals and specialists. When individuals are no longer able to consult with their existing GPs (e.g., due to GP retirement), the Norwegian Health Economics Administration (HELFO) randomly reassigns them to new GPs conditional on municipality and availability. We use GP reassignments due to GP retirement or other causes outside the patient's control as a source of exogenous variation to test if childhood exposure to female GPs, a group of successful female STEMM role models, has an effect on the educational choice and performance of girls.

Using doctor-patient interactions to test for same-gender role model effects has several benefits. First, the interactions take place in childhood before any educational decisions have been made. Second, by tracing the role model effects throughout a child's education career, from compulsory school to college, we can examine the persistence of the effect as children age. Third, the role model interactions take place outside the classroom. This allows us to better understand to what extent same-sex role model effects, as identified in the education literature, extend to more general settings. This is interesting as the majority of role model studies in the classroom have focused on teacher-student interactions. Such effects may be driven by gender differences in teaching practices, the student-teacher interaction may be different from other types of social interactions, and the purpose of a classroom (to create an environment conducive to academic development) may make children more receptive to role model influences. In addition, while disentangling same-gender role model effects from other potential mechanisms in the classroom is difficult (e.g., differences in teaching practices), our setting allows us to test for these confounders (gender differences in health practices) directly. Finally, while females remain underrepresented among GPs (37% of GPs were females during our analysis period), this is a less male-dominated field than, for example, math, physics, and computer science. Understanding if same-gender role models can have an impact on girls' educational performance and career choices even in relatively more balanced fields has important policy implications.

For our analysis, we leverage rich matched doctor-patient administrative data on all children in Norway who were subject to an exogenous GP reassignment between 2002 and 2011. We link these data to detailed information on educational performance and choices made throughout individuals' academic careers from tenth grade, the earliest age at which students have subject specialization options, into college. To account for the potential systematic correlation between previous GP characteristics (such as gender) and the gender of the new exogenously assigned GP, all analyses incorporate a full set of previous GP fixed effects.

Exposure to a female GP has a statistically significant and meaningful effect on both STEMM choice and educational performance among girls. Specifically, assignment to a female GP during childhood increases the probability of choosing a STEMM program in high school by 4 percentage points (20% relative to the mean) and increases high school STEMM GPA by 0.09 standard deviations. These effects persist as girls enter college: assignment to a female GP increases the probability of choosing a STEMM college major by more than 2 percentage points. This suggests that female role models can close the gender gap in college STEMM choice by almost 20%. The effects we identify are large, but they fall within the range of the effects identified from shorter information interventions in the classroom (Breda et al., 2018; Porter & Serra, 2019).3 Our paper shows that role model effects in education need not involve the classroom, but can arise due to everyday interactions with medical professionals.

We find significantly larger effects for girls with low-educated mothers, a group that may be less exposed to same-gender STEMM role models in general. This suggests that same-gender role models may facilitate intergenerational occupational mobility, contributing to a long-standing debate on the intergenerational transmission of human capital and how to facilitate socioeconomic mobility (Black et al., 2005). Quantile regressions suggest that high-ability children with low-educated mothers drive the results. This demonstrates that female role models may help narrow the gap between high-ability students from more and less advantaged socioeconomic backgrounds. This is consistent with evidence suggesting that investments in early childhood, in particular among children from disadvantaged backgrounds, can reduce inequality (Carneiro & Heckman, 2003; Elango et al., 2015). Consistent with papers on classroom role models (Carrell et al., 2010), we find no effect of GP gender match on male educational outcomes.

This paper provides novel insights into how same-gender role models in childhood outside educational settings may shape educational choices and career decisions. These results have important policy implications. Specifically, educational choices in high school and college likely have significant effects on future labor market opportunities and career decisions, and may help close the gender wage gap (Carrell et al. 2010; Lavy and Sand 2018). Further, female students with same-gender role models not only select into traditionally male-dominated education programs but also perform better in school, suggesting that same-gender role models improve education matches. Intentionally matching girls to female role models may be an effective tool for narrowing the gender gap in educational choice and labor market outcomes.

In addition to a direct role model effect, some of our results could operate through health (same-gender GPs may have an impact on interactions with the health system) and family (mothers' interactions with the GP might also expose them to a female role model, which could have an impact on the mothers' outcomes directly and their children's outcomes indirectly). While these alternative pathways do not threaten our identification strategy, they would affect the interpretation of our results because some of the effects could operate through a health-based non–role model channel or through an indirect role-model channel via the impact on the girl's mother.

We find no evidence of these alternative pathways: assignment to a same-gender GP has no impact on the number of diagnoses, the likelihood of a mental health diagnosis, the probability of visiting the GP for birth control reasons, or fertility. In addition, our findings do not indicate that the effects operate through the mother as measured by her education, labor market, and health outcomes. This suggests the effects we identify likely are driven by direct role model influences between GPs and children.

Our paper contributes to the existing literature in several ways. First, this is the first paper to study one-on-one role model interactions in childhood. This is an important contribution as these children have not made any educational decisions prior to exposure, such that the potential impact of role models is greater. Second, no other paper has examined effects of same-gender role models outside educational settings on educational choices. Our setting is important for understanding the extent to which the earlier research on teacher-student interactions generalizes to nonclassroom settings.4 Third, by examining the effect of GP gender match on health and parents, we can rule out alternative pathways and better isolate the role model effect. Finally, Norwegian registry data allow us to trace children throughout their educational careers and explore long-term effects.

A. Health Care System and GP Assignment in Norway

The Norwegian public health care system is based on universal access, and enrollment is automatic. As in much of Europe, the health system is a two-part system, with primary care provided by the local municipalities and specialist care provided by larger health regions.5

Access to specialist care and hospitals can normally be achieved only through referrals from GPs in the primary care sector (except in emergencies). The GP is therefore the first point of contact for nonemergency care and is responsible for diagnosis and treatment. When GPs deem it necessary, they refer patients to specialists. In other words, the Norwegian GPs act as gatekeepers to the country's health system. The average time GPs spend with a patient during an appointment is 20 minutes (Mjølstad & Stund, 2019), and most children therefore spend several hours interacting with their GP before deciding on education specialization at age 15.

Since 2001, the government has assigned every resident to a local GP.6 In most cases, patients interact with their assigned GP every time they use the health care system.7 Prior to this system, individuals were not tied to a specific GP and had to find a GP every time they needed care. The system was meant to improve doctor-patient relationships and ensure appropriate use of health care, and the initial assignment in 2001 was primarily based on patient preferences. As of 2015, there were 4,500 GPs, and each GP had an average of 1,200 patients. The average GP was 47 years old, and 60% were male.

When GPs retire, move, or for some other reason decide to terminate or reduce their patient list, patients on that list are reassigned to a new GP in the municipality.8 Within the system's legal framework, there are two important things to note (FOR, 2018). First, in the event of list reductions, GPs must randomly select which patients to remove from the list. Second, in the event of reassignment, patients should be randomly assigned to new GPs in the municipality conditional on availability.9 However, the regulatory framework does not specify a randomization device. In section V, we perform balance tests and falsification checks to show that the data are consistent with the new GPs being exogenously assigned to children.

We use GP reassignments due to GP retirement or other causes outside the patient's control as a source of exogenous variation to test whether exposure to female GPs during childhood affects the educational choice and performance of females. We do not use the initial assignment or any swaps initiated by patients due to endogeneity concerns. It is important to note that children often have the same GP as their mother. However, we find no evidence that our effects of same-gender GP on girls' education are operating through their mothers.

The majority of GPs are self-employed (less than 5% are salaried municipality employees), and municipalities contract with individual GPs to provide services to their residents by assigning them to a list of patients. GPs receive a combination of capitation from the municipalities (around 30% of their income), fee-for-service from the Health Economics Administration (almost 70% of their income), and out-of-pocket payments from patients. GP financing is determined nationally through collective bargaining.

With respect to the gender balance of physicians in Norway, the number of female physicians has steadily increased over the past several decades, from 10% in 1930 to 40% in 2010. This number will increase over time as the current share of female medical students exceeds 50%. Consistent with other OECD countries, female share across different specializations varies widely, ranging from 6% in thoracic surgery to 100% in medical genetics (Legeforeningen, 2019a). In general, specializations that guarantee a fixed monthly pay (non-self-employed) and do not require on-call duty tend to have greater female representation. Because the majority of GPs are self-employed and all GPs are required by law to be on call at the emergency room a certain number of weeks per year, women remain underrepresented as GPs.10 In 2010, 37% of GPs were women (Legeforeningen, 2019b).

B. The Norwegian Education System

The Norwegian education system consists of ten years of tuition-free compulsory education starting at age 6, with the curriculum set by the central government. During the first seven years, children are taught a wide range of subjects but receive no official grades. In the last three years, students study a smaller set of subjects and receive grades. Following successful completion of compulsory education, each child has a right to three years of tuition-free high school.

High school in Norway is very different from that in the United States. It offers thirteen distinct education programs: five academic and eight vocational. Academic programs consist of three years of classroom education, while the vocational programs consist of two years of classroom education followed by one to two years of practical training in the field. In the first year of high school, students enroll in one of the thirteen programs. Then they choose a specialization within their broad education program. Around 60% of students pursue an academic track.

High school education provides students with university admission certification, vocational competence, or basic (craft) competence. Those in academic programs receive a university admission certification and can apply to college, while most students in vocational programs do not.11 Table A1 provides an overview of the education programs and tracks available at the high school level. To obtain a high school STEMM credential, a student has to select the Specialization in General Studies education program in the first year of high school and then specialize in natural science and mathematics in the second and third years.

Students apply to high school through a centralized system based on grades from their final year of compulsory education. The application consists of ranking three programs in the county of residence. If the number of applications exceeds program capacity, students are assigned based on compulsory school grades. However, although admission to specific programs are based on these grades, national law ensures that all students gain admission to one of the three programs on their list.

A range of universities and colleges offer higher education in Norway, and most are tuition-free public institutions. The Norwegian Universities and Colleges Admission Service coordinates the admissions process. Students apply to specific programs at the different universities, and if the number of applications exceeds the number of seats, students are assigned based on high school grades. Admission is conditional on having graduated from high school with a university admission certification. Additionally, some programs, most notably STEMM, impose specific high school course prerequisites. This makes it difficult for students with non-STEMM high school diplomas to enroll in STEMM programs.

A. Data

Our data come from rich administrative records on the universe of Norwegian residents. We restrict our sample to individuals who graduated from high school between 2006 and 2014 (born between 1988 and 1996). We start with 2006, because the current high school structure was introduced in that year. We stop with 2014, because that is the final year for which we have education data. The GP system was introduced in 2001, so we have information on all exogenous swaps starting in 2002, when the youngest individuals in our sample (born in 1996) were 6 years old. Because high school applications are submitted when individuals are 15 years old, swaps after age 15 should not affect the outcomes we examine. As such, our main analysis exploits variation in GP-patient gender match among individuals born between 1988 and 1996 who experienced an exogenous GP swap in 2002 or later and are between ages 6 and 15. Panel A of table 1 shows demographic information for these individuals. Only those who attended high school are included in the data. Since the probability of attending high school differs between men and women (24,500 males completed high school in 2016 compared to 31,600 females), our female sample is larger than our male sample.12

Table 1.

Descriptive Statistics of Individuals in Sample

GirlsBoys
MeanSDMeanSD
A: Family Composition  
Birth order 1.864 0.983 1.850 0.985 
Siblings 1.697 1.064 1.707 1.052 
Born in Norway 0.861 0.346 0.851 0.356 
Mother age 28.987 5.008 29.414 4.822 
Mother marital status 0.567 0.496 0.595 0.491 
Mother years of education 14.146 2.396 14.445 2.356 
Mother log earnings 12.740 0.560 12.782 0.587 
Mother not in labor force 0.067 0.250 0.070 0.255 
B: GP Visiting Behavior  
GP visits age 15 1.165 1.630 0.898 1.525 
GP visits age 15 conditional on visiting 2.130 1.680 1.915 1.730 
Still with ex. GP at age 15 0.613 0.487 0.616 0.486 
GP gender match 0.405 0.491 0.627 0.483 
C: Educational Performance and Choice  
Compulsory school GPA 4.591 0.573 4.430 0.595 
Compulsory school STEMM GPA 4.303 0.889 4.351 0.914 
High school GPA 4.314 0.654 4.185 0.681 
High school STEMM GPA 3.993 0.890 3.940 0.905 
Academic track year 1 0.734 0.440 0.807 0.395 
High school STEMM credential 0.195 0.396 0.303 0.460 
Ever College 0.751 0.433 0.678 0.467 
Ever College STEMM 0.077 0.266 0.185 0.388 
GirlsBoys
MeanSDMeanSD
A: Family Composition  
Birth order 1.864 0.983 1.850 0.985 
Siblings 1.697 1.064 1.707 1.052 
Born in Norway 0.861 0.346 0.851 0.356 
Mother age 28.987 5.008 29.414 4.822 
Mother marital status 0.567 0.496 0.595 0.491 
Mother years of education 14.146 2.396 14.445 2.356 
Mother log earnings 12.740 0.560 12.782 0.587 
Mother not in labor force 0.067 0.250 0.070 0.255 
B: GP Visiting Behavior  
GP visits age 15 1.165 1.630 0.898 1.525 
GP visits age 15 conditional on visiting 2.130 1.680 1.915 1.730 
Still with ex. GP at age 15 0.613 0.487 0.616 0.486 
GP gender match 0.405 0.491 0.627 0.483 
C: Educational Performance and Choice  
Compulsory school GPA 4.591 0.573 4.430 0.595 
Compulsory school STEMM GPA 4.303 0.889 4.351 0.914 
High school GPA 4.314 0.654 4.185 0.681 
High school STEMM GPA 3.993 0.890 3.940 0.905 
Academic track year 1 0.734 0.440 0.807 0.395 
High school STEMM credential 0.195 0.396 0.303 0.460 
Ever College 0.751 0.433 0.678 0.467 
Ever College STEMM 0.077 0.266 0.185 0.388 

Sample includes all boys and girls born between 1988 and 1996 who were subject to at least one exogenous GP swap prior to age 15. Number of observations is approximately 8,500 girls and 5,500 boys. Mother's age and marital status are calculated at the year of birth. Mother's earnings, education, and employment are measured when the child is 15 years old (when the children in our sample make their high school choices; our data do not extend far back enough for us to get this information at the year of birth).

The main strength of our data is that we can link individuals across different longitudinal data registries through unique identifiers. This allows us to combine the demographic information in panel A of table 1 with detailed information on GP interactions (through GP and health care registries), educational choice and performance (through education registries), and family characteristics (through intergenerational identifiers and the population registry).13

The GP registry provides information on the GP of every individual in our sample, for each year since the introduction of the GP list system. We use unique GP identifiers to combine this registry with health care registry information, which includes the number of times an individual visited her GP for each year since 2006. The GP registry also contains information on whether an individual changed GP during the year and the reason for that change. For our study, we are interested in GP swaps that are outside the patient's control, which generates plausibly exogenous variation in the gender of the patient's new GPs. To this end, we focus on GP swaps that the doctor decided to terminate, or reduce, the patient list. In section V, we show that these swaps are unlikely to be correlated with the gender of the assigned GP.

With respect to the availability of male and female GPs within each municipality (the level of assignment), the average is nine GPs, around two-thirds of them male, per municipality. Approximately 90% of all girls live in municipalities that had at least one GP of each gender. These girls are different from the small subset of girls who live in municipalities without both a male and a female GP.14 Although these differences are small, we need to be careful to extrapolate our results to the municipalities in which there were not both a male and a female GP available. However, seeing as this represents less than 10% of the girls in the country, we do not believe that this constitutes a significant limitation of our paper.

Panel B of table 1 provides summary statistics for GP visits of the individuals in our sample, as well as information on GP swaps. Approximately 40% of girls and 60% of boys are matched to a same-gender GP after an exogenous swap. This indicates that there are more male than female GPs in our sample. The individuals in our sample met with their GP an average of two times per year, which amounted to thirty times before they decided on a high school education program. Given the average appointment time of 20 minutes, this means that each child in our sample spends approximately 10 hours with their GP before deciding which high school education program to pursue. This is an upper bound on exposure to GPs as role models, since the average number of years that the individuals in our sample remain with their exogenously assigned GP is three (indicating 120 minutes of mean exposure). The GP intervention we examine is less intense than the classroom experiments where students interact with teachers on a daily basis for a relatively long time (Carrell et al., 2010; Lim & Meer, 2017), but more intense than the information interventions in which females come to classrooms to talk (Breda et al., 2018; Porter & Serra, 2019). Importantly, these are one-on-one interactions, which contrasts with much of the previous literature.

Panel B of table 1 also shows that girls are more likely to visit their GPs than boys are. While a nonnegligible fraction of our sample experienced a second swap during the study period, very few individuals swapped GPs more than twice (table A3). The average number of years that individuals remained with their exogenously assigned GP is three, and approximately 60% of children who experienced an exogenous swap prior to age 15 still have the same GP at age 15. This is important for the interpretation of our results as we are identifying intent-to-treat effects based on the initial exogenous swap.15

Our education data include detailed information on educational choice and academic performance. In terms of educational choice, we begin by using high school registry data to examine if GP gender match has an effect on the probability of choosing one of the five academic tracks discussed in section II. Next, we focus on the primary research question of this paper: Does a same-gender GP role model encourage girls to enroll in STEMM? We examine this question by estimating the effect of GP gender match on the probability of graduating with a high school STEMM credential. As discussed in section II, this choice is important for STEMM eligibility at the university level (the correlation between high school STEMM and college STEMM is 0.35), and in supplemental analyses we use information from the university registry to explore if any potential educational choice effects in high school persist in college.

After exploring the impact of GP gender match on educational choice, we examine if it had an impact on educational performance. The outcomes we examine include compulsory and high school GPA. Since we are interested in the impact of same-gender role models on the STEMM gender gap, we focus on STEMM GPA. In auxiliary analyses, we extend the outcome set and examine if potential STEMM GPA effects extend to non-STEMM GPA.

Compulsory school STEMM GPA is measured in grade 10 and is used to apply to high school. High school STEMM GPA is measured in grade 13 and is used to apply to college. Conditional on finding effects on educational choice, examining the effect of same-gender GP on academic performance in high school is particularly interesting as selection into STEMM could result in either lower academic performance (because STEMM is a more academically challenging program) or higher academic performance (through improved program match and motivation). Panel C of table 1 shows descriptive statistics of the educational performance and choice variables. On average, girls and boys do equally well in school as measured by GPA. However, girls are more likely to attend university but much less likely to sort into STEMM.

B. Method

We estimate the following model separately for males and females:
(1)
where Yi is one of the educational outcomes listed above for individual i. The variable GP_Matchi is a dichotomous variable equal to 1 if the gender of the exogenously assigned GP matches that of the individual and zero otherwise. The coefficient β1 measures the effect of being exogenously assigned to a same-gender GP in childhood compared to being exogenously assigned to an opposite-gender GP.16 As the decision to swap GPs or comply with the assigned GP is endogenous, we focus on the first exogenous swap of the individuals in our sample.17 This protects us against potential endogeneity concerns after the initial swap but attenuates our estimates. The results should therefore be interpreted as intent-to-treat effects.

Equation (1) includes birth cohort (θc), previous doctor (ρd), municipality (πm), and year of swap (τt) fixed effects. Municipality fixed effects account for systematic differences across municipalities correlated both with being assigned to a same-gender GP and the outcomes. The average size of a municipality (12,000 individuals) is small.18

The previous doctor fixed effects absorb systematic differences between the GPs that individuals were assigned to prior to a swap, such that our identifying variation is driven by girls who had the same previous GP but were exogenously moved to differently gendered new GPs (one girl assigned to a male GP and one to a female GP). This eliminates the risk of our results being confounded by characteristics of the girls' previous GPs and increases the similarity of the girls we compare. The cohort fixed effects control for any time-invariant differences between cohorts that may be correlated with both GP-gender match and outcomes. Finally, the year of swap fixed effects accounts for systematic differences across years.

A. Baseline Results

Panel A of table 2 presents the effect of same-gender GP on female educational choice and performance using the four outcomes defined above: academic high school track, STEMM high school credential, compulsory school STEMM GPA, and high school STEMM GPA.19 Column 1 shows that girls who are exposed to a female GP during childhood are 5 percentage points more likely to choose an academic program in high school (7% relative to the mean). This choice ensures access to higher education and greatly improves an individual's chance of obtaining a university degree.20 Column 2 explores if GP gender affects the probability of graduating from high school with a STEMM credential (recall that STEMM specialization can be chosen first in the second year of high school). The point estimate closely mirrors that in column 1 and shows that females who are assigned to female GPs in childhood are 4 percentage points more likely to obtain a STEMM credential in high school (20% relative to the mean). Columns 1 and 2 show that female GPs lead girls to sort into, and graduate from, educational programs that traditionally have been underrepresented by women and are associated with larger financial returns.

Table 2.

Effect of Same-Gender GP on Educational Choice and Performance

High school academic trackHigh school STEMM credentialCompulsory school STEMM GPAHigh school STEMM GPA
A: Girls 
Same-gender GP 0.052*** 0.039** 0.084** 0.109*** 
 (0.017) (0.018) (0.039) (0.039) 
Mean 0.736 0.194 4.297 3.993 
Observations 8,679 8,424 8,617 8,258 
B: Boys 
Same-gender GP 0.011 0.002 −0.038 0.010 
 (0.020) (0.025) (0.047) (0.049) 
Mean 0.807 0.600 4.350 3.940 
Observations 5,514 5,338 5,475 5,253 
High school academic trackHigh school STEMM credentialCompulsory school STEMM GPAHigh school STEMM GPA
A: Girls 
Same-gender GP 0.052*** 0.039** 0.084** 0.109*** 
 (0.017) (0.018) (0.039) (0.039) 
Mean 0.736 0.194 4.297 3.993 
Observations 8,679 8,424 8,617 8,258 
B: Boys 
Same-gender GP 0.011 0.002 −0.038 0.010 
 (0.020) (0.025) (0.047) (0.049) 
Mean 0.807 0.600 4.350 3.940 
Observations 5,514 5,338 5,475 5,253 

The table shows the β1 coefficients obtained through estimation of equation (1) as described in the text and reproduced here for clarity: yi=α+β1GP_Matchi+τt+πm+θc+ρd+εi. yi is a general term denoting the outcome listed at the top of each column, and each estimation includes municipality (πm), year of swap (τt), birth year (θc), and previous GP (ρd) fixed effects. The point estimates depicted in the table should be interpreted as the effect of random assignment to same-gender GP in childhood on the outcome listed at the top of the column. Standard errors are clustered at the level of the exogenously assigned GP. Sample includes all girls (boys) born between 1988 and 1996 who were subject to at least one exogenous GP swap prior to age 15. Significant at *10%, **5%, ***1%.

The last two columns of panel A in table 2 examine if female GPs affect the educational performance of girls. Column 3 presents results for STEMM GPA at the compulsory level and provides clear evidence of a positive effect of GP match on performance: the point estimate indicates that a girl who is exposed to a female GP experiences a 0.084 unit increase in STEMM GPA. This effect is 0.09 standard deviations (table 1). As discussed in section II, compulsory school GPA is imperative for admission to selective high school programs, and this result is consistent with role model exposure motivating individuals to work harder in compulsory school to get accepted by more selective high school programs.

We also study if the performance effects identified in compulsory school persist as girls enter high school. Examining academic performance in high school is interesting as selection into STEMM could result in either lower academic performance (because STEMM is a more challenging program) or higher academic performance (through improved program match and motivation). Column 4 of table 2 provides clear evidence of a positive effect of GP gender match on performance in high school. The point estimate is similar to the compulsory school STEMM GPA effect. This suggests that the effect of same-gender GP on educational choice does not induce worse academic performance.21 Rather, it leads to improved educational achievement, which could be due to improved education matching and greater motivation.

The interpretation of our estimates is complicated by the fact that they are reduced form and may be attenuated by the fact that nonmatched individuals may also be exposed to female GPs at some point during their childhood. We have also estimated a modified version of equation (1) in which we instrument whether the child had a female GP at any point between ages 6 and 15 using the gender of the exogenously assigned GP as the instrumental variable. The results from this exercise return first-stage F statistics in the 800 range and second-stage results that are approximately 50% larger than our baseline results (table A8). However, we encourage caution when interpreting these results, because the first stage of this analysis is exclusively driven by girls whose only female GP is the exogenously assigned GP (compliers). Girls who would have a female GP regardless of the exogenously assigned female GP (always-takers) do not contribute to identification of the first stage. Yet exogenous assignment to a female GP could have an impact on education outcomes even for girls who have other female GPs, and the always-takers might thus contribute to the reduce form effect.

To examine if the same-gender GP effects for girls identified in panel A of table 2 extend to boys, panel B provides results from estimating equation (1) on our boys. The point estimates are small not statistically significant, suggesting that boys are not affected by same-gender GPs. The results are consistent with previous literature on this topic in a classroom setting (Carrell et al. 2010). While the lack of significant effects among boys could be due to gender differences in receptiveness to role models, it could also be because the channels through which roles models operate (eradication of stereotype threats and provision of information) are less important for boys. Specifically, the percentage of boys in STEMM is substantial, and there is an abundance of public figure male STEMM role models.

Panel B of table 2 also suggests that there is no across-the-board effect of female GPs on children: if female GPs had a positive influence on male children, we would find a negative effect of same-gender GP on boys. This is an interesting result, especially in light of a few cross-sectional studies suggesting that female GPs are associated with slightly better outcomes for patients than male GPs (Tsugawa et al., 2017). However, it is also worth noting that the individuals in our sample are very young and on average have very few health problems.

Taken together, the results in table 2 indicate that same-gender GPs have a significant effect on females', but not males', educational performance and choice.22 The education effects we identify are large, but are considerably smaller than existing differences in the outcome variables by socioeconomic factors such as mother's income, education, and marital status (table A10), and fall within the range of the effects identified from shorter information interventions in the classroom (Breda et al., 2018; Porter & Serra, 2019).23 Due to the lack of statistically significant results among boys, we focus on same-gender GP matches on the educational choice and performance of girls in the remainder of the paper.

B. Heterogeneity Analysis

Mother education level.

The effect of same-gender role models likely differs across girls depending on the availability of same-gender role models in their families and surroundings. For example, a girl with a highly educated mother may benefit less from a female GP than a girl with a less educated mother, as many of the channels through which role models operate (providing higher aspirations, reducing stereotype threats, and conveying information) are filled by the mother.

To examine this possibility, we estimate equation (1) for our core outcomes stratified by whether the female's mother has a college degree. These results, in table 3, show that the role model effects identified in section IVA are driven by daughters of less educated mothers. The results highlight that same-gender role models are important not only for closing the gender STEMM gap but also for closing the within-gender socioeconomic STEMM gap. This suggests same-gender role models may be an important tool for improving intergenerational occupational mobility.24 To ensure that this heterogeneity is not driven by female GPs being better at communicating with low-educated families than male GPs, we also estimate equation (1) for boys of low-educated mothers. The results are provided in table A11. None of the coefficients are statistically significant, consistent with our main findings.

Table 3.

Effect of Same-Gender GP on Females, by Mother's Education

High school academic trackHigh school STEMM credentialCompulsory school STEMM GPAHigh school STEMM GPA
A: Mother college or more 
Same-gender GP 0.029 0.035 0.021 −0.014 
 (0.033) (0.050) (0.091) (0.098) 
Mean 0.851 0.291 4.632 4.243 
Observations 2,341 2,341 2,339 2,337 
B: Mother less than college 
Same-gender GP 0.070*** 0.093*** 0.101* 0.137** 
 (0.026) (0.024) (0.054) (0.053) 
Mean 0.675 0.152 4.212 3.874 
Observations 4,654 4,643 4,637 4,652 
High school academic trackHigh school STEMM credentialCompulsory school STEMM GPAHigh school STEMM GPA
A: Mother college or more 
Same-gender GP 0.029 0.035 0.021 −0.014 
 (0.033) (0.050) (0.091) (0.098) 
Mean 0.851 0.291 4.632 4.243 
Observations 2,341 2,341 2,339 2,337 
B: Mother less than college 
Same-gender GP 0.070*** 0.093*** 0.101* 0.137** 
 (0.026) (0.024) (0.054) (0.053) 
Mean 0.675 0.152 4.212 3.874 
Observations 4,654 4,643 4,637 4,652 

The table shows the β1 coefficients obtained through estimation of equation (1) as described in the text and reproduced here for clarity: yi=α+β1GP_Matchi+τt+πm+θc+ρd+εi. yi is a general term denoting the outcome listed at the top of each column, and each estimation includes municipality (πm), year of swap (τt), birth year (θc), and previous GP (ρd) fixed effects. The point estimates depicted in the table should be interpreted as the effect of random assignment to same-gender GP in childhood on the outcome listed at the top of the column. Standard errors are clustered at the level of the exogenously assigned GP. Panel A includes all girls who were subject to at least one exogenous GP swap prior to age 15 and have a mother with at least a college education. Panel B includes includes all girls who were subject to at least one exogenous GP swap before age 15 and have a mother with less than a college education. Significant at *10%, **5%, ***1%.

Distributional effects.

Equation (1) estimates the average treatment effect. However, looking only at the mean effect likely misses important heterogeneity in effect size across the ability distribution. Specifically, exposure to same-gender role models is likely to incentivize students at the right tail of the ability distribution who satisfy, or are close to satisfying, the requirements for choosing STEMM programs, while it may not be sufficient for incentivizing students at the left tail. To explore this, table 4 show results from estimating unconditional quantile regressions, using the methodology of Firpo, Fortin, and Lemieux (2009). With respect to compulsory school STEMM GPA, the results in panel A of table 4 suggest that the effect of same-gender GP match on educational performance loads on individuals in the right tail of the ability distribution. While the results for high school STEMM GPA are slightly noisier with respect to quantiles 7 and 8 (panel B of table 4), the general pattern of results is similar to that for compulsory school STEMM GPA.

Table 4.

Effect of Same-Gender GP on STEMM GPA: Quantile Effects

QuantileQuantileQuantileQuantileQuantileQuantileQuantileQuantileQuantile
123456789
A: Compulsory school STEMM GPA 
Same-gender GP 0.006 −0.000 0.011 0.011 0.082* 0.082* 0.067* 0.067* 0.149* 
 (0.030) (0.044) (0.032) (0.032) (0.043) (0.043) (0.035) (0.035) (0.070) 
B: High school STEMM GPA 
Same-gender GP −0.027 0.060 0.076 0.145** 0.135** 0.156** 0.096 0.045 0.140** 
 (0.084) (0.066) (0.056) (0.062) (0.052) (0.063) (0.058) (0.067) (0.068) 
QuantileQuantileQuantileQuantileQuantileQuantileQuantileQuantileQuantile
123456789
A: Compulsory school STEMM GPA 
Same-gender GP 0.006 −0.000 0.011 0.011 0.082* 0.082* 0.067* 0.067* 0.149* 
 (0.030) (0.044) (0.032) (0.032) (0.043) (0.043) (0.035) (0.035) (0.070) 
B: High school STEMM GPA 
Same-gender GP −0.027 0.060 0.076 0.145** 0.135** 0.156** 0.096 0.045 0.140** 
 (0.084) (0.066) (0.056) (0.062) (0.052) (0.063) (0.058) (0.067) (0.068) 

Authors' estimation of equation (1) as described in the text using the unconditional quantile regression method discussed in Firpo, Fortin, and Lemieux (2009). Regressions include municipality, year of swap, birth year, and previous GP fixed effects. Standard errors are clustered at the level of the exogenously assigned GP. Sample includes all girls born between 1988 and 1996 who were subject to at least one exogenous GP swap prior to age 15. Significant at *10%, **5%, ***1%.

Age at swap.

Average treatment effects from equation (1) may also miss treatment heterogeneity across age at swap. Specifically, it is likely that the effect of being assigned to a same-gender GP at age 6 is different from being assigned to a same-gender GP at age 15: young children may be differentially affected since the GP-patient interaction likely is different, and children who experience a swap at an earlier age may be exposed to the new GP for a longer period. However, this does not mean that we expect the effect to be zero among girls exposed at a later age. First, the average person spends around three years with the assigned GP, and the average exposure time for young girls is not much different from that of older girls. Second, individuals exposed at the end of compulsory school are preparing their high school applications, and it is possible that exposure during this critical time period is important.

To examine age heterogeneity, we estimate models of the following form,
(2)
where ExogSwapia takes the value 1 if individual i experienced an exogenous swap at age a and 0 otherwise, grouping individuals have three age bins: 6 to 9, 10 to 12, and 13 to 15. These age groups represent distinct stages of the children's educational careers: lower primary education, upper primary education, and lower secondary education. The term (ExogSwapia×GP_Matchi) is the interaction of age at swap (a) and gender match. This interaction term takes the value 1 if the person experienced an exogenous swap to a same-gender GP and the swap happens at that age and 0 if the person swapped at a different age or did not experience a GP gender match. These results are shown in figure 1 for our four core outcomes.
Figure 1.
Same-Gender Role Model Effects by Age at Swap, Females

Authors' estimation of a modified version of equation (1) as described in the text and reproduced here for clarity: yi=α+β1GP_Matchi+τt+πm+θc+ρd+εi. yi is a general term denoting the outcome listed at the legend, and each estimation includes municipality (πm), year of swap (τt), birth year (θc), and previous GP (ρd) fixed effects. The point estimates depicted in the table should be interpreted as the effect of random assignment to same-gender GP in the age range denoted on the x-axis. As equation (2) contains both year-of-swap and birth cohort fixed effects, the equation does indirectly control for the main effects for age at assignment as well (as this is a linear combination of year of swap and birth cohort). Heterogeneity in effect size across age ranges is driven both by differences in the length of exposure and differences in how susceptible individuals are to role model influences in the different age ranges. Standard errors are clustered at level of the exogenously assigned GP. Sample includes all girls born between 1988 and 1996 who were subject to at least one exogenous GP swap prior to age 15.

Figure 1.
Same-Gender Role Model Effects by Age at Swap, Females

Authors' estimation of a modified version of equation (1) as described in the text and reproduced here for clarity: yi=α+β1GP_Matchi+τt+πm+θc+ρd+εi. yi is a general term denoting the outcome listed at the legend, and each estimation includes municipality (πm), year of swap (τt), birth year (θc), and previous GP (ρd) fixed effects. The point estimates depicted in the table should be interpreted as the effect of random assignment to same-gender GP in the age range denoted on the x-axis. As equation (2) contains both year-of-swap and birth cohort fixed effects, the equation does indirectly control for the main effects for age at assignment as well (as this is a linear combination of year of swap and birth cohort). Heterogeneity in effect size across age ranges is driven both by differences in the length of exposure and differences in how susceptible individuals are to role model influences in the different age ranges. Standard errors are clustered at level of the exogenously assigned GP. Sample includes all girls born between 1988 and 1996 who were subject to at least one exogenous GP swap prior to age 15.

Close modal

Figure 1 implies that girls who are exogenously assigned a female GP at an earlier age experience larger effects than girls exposed at a later age. This suggestive heterogeneity is largest when examining STEMM GPA in compulsory school but is also noticeable with STEMM GPA in high school and STEMM high school credential. With respect to academic high school track, the effect appears constant across the different age groups. However, as shown in table A13, which provides the full set of point estimates and standard errors of the results in figure 1, the effects are noisy and not statistically significantly different from each other. Thus, this is only suggestive evidence regarding heterogeneity by age of swap.

It is also interesting that exposure at ages 13 through 15, when children are preparing their high school applications, is associated with statistically significant and economically meaningful effects. This is consistent with the literature on role models in the classroom (Breda et al., 2018; Porter & Serra, 2019) that finds that information interventions have an immediate impact on a girl's educational choice. Having said that, the point estimate is monotonically declining with age for all but the academic high school track outcome.

C. Additional Outcomes

Non-STEMM GPA.

Table 2 shows that female GPs improve the STEMM GPA of female students. It is not clear that these effects lead to an improvement in overall GPA, because the improved STEMM GPA could be due to students' spending less time on other subjects. To examine this, column 1 of table 5 shows the result from estimating equation (1) using non-STEMM GPA as the dependent variable. This result shows that the STEMM GPA effect in table 2 is not restricted to STEMM subjects: girls exposed to same-gender GPs perform better in non-STEMM subjects as well, though the effects are smaller. It is important to note that admission to high school and university depends not only on subject-specific GPA but also on overall GPA. Thus, a high STEMM GPA is necessary, but not sufficient, for admission to STEMM programs.25 The effect on non-STEMM GPA is consistent with the idea that STEMM role models motivate individuals to work harder to get accepted into more selective programs.

Table 5.

Effect of Same-Gender GP: Additional Outcomes

Compulsory school non-STEMM GPAHigh school non-STEMM GPACollege enrollmentCollege STEMM enrollmentCollege Medicine enrollmentCollege STEM enrollment
Same-gender GP 0.040* 0.067** 0.005 0.022* 0.011 0.009 
 (0.023) (0.028) (0.017) (0.013) (0.008) (0.011) 
Mean 4.297 4.384 0.754 0.077 0.019 0.059 
Observations 8,617 8,678 8,680 8,680 8,680 8,680 
Compulsory school non-STEMM GPAHigh school non-STEMM GPACollege enrollmentCollege STEMM enrollmentCollege Medicine enrollmentCollege STEM enrollment
Same-gender GP 0.040* 0.067** 0.005 0.022* 0.011 0.009 
 (0.023) (0.028) (0.017) (0.013) (0.008) (0.011) 
Mean 4.297 4.384 0.754 0.077 0.019 0.059 
Observations 8,617 8,678 8,680 8,680 8,680 8,680 

The table shows the β1 coefficients obtained through estimation of equation (1) as described in the text and reproduced here for clarity: yi=α+β1GP_Matchi+τt+πm+θc+ρd+εi. yi is a general term denoting the outcome listed at the top of each column, and each estimation includes municipality (πm), year of swap (τt), birth year (θc), and previous GP (ρd) fixed effects. The point estimates depicted in the table should be interpreted as the effect of random assignment to same-gender GP in childhood on the outcome listed at the top of the column. Standard errors are clustered at the level of the exogenously assigned GP. Sample includes all girls born between 1988 and 1996 who were subject to at least one exogenous GP swap prior to age 15. Significant at *10%, **5%, ***1%.

College enrollment and college major choice.

In table 5, we examine if the effects on educational choice in compulsory school and high school persist as individuals enter college. Looking at these outcomes is important for understanding the persistence of the role model effects. While Gershenson et al. (2018) have documented effect persistence with respect to same-race teachers, we are aware of no studies that have examined this with respect to same-sex role models. We focus on two outcomes: the probability of attending college and the probability of choosing a STEMM major.

Column 3 of table 5 shows that there is no effect on enrolling in college, suggesting that same-gender role models do not have an impact on the extensive margin of females' decision to pursue higher education. This result is consistent with the quantile regression results discussed above, which suggest that same-gender role models may be effective in motivating students on the margin but not those at the bottom of the ability distribution.

Column 4 of table 5 shows that female GPs increase girls' likelihood of choosing a STEMM college major by 2 percentage points. This suggests that female role models can close the gender gap in college STEMM choice by almost 20%. The effect is economically large but falls within the range of the effects that have been identified from shorter information interventions in the classroom. For example, Breda et al. (2018) find that a one-hour classroom intervention by a female role model raises the probability that girls enroll in selective STEM tracks in higher education by 30%, and Porter and Serra (2019) find that a 15-minute classroom intervention by a female economics alumna increases the likelihood that female students major in economics by 8 percentage points (a 90% increase).

It is possible that the college STEMM major effect is driven exclusively by treated individuals being more inclined to pursue medicine. To examine this, we estimate equation (1) using the probability of pursuing medicine and the probability of pursuing STEM as outcomes. The results are shown in the last two columns of table 5. While we lack statistical power to identify precise effects at this disaggregated level, the standard errors are smaller than the point estimates, and the results show that about half of the effect comes from enrolling in medicine. However, as the share of girls pursuing medicine is considerably smaller than the share of females pursuing STEM, the effect of enrolling in medicine is much larger as a percentage of the mean. This provides evidence on the channel through which the STEMM effect operates.

A. Balance Test

The key assumption underlying our estimation strategy is that the gender of the exogenously assigned GP is orthogonal to other characteristics that influence the educational outcomes we study. To examine this assumption, we conduct a balance test in which we regress a set of predetermined characteristics on the gender match of the assigned GP using equation (1). If the GP gender is orthogonal to individual characteristics that may influence future educational decisions, this exercise should produce small and not statistically significant point estimates.

The results from this exercise, presented in table 6, support our identifying assumption, showing small and not statistically significant point estimates for each characteristic. A joint test of significance for the covariates in table 6 further supports the identifying assumption, failing to reject the null hypothesis that the covariates are jointly unable to predict the gender of the exogenously assigned GP (p-value of 0.557).26

Table 6.

Balance Test

Birth orderSiblingsBorn in NorwayMother years of educationMother ageMother marriedMother log incomeMother not in labor force
Same-gender GP 0.044 0.049 −0.025 0.004 0.086 0.009 −0.004 0.009 
 (0.042) (0.055) (0.017) (0.109) (0.195) (0.020) (0.021) (0.012) 
Mean 1.865 1.698 0.861 14.146 28.974 0.567 12.740 0.066 
Observations 8,424 8,424 8,424 8,034 8,423 8,034 8,357 8,424 
Birth orderSiblingsBorn in NorwayMother years of educationMother ageMother marriedMother log incomeMother not in labor force
Same-gender GP 0.044 0.049 −0.025 0.004 0.086 0.009 −0.004 0.009 
 (0.042) (0.055) (0.017) (0.109) (0.195) (0.020) (0.021) (0.012) 
Mean 1.865 1.698 0.861 14.146 28.974 0.567 12.740 0.066 
Observations 8,424 8,424 8,424 8,034 8,423 8,034 8,357 8,424 

The table shows the β1 coefficients obtained through estimation of equation (1) as described in the text and reproduced here for clarity: yi=α+β1GP_Matchi+τt+πm+θc+ρd+εi. yi is a general term denoting the outcome listed at the top of each column, and each estimation includes municipality (πm), year of swap (τt), birth year (θc), and previous GP (ρd) fixed effects. The point estimates depicted in the table should be interpreted as the effect of random assignment to same-gender GP in childhood on the outcome listed at the top of the column. Standard errors are clustered at the level of the exogenously assigned GP. Sample includes all girls born between 1988 and 1996 who were subject to at least one exogenous GP swap prior to age 15. Mother's age and marital status are calculated at the year of birth. Mother's earnings, education, and employment are measured when the child is 15 years old (when the children in our sample make their high school choices; our data do not extend far back enough for us to get this information at the year of birth). Significant at *10%, **5%, ***1%.

It is worth noting that the sample used for the balance test is slightly smaller than that used for the main analysis due to missing information on parental characteristics for a small number of girls in our sample. To ensure that this is not biasing our results, we have also restricted the sample to girls for whom we have all balance variables, and then we reestimated our main results. Our results are robust to this sample restriction (table A17).

B. GP Characteristics

If GPs are as good as randomly assigned to patients, then GP characteristics should be orthogonal to patient characteristics. The balancing test in section VA does not reject this assumption. However, a remaining issue is that GP gender may correlate with other GP characteristics (e.g., age), confounding our estimates. To examine if nongender GP characteristics are orthogonal to GP gender, we have examined the distribution of GP characteristics among newly assigned GPs by estimating the effect of being randomly assigned to a same-gender GP on nongender GP characteristics. We have also estimated our main equation, including controls for the nongender characteristics of the newly assigned GP.

The nongender GP characteristics we look at are age and nationality. Although these are the only nongender GP demographic characteristics available in our data, we believe that they represent two of the most important nongender characteristics, since characteristics such as education, certification, and income are similar across GPs: they have the same level of base training, the quality of medical education is constant across the four public medical schools in the country, and they are paid based on national pay scales. The results from the first exercise are in table A18, and the results from the second exercise are in table A19. These results support the claim that GP gender is orthogonal to other GP characteristics such as age.

C. Falsification Test

In addition to balance tests, we perform placebo tests in which we examine the effect of exogenous GP matches at ages 20 to 25 on educational outcomes at the compulsory and high school levels and the effect of GP matches at ages 17 to 20 on educational outcomes at the compulsory school level. The vast majority of people aged 20 to 25 have completed high school, and the vast majority of those aged 17 to 20 have completed compulsory school. Thus, GP matches after age 19 should not affect compulsory and high school outcomes, and GP matches after age 16 should not affect compulsory school outcomes. Looking at the results in table 7, we see that all estimates are much smaller or in the wrong direction and none are statistically significant. These results are inconsistent with the presence of biases due to nonrandom sorting of children to GPs of a specific gender, and support a causal interpretation of our results.27

Table 7.

Placebo Tests; Post-High School GP Swaps

High school academic trackHigh school STEMM credentialCompulsory school STEMM GPAHigh school STEMM GPA
A: All girls who were subject to their first exogenous GP swap between ages 20 and 25 
Same-sex GP 0.008 −0.005 −0.030 −0.022 
 (0.017) (0.018) (0.039) (0.036) 
Mean 0.759 0.199 4.120 3.986 
Observations 6,730 6,730 6,730 6,730 
B: All girls who were subject to their first exogenous GP swap between ages 17 and 20 
Same-sex GP −0.007  −0.028  
 (0.010)  (0.023)  
Mean 0.741  4.199  
Observations 16,868  14,862  
High school academic trackHigh school STEMM credentialCompulsory school STEMM GPAHigh school STEMM GPA
A: All girls who were subject to their first exogenous GP swap between ages 20 and 25 
Same-sex GP 0.008 −0.005 −0.030 −0.022 
 (0.017) (0.018) (0.039) (0.036) 
Mean 0.759 0.199 4.120 3.986 
Observations 6,730 6,730 6,730 6,730 
B: All girls who were subject to their first exogenous GP swap between ages 17 and 20 
Same-sex GP −0.007  −0.028  
 (0.010)  (0.023)  
Mean 0.741  4.199  
Observations 16,868  14,862  

The table shows the β1 coefficients obtained through estimation of equation (1) as described in the text and reproduced here for clarity: yi=α+β1GP_Matchi+τt+πm+θc+ρd+εi. yi is a general term denoting the outcome listed at the top of each column, and each estimation includes municipality (πm), year of swap (τt), birth year (θc), and previous GP (ρd) fixed effects. The point estimates depicted in the table should be interpreted as the effect of random assignment to same-gender GP in childhood on the outcome listed at the top of the column. Standard errors are clustered at the level of the exogenously assigned GP. The sample in panel A includes all girls who were subject to their first exogenous GP swap between ages 20 and 25. The sample in panel B includes all girls who were subject to their first exogenous GP swap between ages 17 and 20. Significant at *10%, **5%, ***1%.

D. Permutation Tests

Another concern is that we are simply picking up random noise and that our results are independent of treatment assignment. To investigate this issue, we perform a series of permutation tests in which we randomly reassign treatment to GPs and reestimate equation (1) using this re-randomization. We perform the permutations 300 times for our four core outcomes and examine where the effects identified in table 2 fall relative to these 300 placebo estimates. If the results in table 2 represent true effects of same-gender GP assignment, the estimates in that table should be larger than the vast majority of these simulations.

Table 8 displays our baseline estimates and the p-values obtained from the permutation exercises. The asterisks accompanying the estimates correspond to the level of statistical significance of our baseline estimates and are included to facilitate the interpretation of the results. The p-values produced by the permutation exercises greatly resemble those obtained in the baseline estimation. We can therefore reject the null hypothesis that any combination of treatment would generate the same magnitude of treatment effects as that displayed in table 2.

Table 8.

P-Values of Permutation Test

High school academic trackHigh school STEMM credentialCompulsory school STEMM GPAHigh school STEMM GPA
Baseline estimate 0.052*** 0.039** 0.084** 0.109*** 
% less than baseline 0.010 0.053 0.053 0.020 
Number of replications 300 300 300 300 
High school academic trackHigh school STEMM credentialCompulsory school STEMM GPAHigh school STEMM GPA
Baseline estimate 0.052*** 0.039** 0.084** 0.109*** 
% less than baseline 0.010 0.053 0.053 0.020 
Number of replications 300 300 300 300 

Authors' estimation of equation (1) as described in the text and reproduced here for clarity: yi=α+β1GP_Matchi+τt+πm+θc+ρd+εi. yi is a general term denoting the outcome listed at the top of each column, and each estimation includes municipality (πm), year of swap (τt), birth year (θc), and previous GP (ρd) fixed effects. The table shows the proportion of times the estimates from the permutation tests are smaller than the baseline estimate. We run 300 simulations in which we randomly assign GPs to children. Standard errors are clustered at level of the exogenously assigned GP. Sample includes all girls born between 1988 and 1996 who were subject to at least one exogenous GP swap prior to age 15. The asterisks accompanying the estimates correspond to the level of statistical significance of our baseline estimates, and are included to facilitate the interpretation of the results. Significant at *10%, **5%, ***1%.

E. Ruling Out Alternative Pathways

In addition to a direct role model mechanism between girls and GPs, some of the identified effects may operate through children's health and parents. First, GPs may affect how girls interact with the health system. For example, girls may be more comfortable with female GPs and be more likely to disclose health issues and receive treatment. It is also possible GPs are better able to relate to same-gender patients. While it is unlikely that this would affect education choices, improved health could affect educational performance and may provide an additional pathway through which our effects operate. Second, it is also possible that some of the effects we identify operate through mothers' exposure to same-gender GPs. Specifically, mothers' interactions with the GP might expose them to female STEMM role models, which could have an impact on their outcomes directly and children's outcomes indirectly (through improved resources at home or through access to better information via the mother).28

To examine the existence of a potential health-based, nonrole model channel, we estimate equation (1) using a battery of health-related outcomes measured at age 15: GP visits, number of diagnoses, mental health diagnoses, birth control visits, fertility (admission for delivery), and probability of remaining with the assigned GP. To examine if any of our effects operate through indirect role model influences via the mother's interaction with the GP, we estimate equation (1) using a set of education, labor, and health outcomes of the mother when the child is 15.

Panel A (health-based channel) and panel B (indirect effect through the mother) of table 9 show these results. The results in panel A show that GP gender match does not have an impact on the health-related outcomes of the child. The one exception is fertility, which displays a statistically significant effect. However, the point estimate is not economically meaningful. The results in panel B show that assignment to a same-gender GP does not affect the mother's education, labor market, and health outcomes. Taken together, the results in table 9 suggest that the effects we identify in section IV are unlikely to operate through a health-based, nonrole model channel or an indirect role model channel via the mother's interaction with the GP.
Table 9.

Potential Pathways and Mechanisms

A: Potential indirect effects through health
Number of GP visitsStill with assigned GPNumber of diagnosesMental health diagnosisBirth control visitsFertility
Same-Gender GP −0.009 −0.014 −0.039 −0.000 0.002 −0.000**  
 (0.070) (0.019) (0.028) (0.000) (0.005) (0.000)  
Mean 1.166 0.613 2.241 0.078 0.069 0.000  
B: Potential indirect effects through mother 
 Years of Total Not in Number of Mental health Birth control  
 education income labor force diagnoses diagnosis visits Fertility 
Same-Gender GP 0.004 −0.004 0.009 0.176 −0.003 0.001 −0.001 
 (0.109) (0.021) (0.012) (0.141) (0.018) (0.012) (0.001) 
Mean 14.146 12.740 0.066 3.614 0.186 0.068 0.003 
A: Potential indirect effects through health
Number of GP visitsStill with assigned GPNumber of diagnosesMental health diagnosisBirth control visitsFertility
Same-Gender GP −0.009 −0.014 −0.039 −0.000 0.002 −0.000**  
 (0.070) (0.019) (0.028) (0.000) (0.005) (0.000)  
Mean 1.166 0.613 2.241 0.078 0.069 0.000  
B: Potential indirect effects through mother 
 Years of Total Not in Number of Mental health Birth control  
 education income labor force diagnoses diagnosis visits Fertility 
Same-Gender GP 0.004 −0.004 0.009 0.176 −0.003 0.001 −0.001 
 (0.109) (0.021) (0.012) (0.141) (0.018) (0.012) (0.001) 
Mean 14.146 12.740 0.066 3.614 0.186 0.068 0.003 

The table shows the β1 coefficients obtained through estimation of equation (1) as described in the text: yi=α+β1GP_Matchi+τt+πm+θc+ρd+εi. yi denotes the outcome, and each estimation includes municipality (πm), year of swap (τt), birth year (θc), and previous GP (ρd) fixed effects. Standard errors are clustered at the GP. The sample in panel A includes all girls born between 1988 and 1996 who were subject to an exogenous GP swap prior to age 15. The sample in panel B includes all mothers to girls who were born between 1988 and 1996 and exposed to an exogenous GP swap prior to age 15. Outcomes are measured when the girls are 15 years old. Income is reported in log form. Significant at *10%, **5%, ***1%.

Figure 2.
Estimated Effects with Varying Fixed Effects

Authors' estimation of a modified version of equation (1) as described in the text and reproduced here for clarity: yi=α+β1GP_Matchi+τt+πm+θc+ρd+εi. yi is a general term denoting the outcome listed on the horizontal axis, and each estimation includes municipality (πm), year of swap (τt), birth year (θc), and previous GP (ρd) fixed effects. Standard errors are clustered at the level of the exogenously assigned GP. The sample includes all girls born between 1988 and 1996 who were subject to at least one exogenous GP swap prior to age 15. The specification that replaces the previous GP fixed effects with previous GP characteristics includes GP age, gender, and Norwegian-born status. For each of the four outcomes, the following specification legend applies: Specification 1: no fixed effects; specification 2: cohort-by-municipality fixed effects; specification 3: cohort fixed effects and municipality-specific trends; specification 4: cohort, municipality, and year fixed effects; specification 5: cohort, municipality, and year fixed effects, and controls for previous GP characteristics; specification 6: cohort and previous GP fixed effects; specification 7: cohort, municipality, and previous GP fixed effects; specification 8: cohort, year, and previous GP fixed effects; specification 9: cohort, municipality, year, and previous GP fixed effects.

Figure 2.
Estimated Effects with Varying Fixed Effects

Authors' estimation of a modified version of equation (1) as described in the text and reproduced here for clarity: yi=α+β1GP_Matchi+τt+πm+θc+ρd+εi. yi is a general term denoting the outcome listed on the horizontal axis, and each estimation includes municipality (πm), year of swap (τt), birth year (θc), and previous GP (ρd) fixed effects. Standard errors are clustered at the level of the exogenously assigned GP. The sample includes all girls born between 1988 and 1996 who were subject to at least one exogenous GP swap prior to age 15. The specification that replaces the previous GP fixed effects with previous GP characteristics includes GP age, gender, and Norwegian-born status. For each of the four outcomes, the following specification legend applies: Specification 1: no fixed effects; specification 2: cohort-by-municipality fixed effects; specification 3: cohort fixed effects and municipality-specific trends; specification 4: cohort, municipality, and year fixed effects; specification 5: cohort, municipality, and year fixed effects, and controls for previous GP characteristics; specification 6: cohort and previous GP fixed effects; specification 7: cohort, municipality, and previous GP fixed effects; specification 8: cohort, year, and previous GP fixed effects; specification 9: cohort, municipality, year, and previous GP fixed effects.

Close modal

The lack of a health-based, nonrole model channel is an important finding, as one of the main concerns with existing research on same-gender role models in the classroom has been that it may represent differences in teaching practices rather than true role model effects. While we acknowledge that there are additional unobserved health outcomes that we cannot examine, it is unlikely that they are driving our findings as they would have to be uncorrelated with all the outcomes in table 9, not subsumed by the fixed effects in equation (1) but correlated with the probability of being assigned a same-gender GP and the outcomes of interest.

We use exogenous assignment of children to GPs to examine if female role models can reduce the gender gap in educational choice. This is the first paper to study the effects of female role models in childhood on the long-run educational outcomes of girls. It is also the first paper to explore the effects of same-gender role model interactions outside the classroom.

We find that exposure to female GPs has a statistically significant and economically meaningful positive effect on the probability that girls pursue academic high school programs, graduate with STEMM credentials from high school, and choose STEMM majors at university. A back-of-the-envelope calculation suggests that female role models can close the gender gap in college STEMM choice by 20%. This effect is large but within the range of effects identified from shorter information interventions in the classroom. The persistence of role model effects is interesting given the “leaking pipeline” phenomenon discussed in the literature.

In terms of mechanisms, our results show that high-ability girls with limited access to female role models at home (as measured by mother's education) drive the results. This suggests that a main channel through which the effects operate is information and the elimination of stereotype threats, enabling individuals with high underlying ability to realize their full potential. That more than two-thirds of the girls who switch into STEMM come from other relatively demanding non-STEMM academic tracks reinforces this argument. We observe no differential effect by the presence of high-educated fathers, which suggests that the effects are due not to a general information flow but to information provision unique to the mother or other same-gender role models. By showing that the effects are confined to those who remain with their GPs over time, we also provide suggestive evidence that repeated interactions, rather than just general awareness, matter for the realization of general role model effects.

While previous literature in economics of education has documented the value of same-gender role models, we are the first to show that role model effects in education need not involve classrooms but can arise due to everyday interactions with other potential nonteacher role models. A particularly novel feature of our study is ruling out the possibility that the effects operate through gender differences in health practices. This is interesting as the majority of role model studies in the classroom have focused on teacher-student interactions. A concern with these studies has been that the effects may simply be driven by gender differences in teaching practices, something that appears unlikely in light of the results presented here.

Another novel aspect of our study is the ability to examine the effects of role models before any educational decisions have been made and how these effects differ by age at swap. In general, the results provide suggestive evidence that earlier exposure generates larger effects (consistent with research on early childhood investments). However, the results show that exposure during the final phase of compulsory school, when students prepare their high school applications and decide which type of track to choose, also matters.

Finally, by following individuals over time and examining the persistence of the effects as individuals enter college and choose majors, we demonstrate that role model effects are long-lasting and perhaps deserving of even more policy attention. Specifically, the findings imply that intentionally matching girls to female role models (doctors, professors, supervisors, mentors) and scaling up existing same-gender mentorship programs may be effective policy tools for narrowing the gender gap in educational choice and labor market outcomes.

In contextualizing our results, it is important to note that Norway ranks as one of the most gender-equal countries in the world (WEF, 2018). However, it is difficult to speculate whether we would expect larger or smaller effects in countries with greater gender inequality. On the one hand, female role model effects may be larger in countries where female role models are rare. On the other hand, same-gender role models may interact positively with a country that is more progressive on gender, such that the effects are smaller in countries where female role models are rare.29 Nevertheless, we believe that the general pattern of our results is relevant to a large number of countries and settings. Norway is still far from reaching perfect gender equality, and similar to other OECD countries, the gender-divided labor market represents the largest challenge to reaching this goal. For example, females are more likely to work part-time (36.8% compared to 12.5%), more likely to work in the public sector (70.1% of public employees are women), less likely to hold leadership positions (35.3% of individuals in leadership positions are females), and the annual income is only 70% of the income of males (SSB 2018). In terms of education, women dominate nursing, welfare, and teaching degrees at college and are less likely to major in natural science, leading to gender-segregated occupational patterns (SSB, 2018). Thus, we cannot speak to how the effects would differ across countries, but we believe that our results generalize to the majority of OECD countries that are actively working toward gender parity and face similar labor market barriers.

1

We define role models as “a person whose behavior in a particular role is imitated by others” (Merriam-Webster, 2020). However, we acknowledge that several definitions and types of role models exist. For a concise discussion on this topic, see Chung (2000).

2

Bettinger and Long (2005); Breda et al. (2018); Carrell et al. (2010); Dee (2004, 2005); Eble and Hu (2017); Griffith (2014); Hoffman and Oreopoulos (2009); Kofoed and McGovney (2017); Lim and Meer (2017); Mansour et al. (2018); Gershenson et al. (2018); Porter & Serra (2019); Canaan and Mougaine (2019).

3

We focus on STEMM (STEM plus medicine) rather than STEM since the role models in our setting belong to the former rather than the latter group.

4

Beaman et al. (2012) show that female representation among politicians can affect the gender gap in aspirations and education among adolescents. Although that study does not examine the implications of one-on-one interactions between individuals and potential role models, it suggests that role model effects may exist in nonclassroom settings.

5

There are currently 422 municipalities and 4 health regions in Norway.

6

Specifically, the Norwegian Health Economics Administration (part of the Norwegian Directorate of Health) assigns individuals to local GPs on behalf of the government.

7

There are a few exceptions to this, for example, if the patient is brought into the ER.

8

Twice per year, individuals can independently change the GP they have been assigned. Using information on the exact cause of the GP swap, we ignore such endogenous swaps.

9

In certain cases, entering GPs can take over the entire list from a retiring GP. However, our identification strategy consists of comparing individuals who had the same GP who then were allocated to different GPs, such that these do not contribute to the identifying variation.

10

There are three exceptions to the on-call requirement of GPs: individuals over the age of 60, women in the last three months of pregnancy, and women with children under the age of 1.

11

Individuals in vocational programs can take supplemental courses to attain this qualification.

12

See “Upper Secondary Education Advanced Course II/Certificate” at https://ssb.no/341521/completed-educational-programmes-in-upper-secondary-education-by-gender-and-results.

13

Table A2 reproduces panel A of table 1 for individuals not subject to an exogenous swap before age 15. The table shows that the individuals included in our analysis are very similar to those excluded from the analysis on all observable dimensions.

14

Girls who lived in municipalities that had at least one male and one female GP were slightly less likely to have siblings (0.180) and be Norwegian born (0.078) and were more likely to have mothers who were more educated (0.17 years of education), slightly older (0.508), more likely to be married (0.028), and had higher earnings (0.070 log points).

15

Table A4 compares the characteristics of girls who remain with their exogenously assigned GPs until age 15 with the characteristics of girls who do not remain with their exogenously assigned GP. We fail to reject the null hypothesis that the characteristics of the girls who remain with their exogenously assigned GP are the same as the characteristics of the girls who do not remain with their exogenously assigned GP. While this speaks in favor of the generalizability of our results to the broader population, we acknowledge that these groups may differ on other dimensions that we cannot identify.

16

There are two types of papers in the literature: papers comparing exposure to female role models to exposure to nothing (Porter & Serra, 2019; Breda et al., 2018) and those comparing exposure to female role models to exposure to male role models (Carrell et al., 2010; Canaan & Mouganie, 2019). Our paper is part of the second group, and a limitation with this approach is that it is not possible to know if female GPs positively affect girls' likelihood to study STEMM or if male GPs negatively affect it. However, all individuals require a GP and will be exposed to either a male or a female GP, and this paper provides evidence of the implications associated with assigning girls to female GPs.

17

To understand what the results are when we ignore the endogeneity of treatment (recognizing these are only correlational), we also estimate a version of equation (1) in which the match variable is based on the gender of the first GP the girl was observed with. These results are consistent with a story of positive selection of girls to male GPs; failing to account for the endogeneity in GP selection would bias us toward finding no effects (table A5). Such bias may occur if parents believe male GPs are more competent than female GPs.

18

While the legal framework contains no mention of geographic matching within municipalities, there could potentially be systematic differences across areas within municipalities that are not absorbed by the municipality fixed effects. However, this concern is alleviated by the previous GP fixed effects; our results are identified from girls who went to the same GP in the same location but then were moved to differently gendered new GPs.

19

Missing data for a small number of individuals for some of the outcomes means that there are minor differences in sample sizes between the different columns.

20

Table A6 shows results from equation (1) using non-STEMM academic track and nonacademic track as outcomes. The results show that girls are pulled into the STEMM track from both of these groups, though more than two-thirds are coming from academic non-STEMM programs.

21

As discussed in section II, the exogenous GP swaps are driven by GPs reducing, or terminating, their patient list. List terminations are driven primarily by GP retirement and may be perceived as a cleaner source of exogenous variation than list reductions. We therefore perform a robustness check in which we estimate equation (1) using only GP swaps that were caused by list terminations. Results from this exercise are shown in table A7. This adjustment leads to larger point estimates and smaller standard errors.

22

An interesting and policy-relevant question with respect to our results is whether the effects are driven by increased awareness of females within the STEMM fields or due to repeated interactions with a female in STEMM. To provide suggestive evidence, we estimate equation (1) separately for those who remain with their GPs until age 15 and those who do not remain with them. These results are speculative as the decision to remain with an exogenously assigned GP is likely endogenous. The results, set out in table A9, suggest that the effects are driven by girls who remain with their GPs. Conditional on the endogeneity issue, this suggests that repeated interactions, rather than simply increased awareness, drive the effects.

23

The male sample is smaller than the female sample, such that our power to detect effects among boys is smaller. However, the differences in estimates are substantial, while the standard errors are similar, making it unlikely that the lack of effects among boys is due to sample size.

24

The fact that we observe no statistically significantly different effect by the presence of high-educated fathers suggests that the effects are due not to a general information flow but to information provision unique to the mother or other same-gender role models (table A12).

25

This is a different setting from Carrell et al. (2010), for example, where performance in non-STEM courses does not affect students' ability to major in STEM (conditional on passing).

26

The balance test for males is in table A14, and that for females without previous GP fixed effects is in table A15. An alternative balance test is to use the out-of-sample data (girls with no GP swaps) to predict the four main outcomes with the independent covariates. We can then compare the predicted outcomes for girls with a female GP and a male GP (after a swap). These results, provided in table A16, show that there are no statistically significant differences between the predicted outcomes for girls with a female GP and a male GP.

27

Another placebo test is to examine the effect of GP gender match on girls who never visited their GP. The idea behind this test is that these girls did not interact with the GP and would therefore not be affected by her. None of the estimates obtained from equation (1) using this sample are statistically significant, and with the exception of the coefficient on academic high school track, the magnitude of these effects is very close to 0. However, it is difficult to interpret these results due to endogenous selection into the zero-visit sample and due to the fact that assignment to same-sex GP may affect the number of times a girl visits the GP.

28

Not only because mothers are likely to accompany their children to the GP but also because children are generally assigned to the same GP as their mother.

29

This ambiguity is further exemplified by the gender-equality paradox, in which countries with greater equality have been observed to have a more unequal gender distribution in STEM fields. For example, the share of female tertiary graduates in science in Norway is lower than that in countries that are commonly considered less gender equal; see UNESCO (2015).

Beaman
,
Lori
,
Esther
Duflo
,
Rohini
Pande
, and
Petia
Topalova
, “
Female Leadership Raises Aspirations and Educational Attainment for Girls: A Policy Experiment in India
,”
Science
335
:
6068
(
2012
),
582
586
.
[PubMed]
Bettinger
,
Eric
, and
Bridget
Long
, “
Do Faculty Serve as Role Models? The Impact of Instructor Gender on Female Students
,”
American Economic Review
95
:
2
(
2005
),
152
157
.
Black
,
Sandra
,
Paul
Devereux
, and
Kjell
Salvanes
, “
Why the Apple Doesn't Fall Far: Understanding Intergenerational Transmission of Human Capital
,”
American Economic Review
95
:
1
(
2005
),
437
449
.
Breda
,
Thomas
,
Julien
Grenet
,
Marion
Monnet
, and
Clémentine
Van Effenterre
, “
Can Female Role Models Reduce the Gender Gap in Science? Evidence from Classroom Interventions in French High Schools
,”
PSE working paper
2018-06
(
2018
).
Canaan
,
Serena
, and
Pierre
Mougaine
, “
Female Science Advisors and the STEM Gender Gap
,”
IZA working paper
12415
(
2019
).
Card
,
David
, and
Abigail
Payne
, “
High School Choices and the Gender Gap in STEM
,”
NBER working paper
23769
(
2017
).
Carneiro
,
Pedro
, and
James
Heckman
, “
Human Capital Policy
,” in
J.
Heckman
and
A.
Krueger
, eds.,
Inequality in America: What Role for Human Capital Policy?
(
Cambridge, MA
:
MIT Press
,
2003
).
Carrell
,
Scott
,
Marianne
Page
, and
James
West
, “
Sex and Science: How Professor Gender Perpetuates the Gender Gap
,”
Quarterly Journal of Economics
125
:
3
(
2010
),
1101
1144
.
Ceci
,
Stephen
,
Donna
Ginther
,
Shulamit
Kahn
, and
Wendy
Williams
, “
Women in Academic Science: A Changing Landscape
,”
Psychological Science in the Public Interest
15
:
3
(
2014
),
75
141
.
Chung
,
Kim-Sau
, “
Role Models and Arguments for Affirmative Action
,”
American Economic Review
90
:
33
(
2000
),
640
648
.
Dee
,
Thomas
, “
Teachers, Race, and Student Achievement in a Randomized Experiment
,” this review
86
:
1
(
2004
),
195
210
.
Dee
,
Thomas
, “
A Teacher Like Me: Does Race, Ethnicity, or Gender Matter?
American Economic Review
95
:
2
(
2005
),
158
165
.
Eble
,
Alex
, and
Feng
Hu
, “
Stereotypes, Role Models, and the Formation of Beliefs
,” mimeograph (
2017
).
Elango
,
Sneha
,
Jorge
Garcia
,
James
Heckman
, and
Andrés
Hojman
, “
Early Childhood Education
,” in
R.
Mofitt
, ed.,
Economics of Means-Tested Transfer Program in the United States
(
Chicago
:
University of Chicago Press
,
2015
).
Firpo
,
Sergio
,
Nicole
Fortin
, and
Thomas
Lemieux
, “
Unconditional Quantile Regressions
,”
Econometrica
77
:
3
(
2009
),
953
973
.
FOR
,
Forskrift om fastlegeordning I kommunene
(Helse-og omsorgsdepartementet, Norge,
2018
), https://lovdata.no/dokument/SF/forskrift/2012-08-29-842.
Gershenson
,
Seth
,
Cassandra
Hart
,
Joshua
Hyman
,
Constance
Lindsay
, and
Nicholas
Papageorge
, “
The Long-Run Impacts of Same-Race Teachers
,” NBER working paper 25254 (
2020
).
Griffith
,
Amanda
, “
Faculty Gender in the College Classroom: Does It Matter for Achievement and Major Choice?
,”
Southern Economic Journal
49
:
3
(
2014
),
730
749
.
Hoffmann
,
Florian
, and
Philip
Oreopoulos
, “
A Professor Like Me: The Influence of Instructor Gender on College Achievement
,”
Journal of Human Resources
44
:
2
(
2009
),
479
494
.
Hyde
,
Janet
, “
The Gender Similarities Hypothesis,
American Psychologist
60
(
2005
),
581
592
.
Kofoed
,
Michael
, and
Elizabeth
McGovney
, “
The Effect of Same-Gender and Same-Race Role Models on Occupational Choice: Evidence from Randomly Assigned Mentors at West Point,
Journal of Human Resources
54
(
2017
),
430
467
.
Lavy
,
Victor
, and
Edith
Sand
, “
On the Origins of Gender Gaps in Human Capital: Short and Long Term Consequences of Teachers' Biases,
Journal of Public Economics
167
(
2018
),
263
279
.
Legeforeningen
, “
Active Specialists under 70 Years Old in Norway, By Gender
” (
2019a
), https://www.legeforeningen.no/contentassets/0bdafe0d9fe0431bb968fa006ffef34d/2019-januar-kjonnsfordeling-etter-spesialitet.pdf.
Legeforeningen
, “
GPs and Specialization in General Medicine 2010–2019
” (
2019b
), https://www.legeforeningen.no/om-oss/legestatistikk/spesialister/.
Lim
,
Jaegeum
, and
Jonathan
Meer
, “
The Impact of Teacher-Student Gender Matches: Random Assignment Evidence from South Korea
,”
NBER working paper
21407
(
2017
).
Mansour
,
Hari
,
Daniel
Rees
,
Bryson
Rintala
, and
Nathan
Wozny
, “
The Effects of Professor Gender on the Post-Graduation Outcome of Female Students
,”
IZA DP
11820
(
2018
).
Merriam-Webster
, “
Role Model
,” accessed
June
2,
2020
, at https://www.merriam-webster.com/dictionary/role%20model.
Mjølstad
,
Bente
, and
Mali
Sund
, “
Er 20 minutter hos fastlegen nok?
Dagens Medisin
(
2019
), https://www.dagensmedisin.no/artikler/2019/09/30/er-20-minutter-hos-fastlegen-nok/.
Porter
,
Catherine
, and
Danila
Serra
, “
Gender Differences in the Choice of Major: The Importance of Female Role Models,
American Economic Journal: Applied
12
(
2020
),
226
254
.
Tsugawa
,
Yusuke
,
Anupam
Jena
,
Jose
Figueroa
,
John
Orav
,
Daniel
Blumenthal
, and
Ashishi
Jha
, “
Comparison of Hospital Mortality and Readmission Rates for Medicare Patients Treated by Male vs Female Physicians
,”
JAMA Intern. Med.
,
177
(
2020
),
206
213
.
UNESCO
, “
Is the Gender Gap Narrowing in Science and Engineering?
” (
2015
), https://zh.unesco.org/sites/default/files/usr15_is_the_gender_gap_narrowing_in_science_and_engineering.pdf.
WEF
,
The Global Gender Gap Report 2018
(
2018
), https://www.weforum.org/reports/the-global-gender-gap-report-2018.
Weinberger
,
Catherine
, “
Mathematical College Majors and the Gender Gap in Wages
,”
Industrial Relations
38
:
3
(
1999
),
407
413
.

Author notes

A.W. gratefully acknowledges financial support from the Research Council of Norway through its Centers of Excellence Scheme, FAIR project 262675. We thank Benjamin Castleman, Chloe East, Ariel Kalil, Katrine Løken, Marianne Page, Bertil Tunggodden, Lise Vesterlund, Aparna Soni, and Analisa Packham, as well as several other colleagues and seminar participants, for valuable comments and suggestions.

A supplemental appendix is available online at https://doi.org/10.1162/rest_a_00975.

Supplementary data